Embracing evolutionary theories of autism: Implications for psychiatry

IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 PSYCHIATRY Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Pub Date : 2024-01-15 DOI:10.1111/acps.13653
Tanya L. Procyshyn, Alex Tsompanidis, Simon Baron-Cohen
{"title":"Embracing evolutionary theories of autism: Implications for psychiatry","authors":"Tanya L. Procyshyn,&nbsp;Alex Tsompanidis,&nbsp;Simon Baron-Cohen","doi":"10.1111/acps.13653","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Autism is one of most common disabilities, with recent estimates that 1 in 36 children<span><sup>1</sup></span> have an autism diagnosis and late diagnoses in adults also increasing. The DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism now include sensory sensitivities alongside the long-established criteria of social and communication challenges and repetitive and restrictive behaviours and interests. Most clinicians no longer view autism as something to be cured or treated, instead providing support for the challenges that autistic people experience and treatments or interventions for any co-occurring conditions, while respecting their unique differences and strengths. As the prevalence of autism, who is being diagnosed with autism, and clinical understanding of autism are changing, so are perspectives on what it means to be autistic.</p><p>One field aiming to shift scientific perspectives on how we think about autism is evolutionary psychiatry,<span><sup>2</sup></span> a relatively new discipline that now has its own special interest group within the Royal College of Psychiatrists in the UK as well as dedicated textbooks. Like the related field of evolutionary medicine, evolutionary psychiatry sets its focus beyond biomedical causes (e.g., genetic and environmental factors), instead asking the “ultimate” question of <i>why</i> neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions have persisted in the human population over time. At first glance, autism presents an evolutionary paradox<span><sup>3</sup></span>: it is a highly heritable condition that affects “reproductive success” yet is relatively prevalent. Evolutionary psychiatry proposes possibilities that move us away from viewing autism as a disorder or disease to one that considers the role of crucial adaptations that have shaped the evolution of the human brain and our complex cognitive abilities. In this article, we consider several evolutionary explanations of autism, the supporting evidence, and how embracing an evolutionary perspective could influence clinical practice in psychiatry.</p><p>Most evolutionary theories conceptualise autism as a specialised neurocognitive profile characterised by a trade-off between costs (social cognition) and benefits (non-social cognition). The empathising-systemizing theory<span><sup>4</sup></span> was the first to suggest this, framing autism as enhanced motivation to systemize (the ability to analyse the variables in a system and derive the underlying rules that govern it) paired with challenges in empathising (the ability to imagine another person's thoughts and feelings and respond with an appropriate emotion). The link between autism and occupations that require a high level of systemizing ability<span><sup>5</sup></span>—such as science, technology, engineering, and mathematics—provides further support for the theory that autism reflects a brain specialised in rule-based thinking. Similarly, the “diametric model” of autism<span><sup>6</sup></span> highlights enhancement in diverse cognitive domains including visuo-spatial skills, reading ability, and local processing, with co-occurring reductions in imagination, shared attention, and personal agency (with, interestingly, opposite patterns observed for psychotic spectrum conditions). Broadly, these two theories view autism as reflecting enhanced development of the analytical brain, with the trade-off of reduced efficiency for processing social information. An updated theory from one author of the diametric model views autism as reflecting “enhanced, but imbalanced, components of intelligence,”<span><sup>7</sup></span> supported by recent findings of overlap between the genetics of autism and aspects of cognition.</p><p>Autism, like many conditions of interest to evolutionary psychiatry, is highly heritable. It is thought that thousands of genetic variants contribute to the likelihood of autism; notably, most of these genetic variants individually have very small effects and are common in the general population.<span><sup>8</sup></span> As a result, studies often add together autism-associated genes to create an index referred to as a polygenic score. While polygenic scores for autism are, by definition, associated with autism likelihood, they are also associated with various aspects of cognitive ability in the general population<span><sup>9</sup></span> and brain development.<span><sup>10</sup></span> Furthermore, there is evidence for positive selection of autism-related genes, implying that possessing autism-related genes (although not necessarily being autistic) was adaptive during human evolution.<span><sup>11</sup></span> The possibility that the same genes that underlie autism are those that played a crucial role in the development of the human brain and our unique cognitive abilities can help resolve the above-mentioned paradox of why heritable conditions perceived as disabilities could be maintained at high prevalence rates.</p><p>Given the link between autism and analytical cognition, as reflected by enhanced systemizing, positive selection for autism-related genes could have been the foundation of the leap in cause-effect thinking that led to humankind's unique ability to innovate.<span><sup>12</sup></span> In the words of autistic inventor and advocate Temple Grandin: “What would happen if the autism gene was eliminated from the gene pool? You would have a bunch of people standing around in a cave, chatting and socializing and not getting anything done.”<span><sup>13</sup></span></p><p>Evolutionary theory may also be useful for understanding the sex difference in the prevalence of autism. While it is now recognised that autism often presents differently in girls and women, that screening protocols may be biased toward recognising autism in males, and that girls and women are more likely to “camouflage” autism, males are still more likely to receive an autism diagnosis.<span><sup>14</sup></span></p><p>The prenatal sex steroid theory of autism proposes that elevated levels of sex steroid hormones interact with other factors (e.g., genetic variants) to disproportionately increase autism likelihood among males.<span><sup>15</sup></span> Gonadal sex differentiation during development, which is determined by the sex chromosomes, results in higher average levels of prenatal androgens in males compared to females. High sex steroid hormone levels during pregnancy may be a unique adaptation that has arisen during human evolution, as peak levels exceed than those in non-human primates despite differences in body weight.<span><sup>16</sup></span> These hormones are believed to play crucial roles during brain development, particularly in terms of cortical expansion and synaptogenesis. Recent experiments involving human stem cell-derived brain organoids show that androgens enhance neuronal development during formation of the cortex,<span><sup>17</sup></span> a brain region associated with many higher cognitive functions. Together, these lines of evidence suggest that human-specific evolutionary adaptations in the steroid hormone system have resulted in high levels of both androgens and oestrogens which, in turn, are associated with increased autism likelihood in males.<span><sup>18</sup></span> Further research is needed to explore the role of sex-differential biological factors, including sex hormones and sex-linked genes, in human development and evolution and their potential links to autism.<span><sup>19</sup></span></p><p>Another commonly used framework in evolutionary psychiatry is that an extreme of an adaptation may manifest in a way that resembles a disorder. While an analytical form of cognition such as systemizing is a strength at a certain level and may underlie technical aptitude, hyper-systemizing could result in the challenges experienced by some autistic people.<span><sup>20</sup></span> Specifically, hyper-systemizing may manifest as intense focus on details and patterns, rigid adherence to routine, feeling overwhelmed by too much detail, and distress when routines are interrupted. As males, on average, score higher on measures of systemizing than females, the tipping point of when high levels of systemizing become “too much of a good thing” could contribute to the higher rates of autism in males.</p><p>In its present state, evolutionary psychiatry mainly exists as an academic discipline, applying theory to understand how evolutionary processes have shaped the human mind and how certain psychiatric conditions might be better understood as adaptations or byproducts of our evolutionary history. However, psychiatry is a dynamic field with best practices adapting in response to changes in our understanding of conditions, their underlying causes, and the best way to provide support. Although applying evolutionary psychiatry theory in clinical practice is a new frontier, early efforts suggest that it is one worth exploring. Clinicians who adopt a holistic approach could help autistic people navigate challenges while directing them how to focus their strengths, such as excellent attention to and memory for detail, pattern-recognition skills, and ability to focus on one topic in great depth.</p><p>One of few randomised controlled trials to put evolutionary psychiatry thinking into practice presented participants with a history of depression with one of two sets of videos: the first explained depression as a disease with known biopsychosocial risk factors, while the second described depression being an evolutionarily conserved signal that serves an adaptive function in specific situations.<span><sup>21</sup></span> Participants who viewed the second set of videos later reported lower self-stigma and stronger beliefs about their agency to reduce depressive symptoms. Whether greater awareness of evolutionary theories could have similar benefits for autistic people remains to be seen.</p><p>Perhaps most obviously, evolutionary psychiatry provides a scientific basis supporting that autism reflects variation in the human brain and thinking, rather than defects that need to be fixed, and supporting shifts in social attitudes. In many ways, the aims of evolutionary psychiatry are shared with those of the neurodiversity movement,<span><sup>22</sup></span> which advocates that the differences associated with autism should be recognised and respected as a normal part of human diversity. For clinicians, understanding the strengths and challenges of autism through the lens of evolution could lead to providing support and accommodations that enable neurodivergent individuals to thrive in different environments. As progress in biomedical understanding of autism has not had a transformative effect on clinical practice, evolutionary psychiatry may present a new framework for understanding autism that is still founded in science.</p>","PeriodicalId":108,"journal":{"name":"Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.3000,"publicationDate":"2024-01-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/acps.13653","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.13653","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHIATRY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Autism is one of most common disabilities, with recent estimates that 1 in 36 children1 have an autism diagnosis and late diagnoses in adults also increasing. The DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism now include sensory sensitivities alongside the long-established criteria of social and communication challenges and repetitive and restrictive behaviours and interests. Most clinicians no longer view autism as something to be cured or treated, instead providing support for the challenges that autistic people experience and treatments or interventions for any co-occurring conditions, while respecting their unique differences and strengths. As the prevalence of autism, who is being diagnosed with autism, and clinical understanding of autism are changing, so are perspectives on what it means to be autistic.

One field aiming to shift scientific perspectives on how we think about autism is evolutionary psychiatry,2 a relatively new discipline that now has its own special interest group within the Royal College of Psychiatrists in the UK as well as dedicated textbooks. Like the related field of evolutionary medicine, evolutionary psychiatry sets its focus beyond biomedical causes (e.g., genetic and environmental factors), instead asking the “ultimate” question of why neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions have persisted in the human population over time. At first glance, autism presents an evolutionary paradox3: it is a highly heritable condition that affects “reproductive success” yet is relatively prevalent. Evolutionary psychiatry proposes possibilities that move us away from viewing autism as a disorder or disease to one that considers the role of crucial adaptations that have shaped the evolution of the human brain and our complex cognitive abilities. In this article, we consider several evolutionary explanations of autism, the supporting evidence, and how embracing an evolutionary perspective could influence clinical practice in psychiatry.

Most evolutionary theories conceptualise autism as a specialised neurocognitive profile characterised by a trade-off between costs (social cognition) and benefits (non-social cognition). The empathising-systemizing theory4 was the first to suggest this, framing autism as enhanced motivation to systemize (the ability to analyse the variables in a system and derive the underlying rules that govern it) paired with challenges in empathising (the ability to imagine another person's thoughts and feelings and respond with an appropriate emotion). The link between autism and occupations that require a high level of systemizing ability5—such as science, technology, engineering, and mathematics—provides further support for the theory that autism reflects a brain specialised in rule-based thinking. Similarly, the “diametric model” of autism6 highlights enhancement in diverse cognitive domains including visuo-spatial skills, reading ability, and local processing, with co-occurring reductions in imagination, shared attention, and personal agency (with, interestingly, opposite patterns observed for psychotic spectrum conditions). Broadly, these two theories view autism as reflecting enhanced development of the analytical brain, with the trade-off of reduced efficiency for processing social information. An updated theory from one author of the diametric model views autism as reflecting “enhanced, but imbalanced, components of intelligence,”7 supported by recent findings of overlap between the genetics of autism and aspects of cognition.

Autism, like many conditions of interest to evolutionary psychiatry, is highly heritable. It is thought that thousands of genetic variants contribute to the likelihood of autism; notably, most of these genetic variants individually have very small effects and are common in the general population.8 As a result, studies often add together autism-associated genes to create an index referred to as a polygenic score. While polygenic scores for autism are, by definition, associated with autism likelihood, they are also associated with various aspects of cognitive ability in the general population9 and brain development.10 Furthermore, there is evidence for positive selection of autism-related genes, implying that possessing autism-related genes (although not necessarily being autistic) was adaptive during human evolution.11 The possibility that the same genes that underlie autism are those that played a crucial role in the development of the human brain and our unique cognitive abilities can help resolve the above-mentioned paradox of why heritable conditions perceived as disabilities could be maintained at high prevalence rates.

Given the link between autism and analytical cognition, as reflected by enhanced systemizing, positive selection for autism-related genes could have been the foundation of the leap in cause-effect thinking that led to humankind's unique ability to innovate.12 In the words of autistic inventor and advocate Temple Grandin: “What would happen if the autism gene was eliminated from the gene pool? You would have a bunch of people standing around in a cave, chatting and socializing and not getting anything done.”13

Evolutionary theory may also be useful for understanding the sex difference in the prevalence of autism. While it is now recognised that autism often presents differently in girls and women, that screening protocols may be biased toward recognising autism in males, and that girls and women are more likely to “camouflage” autism, males are still more likely to receive an autism diagnosis.14

The prenatal sex steroid theory of autism proposes that elevated levels of sex steroid hormones interact with other factors (e.g., genetic variants) to disproportionately increase autism likelihood among males.15 Gonadal sex differentiation during development, which is determined by the sex chromosomes, results in higher average levels of prenatal androgens in males compared to females. High sex steroid hormone levels during pregnancy may be a unique adaptation that has arisen during human evolution, as peak levels exceed than those in non-human primates despite differences in body weight.16 These hormones are believed to play crucial roles during brain development, particularly in terms of cortical expansion and synaptogenesis. Recent experiments involving human stem cell-derived brain organoids show that androgens enhance neuronal development during formation of the cortex,17 a brain region associated with many higher cognitive functions. Together, these lines of evidence suggest that human-specific evolutionary adaptations in the steroid hormone system have resulted in high levels of both androgens and oestrogens which, in turn, are associated with increased autism likelihood in males.18 Further research is needed to explore the role of sex-differential biological factors, including sex hormones and sex-linked genes, in human development and evolution and their potential links to autism.19

Another commonly used framework in evolutionary psychiatry is that an extreme of an adaptation may manifest in a way that resembles a disorder. While an analytical form of cognition such as systemizing is a strength at a certain level and may underlie technical aptitude, hyper-systemizing could result in the challenges experienced by some autistic people.20 Specifically, hyper-systemizing may manifest as intense focus on details and patterns, rigid adherence to routine, feeling overwhelmed by too much detail, and distress when routines are interrupted. As males, on average, score higher on measures of systemizing than females, the tipping point of when high levels of systemizing become “too much of a good thing” could contribute to the higher rates of autism in males.

In its present state, evolutionary psychiatry mainly exists as an academic discipline, applying theory to understand how evolutionary processes have shaped the human mind and how certain psychiatric conditions might be better understood as adaptations or byproducts of our evolutionary history. However, psychiatry is a dynamic field with best practices adapting in response to changes in our understanding of conditions, their underlying causes, and the best way to provide support. Although applying evolutionary psychiatry theory in clinical practice is a new frontier, early efforts suggest that it is one worth exploring. Clinicians who adopt a holistic approach could help autistic people navigate challenges while directing them how to focus their strengths, such as excellent attention to and memory for detail, pattern-recognition skills, and ability to focus on one topic in great depth.

One of few randomised controlled trials to put evolutionary psychiatry thinking into practice presented participants with a history of depression with one of two sets of videos: the first explained depression as a disease with known biopsychosocial risk factors, while the second described depression being an evolutionarily conserved signal that serves an adaptive function in specific situations.21 Participants who viewed the second set of videos later reported lower self-stigma and stronger beliefs about their agency to reduce depressive symptoms. Whether greater awareness of evolutionary theories could have similar benefits for autistic people remains to be seen.

Perhaps most obviously, evolutionary psychiatry provides a scientific basis supporting that autism reflects variation in the human brain and thinking, rather than defects that need to be fixed, and supporting shifts in social attitudes. In many ways, the aims of evolutionary psychiatry are shared with those of the neurodiversity movement,22 which advocates that the differences associated with autism should be recognised and respected as a normal part of human diversity. For clinicians, understanding the strengths and challenges of autism through the lens of evolution could lead to providing support and accommodations that enable neurodivergent individuals to thrive in different environments. As progress in biomedical understanding of autism has not had a transformative effect on clinical practice, evolutionary psychiatry may present a new framework for understanding autism that is still founded in science.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
自闭症的进化理论:对精神病学的影响。
对于临床医生来说,通过进化的视角来理解自闭症的优势和挑战,可以为神经变异者提供支持和便利,使他们能够在不同的环境中茁壮成长。由于对自闭症的生物医学理解方面的进展尚未对临床实践产生变革性影响,进化精神病学可能为理解自闭症提供一个仍然建立在科学基础上的新框架。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 医学-精神病学
CiteScore
11.20
自引率
3.00%
发文量
135
审稿时长
6-12 weeks
期刊介绍: Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica acts as an international forum for the dissemination of information advancing the science and practice of psychiatry. In particular we focus on communicating frontline research to clinical psychiatrists and psychiatric researchers. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica has traditionally been and remains a journal focusing predominantly on clinical psychiatry, but translational psychiatry is a topic of growing importance to our readers. Therefore, the journal welcomes submission of manuscripts based on both clinical- and more translational (e.g. preclinical and epidemiological) research. When preparing manuscripts based on translational studies for submission to Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, the authors should place emphasis on the clinical significance of the research question and the findings. Manuscripts based solely on preclinical research (e.g. animal models) are normally not considered for publication in the Journal.
期刊最新文献
A national evaluation of a multi‐modal, blended, digital intervention integrated within Australian youth mental health services The impact of sex in the effectiveness of functional remediation in bipolar disorder. Relationship between nonexercise activity and mood in patients with eating disorders. Association between exposure to atypical antipsychotics during pregnancy and risk of miscarriage. Childhood maltreatment and outcomes following electroconvulsive therapy in adults with depression.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1