Barbara M. Tomotani, Aurelia F. T. Strauß, Dmitry Kishkinev, Huib van de Haar, Barbara Helm
{"title":"Circadian clock period length is not consistently linked to chronotype in a wild songbird","authors":"Barbara M. Tomotani, Aurelia F. T. Strauß, Dmitry Kishkinev, Huib van de Haar, Barbara Helm","doi":"10.1111/ejn.16535","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Circadian clock properties vary between individuals and relate to variation in entrained timing in captivity. How this variation translates into behavioural differences in natural settings, however, is poorly understood. Here, we tested in great tits whether variation in the free-running period length (<i>tau</i>) under constant dim light (LL) was linked to the phase angle of the entrained rhythm (“chronotype”) in captivity and in the wild, as recently indicated in our study species. We also assessed links between <i>tau</i> and the timing of first activity onset and offset under LL relative to the last experienced light–dark (LD) cycle. We kept 66 great tits, caught in two winters, in LL for 14 days and subsequently released them with a radio transmitter back to the wild, where their activity and body temperature rhythms were tracked for 1 to 22 days. For a subset of birds, chronotype was also recorded in the lab before release. Neither wild nor lab chronotypes were related to <i>tau</i>. We also found no correlation between lab and wild chronotypes. However, the first onset in LL had a positive relationship with <i>tau</i>, but only in males. Our results demonstrate that links between <i>tau</i> and phase of entrainment, postulated on theoretical grounds, may not consistently hold under natural conditions, possibly due to strong masking. This calls for more holistic research on how the many components of the circadian system interact with the environment to shape timing in the wild.</p><p>Wild birds showed chronotypes in the field that were unlinked to their circadian period length tau measured in captivity. In males only, the first onset of activity after exposure to constant dim light did correlate with <i>tau</i>. Our study emphasises the need to investigate clocks in the real world, including a need to better understand masking.</p>","PeriodicalId":11993,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"60 7","pages":"5522-5536"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7000,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ejn.16535","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"European Journal of Neuroscience","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16535","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"NEUROSCIENCES","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
Circadian clock properties vary between individuals and relate to variation in entrained timing in captivity. How this variation translates into behavioural differences in natural settings, however, is poorly understood. Here, we tested in great tits whether variation in the free-running period length (tau) under constant dim light (LL) was linked to the phase angle of the entrained rhythm (“chronotype”) in captivity and in the wild, as recently indicated in our study species. We also assessed links between tau and the timing of first activity onset and offset under LL relative to the last experienced light–dark (LD) cycle. We kept 66 great tits, caught in two winters, in LL for 14 days and subsequently released them with a radio transmitter back to the wild, where their activity and body temperature rhythms were tracked for 1 to 22 days. For a subset of birds, chronotype was also recorded in the lab before release. Neither wild nor lab chronotypes were related to tau. We also found no correlation between lab and wild chronotypes. However, the first onset in LL had a positive relationship with tau, but only in males. Our results demonstrate that links between tau and phase of entrainment, postulated on theoretical grounds, may not consistently hold under natural conditions, possibly due to strong masking. This calls for more holistic research on how the many components of the circadian system interact with the environment to shape timing in the wild.
Wild birds showed chronotypes in the field that were unlinked to their circadian period length tau measured in captivity. In males only, the first onset of activity after exposure to constant dim light did correlate with tau. Our study emphasises the need to investigate clocks in the real world, including a need to better understand masking.
昼夜节律钟的特性因个体差异而异,并与圈养中的内定时间差异有关。然而,人们对这种变化如何转化为自然环境中的行为差异还知之甚少。在这里,我们测试了大山雀在恒定暗光(LL)条件下的自由运行周期长度(tau)的变化是否与人工饲养和野外环境中的内隐节律相位角("chronotype")有关。我们还评估了tau与相对于最后经历的光-暗(LD)周期的LL下首次活动开始和抵消的时间之间的联系。我们将66只在两个冬季捕获的大山雀在LL环境中饲养了14天,随后将它们带着无线电发射器放归野外,在野外对它们的活动和体温节律进行了1到22天的追踪。对于一部分鸟类,在释放前还在实验室记录了它们的时间型。野外和实验室的时间型都与 tau 无关。我们还发现实验室和野外的时间型之间没有相关性。然而,LL中的首次发病与tau呈正相关,但仅限于雄鸟。我们的研究结果表明,理论上推测的 tau 与夹带阶段之间的联系在自然条件下可能并不完全成立,这可能是由于强烈的掩蔽作用。这就要求对昼夜节律系统的许多组成部分如何与环境相互作用以形成野生时间进行更全面的研究。野生鸟类在野外表现出的时间型与其在人工饲养条件下测量的昼夜节律周期长度tau不一致。只有雄性鸟类在持续昏暗的光线下首次开始活动的时间确实与tau相关。我们的研究强调了在现实世界中研究时钟的必要性,包括更好地理解遮蔽的必要性。
期刊介绍:
EJN is the journal of FENS and supports the international neuroscientific community by publishing original high quality research articles and reviews in all fields of neuroscience. In addition, to engage with issues that are of interest to the science community, we also publish Editorials, Meetings Reports and Neuro-Opinions on topics that are of current interest in the fields of neuroscience research and training in science. We have recently established a series of ‘Profiles of Women in Neuroscience’. Our goal is to provide a vehicle for publications that further the understanding of the structure and function of the nervous system in both health and disease and to provide a vehicle to engage the neuroscience community. As the official journal of FENS, profits from the journal are re-invested in the neuroscientific community through the activities of FENS.