Of hormones (well, not really!), behavior, and observer bias

IF 16.4 1区 化学 Q1 CHEMISTRY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Accounts of Chemical Research Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI:10.1111/eth.13450
Wolfgang Goymann
{"title":"Of hormones (well, not really!), behavior, and observer bias","authors":"Wolfgang Goymann","doi":"10.1111/eth.13450","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>In our laboratory, we measure hormones such as testosterone, melatonin, or corticosterone to relate hormone concentrations to the behavior of animals. Sometimes, we get samples of animals whose hormone concentrations had been altered by hormone implants or injections. Usually, those are measured to test if the hormonal manipulation had worked. I typically ask the experimenter which of the samples are from manipulated animals and which ones are from control animals. As an answer, I sometimes receive a raised eye brow and a questioning face. This kind of answer makes me happy because it shows my cooperation partner is aware of observer bias. I then explain that I would actually prefer not to know, which animals had been manipulated or not. However, our method to measure the hormones, the radioimmunoassay, requires me to know about it. The method is quite sensitive, but has a narrow range of concentrations in which we can reliably tell the correct hormone concentrations. Therefore, we need to adjust the dilution of the samples to remain in the expected range of concentrations we can measure with high precision. Hormone-treated samples might easily fall out of this range, if not diluted properly.</p><p>Presumably, the radioimmunoassay as a biochemical method is unlikely to produce observer bias, but this is different with behavioral observations, where our expectations as experimenters may inadvertently bias data collection. For this reason, good textbooks such as the classic Martin and Bateson (<span>1985</span>) or its latest edition (Bateson &amp; Martin, <span>2021</span>) caution against observer bias and also highlight the importance of testing for inter-observer reliability. A good way to do so is blinding observers to the treatment and apply established methods to test and improve inter-observer reliability.</p><p>About 12 years ago, Gordon Burghardt and colleagues investigated how major journals in animal behavior did with regard to reporting observer bias. They demonstrated that in 2010 major journals of our field (<i>Animal Behaviour</i>, <i>Behaviour</i>, <i>Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology</i>, and <i>Ethology</i>) had observer bias reporting rates of &lt;10%, therefore lagging behind <i>Infancy</i>, a journal on human infant behavior with reporting rates of more than 75% (Burghardt et al., <span>2012</span>). <i>Journal of Comparative Psychology</i> had reporting rates of 20%.</p><p>In this issue of Ethology, Todd Freeberg, Scott Benson, and Gordon Burghardt offer a follow-up study (2024), showing that all behavioral journals have improved on reporting. This is good news! However, with rates in the range of 50% our field still lags behind <i>Infancy</i>, where basically every study reports observer bias and tests for inter-observer reliability. Also, <i>Journal of Comparative Psychology</i> still does better with roughly 75% or articles reporting on observer bias. Admittedly, it is more difficult and sometimes impossible to conduct blind observations in animal behavior studies, especially when data are collected in the field, but for sure there is still scope to improve blinding and reporting. <i>Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology</i> does slightly better than other journals including <i>Ethology</i>, possibly because they have implemented a submission policy requiring authors to report on observer bias. As a response to Freeberg et al. (<span>2024</span>) renewed investigation <i>Ethology</i> now also decided to require authors to report how they dealt with observer bias. Hopefully, in few more years, Freeberg and colleagues can report that we left infancy behind and our journals reached a mature state of dealing with observer bias.</p><p><b>Wolfgang Goymann:</b> Conceptualization; writing – original draft; writing – review and editing.</p>","PeriodicalId":1,"journal":{"name":"Accounts of Chemical Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4000,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13450","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Accounts of Chemical Research","FirstCategoryId":"99","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13450","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"化学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"CHEMISTRY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

In our laboratory, we measure hormones such as testosterone, melatonin, or corticosterone to relate hormone concentrations to the behavior of animals. Sometimes, we get samples of animals whose hormone concentrations had been altered by hormone implants or injections. Usually, those are measured to test if the hormonal manipulation had worked. I typically ask the experimenter which of the samples are from manipulated animals and which ones are from control animals. As an answer, I sometimes receive a raised eye brow and a questioning face. This kind of answer makes me happy because it shows my cooperation partner is aware of observer bias. I then explain that I would actually prefer not to know, which animals had been manipulated or not. However, our method to measure the hormones, the radioimmunoassay, requires me to know about it. The method is quite sensitive, but has a narrow range of concentrations in which we can reliably tell the correct hormone concentrations. Therefore, we need to adjust the dilution of the samples to remain in the expected range of concentrations we can measure with high precision. Hormone-treated samples might easily fall out of this range, if not diluted properly.

Presumably, the radioimmunoassay as a biochemical method is unlikely to produce observer bias, but this is different with behavioral observations, where our expectations as experimenters may inadvertently bias data collection. For this reason, good textbooks such as the classic Martin and Bateson (1985) or its latest edition (Bateson & Martin, 2021) caution against observer bias and also highlight the importance of testing for inter-observer reliability. A good way to do so is blinding observers to the treatment and apply established methods to test and improve inter-observer reliability.

About 12 years ago, Gordon Burghardt and colleagues investigated how major journals in animal behavior did with regard to reporting observer bias. They demonstrated that in 2010 major journals of our field (Animal Behaviour, Behaviour, Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, and Ethology) had observer bias reporting rates of <10%, therefore lagging behind Infancy, a journal on human infant behavior with reporting rates of more than 75% (Burghardt et al., 2012). Journal of Comparative Psychology had reporting rates of 20%.

In this issue of Ethology, Todd Freeberg, Scott Benson, and Gordon Burghardt offer a follow-up study (2024), showing that all behavioral journals have improved on reporting. This is good news! However, with rates in the range of 50% our field still lags behind Infancy, where basically every study reports observer bias and tests for inter-observer reliability. Also, Journal of Comparative Psychology still does better with roughly 75% or articles reporting on observer bias. Admittedly, it is more difficult and sometimes impossible to conduct blind observations in animal behavior studies, especially when data are collected in the field, but for sure there is still scope to improve blinding and reporting. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology does slightly better than other journals including Ethology, possibly because they have implemented a submission policy requiring authors to report on observer bias. As a response to Freeberg et al. (2024) renewed investigation Ethology now also decided to require authors to report how they dealt with observer bias. Hopefully, in few more years, Freeberg and colleagues can report that we left infancy behind and our journals reached a mature state of dealing with observer bias.

Wolfgang Goymann: Conceptualization; writing – original draft; writing – review and editing.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
关于荷尔蒙(其实不是!)、行为和观察者的偏见
在我们的实验室里,我们测量睾酮、褪黑激素或皮质酮等激素,以便将激素浓度与动物的行为联系起来。有时,我们会得到一些动物的样本,这些动物的激素浓度因植入或注射激素而发生了改变。通常,我们会对这些样本进行测量,以检验激素操纵是否奏效。我通常会问实验者,哪些样本来自被操纵的动物,哪些样本来自对照组动物。作为回答,我有时会得到一个挑眉和质疑的表情。这种回答让我很开心,因为这表明我的合作者意识到了观察者的偏见。然后,我解释说,其实我更希望不知道哪些动物是否被操纵过。不过,我们测量荷尔蒙的方法--放射免疫分析法--要求我知道这一点。这种方法相当灵敏,但浓度范围很窄,我们无法可靠地判断激素的正确浓度。因此,我们需要调整样本的稀释度,使其保持在我们可以高精度测量的预期浓度范围内。据推测,放射免疫分析作为一种生化方法不太可能产生观察者偏差,但这与行为观察不同,我们作为实验者的期望可能会在无意中对数据收集产生偏差。因此,经典教科书马丁和贝特森(Martin and Bateson,1985 年)或其最新版本(Bateson & Martin,2021 年)都提醒我们注意观察者偏差,并强调测试观察者间可靠性的重要性。约 12 年前,Gordon Burghardt 及其同事调查了动物行为学主要期刊在报告观察者偏倚方面的情况。他们发现,2010 年本领域主要期刊(《动物行为学》、《行为学》、《行为生态学与社会生物学》和《选育学》)的观察者偏差报告率为 10%,因此落后于报告率超过 75% 的人类婴儿行为期刊《Infancy》(Burghardt 等人,2012 年)。托德-弗里伯格(Todd Freeberg)、斯科特-本森(Scott Benson)和戈登-伯格哈特(Gordon Burghardt)在本期《选育》杂志上提供了一项后续研究(2024 年),结果显示所有行为学期刊的报告率都有所提高。这是一个好消息!然而,我们的研究领域仍然落后于《婴儿学》(Infancy),后者基本上每项研究都会报告观察者偏差并测试观察者间的可靠性。此外,《比较心理学杂志》(Journal of Comparative Psychology)的情况仍然较好,约有 75% 的文章报告了观察者偏差。诚然,在动物行为研究中进行盲法观察比较困难,有时甚至是不可能的,尤其是在野外收集数据时,但可以肯定的是,盲法观察和报告仍有改进的余地。行为生态学与社会生物学》比包括《选集》在内的其他期刊略胜一筹,这可能是因为他们实施了一项投稿政策,要求作者报告观察者偏差。作为对 Freeberg 等人(2024 年)重新调查的回应,《选育》现在也决定要求作者报告他们是如何处理观察者偏差的。希望再过几年,Freeberg 及其同事能报告说,我们已经告别了稚嫩,我们的期刊在处理观察者偏见方面已经达到了成熟的状态。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Accounts of Chemical Research
Accounts of Chemical Research 化学-化学综合
CiteScore
31.40
自引率
1.10%
发文量
312
审稿时长
2 months
期刊介绍: Accounts of Chemical Research presents short, concise and critical articles offering easy-to-read overviews of basic research and applications in all areas of chemistry and biochemistry. These short reviews focus on research from the author’s own laboratory and are designed to teach the reader about a research project. In addition, Accounts of Chemical Research publishes commentaries that give an informed opinion on a current research problem. Special Issues online are devoted to a single topic of unusual activity and significance. Accounts of Chemical Research replaces the traditional article abstract with an article "Conspectus." These entries synopsize the research affording the reader a closer look at the content and significance of an article. Through this provision of a more detailed description of the article contents, the Conspectus enhances the article's discoverability by search engines and the exposure for the research.
期刊最新文献
Management of Cholesteatoma: Hearing Rehabilitation. Congenital Cholesteatoma. Evaluation of Cholesteatoma. Management of Cholesteatoma: Extension Beyond Middle Ear/Mastoid. Recidivism and Recurrence.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1