Commentary on "Intelligence and Personality as Predictors of Illness and Death" by Deary, Weiss, and Batty.

James R Flynn
{"title":"Commentary on \"Intelligence and Personality as Predictors of Illness and Death\" by Deary, Weiss, and Batty.","authors":"James R Flynn","doi":"10.1177/1529100610388753","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Congratulations to Deary, Weiss, and Batty (2010, this issue) for an encyclopedic and judicious survey of the literature and for their sensible recommendations as to how medical practitioners must tailor prescriptions to the personality and cognitive ability of patients they address. The suggestions for future research are state of the art in terms of analysis of the kind of data psychologists are likely to collect. However, I suggest that we approach sociologists with hypotheses that might motivate them to supplement our knowledge. The curse of any model is that it is underidentified and encourages us to think that we know what human behavior lies behind the numbers it generates. I will comment on what etiology might lie behind the correlation between low IQ and hospitalization for violence-inflicted injury. Others with broader knowledge will I hope make similar contributions. I was reared in a gang-organized area where gangs were staffed by ethnicity: Blacks versus an alliance of non-Black Catholics (Irish, Italian, Puerto Rican, Filipino). The culture was one of defense of honor and territory by fighting. Teenagers challenged other teenagers to fight: The path between IQ and injury was not a matter of being too unintelligent to have mediation or coping skills. If challenged, any resort to such was proof of cowardice, and the sanctions for that were to be outcast and bullied. If you won, you might have the high status of the best street fighter in your group; if you lost honorably, you were a member of the group in good standing. Gangs challenged each other: Failure to fight and risk injury meant having no place to ‘‘play’’ and low group esteem. Pub culture was a major leisure-time amusement and going to a pub was likely to lead to challenges. Football (gridiron) was a leisure sport that led to challenges. In sum, fighting for honor and territory was not a behavioral manifestation of low IQ. Yet as a group we undoubtedly had a lower mean IQ than Washington, D.C., as a whole. But it was our culture, not low-IQ, that was the active factor. It might seem that allowing for lower socioeconomic status (SES) would capture this etiology. Not entirely: Jewish boys in our neighborhood simply did not go out on the street after school; they socialized through the Synagogue. They would avoid risk because their self-esteem did not include honor as the rest of us defined it. We thought they were cowards, but they did not care. I doubt Swedish data would pick up any of this, and I am impressed that it shows a correlation between IQ and violence-inflicted injury after SES is allowed for. I suspect that functional or dysfunctional cognition is the answer. I suspect that U.S. data would show a stronger correlation, particularly before SES is allowed for. But it would be wrong to conclude that the extra is necessarily due to cognitive rather than ethnic or cultural factors. Therefore, let us have some investigation of behavior on the ground if we want a full understanding of our correlations. It is one thing to help people close the gap between functional and dysfunctional intelligence. It is another thing to assume that they will give up what, for them, defines personal self-esteem and a full life. The physician who wishes to communicate with a patient about reducing the incidence of violent injury may need consultation with a case worker familiar with his neighborhood rather than an IQ score.","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"11 2","pages":"80"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2010-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1529100610388753","citationCount":"1","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100610388753","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"Psychology","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

Abstract

Congratulations to Deary, Weiss, and Batty (2010, this issue) for an encyclopedic and judicious survey of the literature and for their sensible recommendations as to how medical practitioners must tailor prescriptions to the personality and cognitive ability of patients they address. The suggestions for future research are state of the art in terms of analysis of the kind of data psychologists are likely to collect. However, I suggest that we approach sociologists with hypotheses that might motivate them to supplement our knowledge. The curse of any model is that it is underidentified and encourages us to think that we know what human behavior lies behind the numbers it generates. I will comment on what etiology might lie behind the correlation between low IQ and hospitalization for violence-inflicted injury. Others with broader knowledge will I hope make similar contributions. I was reared in a gang-organized area where gangs were staffed by ethnicity: Blacks versus an alliance of non-Black Catholics (Irish, Italian, Puerto Rican, Filipino). The culture was one of defense of honor and territory by fighting. Teenagers challenged other teenagers to fight: The path between IQ and injury was not a matter of being too unintelligent to have mediation or coping skills. If challenged, any resort to such was proof of cowardice, and the sanctions for that were to be outcast and bullied. If you won, you might have the high status of the best street fighter in your group; if you lost honorably, you were a member of the group in good standing. Gangs challenged each other: Failure to fight and risk injury meant having no place to ‘‘play’’ and low group esteem. Pub culture was a major leisure-time amusement and going to a pub was likely to lead to challenges. Football (gridiron) was a leisure sport that led to challenges. In sum, fighting for honor and territory was not a behavioral manifestation of low IQ. Yet as a group we undoubtedly had a lower mean IQ than Washington, D.C., as a whole. But it was our culture, not low-IQ, that was the active factor. It might seem that allowing for lower socioeconomic status (SES) would capture this etiology. Not entirely: Jewish boys in our neighborhood simply did not go out on the street after school; they socialized through the Synagogue. They would avoid risk because their self-esteem did not include honor as the rest of us defined it. We thought they were cowards, but they did not care. I doubt Swedish data would pick up any of this, and I am impressed that it shows a correlation between IQ and violence-inflicted injury after SES is allowed for. I suspect that functional or dysfunctional cognition is the answer. I suspect that U.S. data would show a stronger correlation, particularly before SES is allowed for. But it would be wrong to conclude that the extra is necessarily due to cognitive rather than ethnic or cultural factors. Therefore, let us have some investigation of behavior on the ground if we want a full understanding of our correlations. It is one thing to help people close the gap between functional and dysfunctional intelligence. It is another thing to assume that they will give up what, for them, defines personal self-esteem and a full life. The physician who wishes to communicate with a patient about reducing the incidence of violent injury may need consultation with a case worker familiar with his neighborhood rather than an IQ score.
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
对“智力和人格作为疾病和死亡的预测者”的评论,作者:Deary, Weiss和Batty。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
68.00
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊介绍: Psychological Science in the Public Interest (PSPI) is a unique journal featuring comprehensive and compelling reviews of issues that are of direct relevance to the general public. These reviews are written by blue ribbon teams of specialists representing a range of viewpoints, and are intended to assess the current state-of-the-science with regard to the topic. Among other things, PSPI reports have challenged the validity of the Rorschach and other projective tests; have explored how to keep the aging brain sharp; and have documented problems with the current state of clinical psychology. PSPI reports are regularly featured in Scientific American Mind and are typically covered in a variety of other major media outlets.
期刊最新文献
About the Authors. Addiction: Where Framing Can Be a Matter of Life and Death: Response to Flusberg et al. (2024). How Frames Can Promote Agency: Response to Flusberg et al. (2024). The Psychology of Framing: How Everyday Language Shapes the Way We Think, Feel, and Act. About the Authors.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1