Transparency and Inclusion in Psychological Inquiry: Reflecting on the Past, Embracing the Present, and Building an Inclusive Future

IF 7.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Psychological Inquiry Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI:10.1080/1047840X.2023.2172277
I. Grossmann
{"title":"Transparency and Inclusion in Psychological Inquiry: Reflecting on the Past, Embracing the Present, and Building an Inclusive Future","authors":"I. Grossmann","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2023.2172277","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"s of the target articles, first determining common topics and subsequently calculating relative weight of twelve most frequently mentioned topics over time. Figure 1 shows themes which have dominated the discourse so far. Constant features are the topics of motivation (incl. needs and goals) and self-control, cognitive processes and their metacognitive regulation, mental health and well-being, individual differences and social cognition, as well as theoretical issues concerning research methods in psychology. Dominance of some themes reflects the Zeitgeist. The topic of meta-science—present in the journal since the first issue—become especially prominent in the last decade of Open Science movement. On the other hand, the topic of mental health and well-being was pronounced around the time of the discussions about well-being and the subsequent emergence of the Positive Psychology field in late 1990searly 2000s. In the new millennium, cultural diversity and related societal issues became salient, with the trend continuing to this day. Further, judgment and decision-making made a big entry in the last 15 years, possibly due the Nobel Prize in economics to Kahneman in 2002, and greater focus on behavioral economics thereafter. Toward Greater Equity and Diversity of Submissions The original idea behind Psychological Inquiry—a dialogue through open peer exchange about contentious ideas and theories—remains as important today as it was over three decades ago. Interdisciplinary research is on the rise (Van Noorden, 2015). Therefore, concepts and theories have an opportunity to be enriched by perspectives coming from different fields of studies. At the same time, intellectual silos and cultural echo-chambers remain—while more scholars today work in interdisciplinary teams of specialists than before (“Why Interdisciplinary Research Matters,” 2015), focus on specialization can also produce intellectual silos within one’s discipline. Such silos are often not conducive to the cumulative advancement of science. Scientific silos may be especially damaging for psychology (Cacioppo, 2007), where theoretical approaches touch on many neighboring disciplines, from anthropology and economics, to biology, linguistics, and neuroscience, to philosophy and education, to sociology and political science, to health studies, and so on (Boyack, Klavans, & B€orner, 2005). Scholars connecting closer to one of the neighboring fields may diverge in their grand theories, favor methodological paradigms others may find peculiar or simply be unfamiliar with, and develop their own jargon, all contributing to confusion about the concepts, methods, and evaluation of the results. How can we combat such disciplinary isolationism? An idea pursued by Psychological Inquiry since its inception has been to provide scholars with an opportunity for a civil discussion and debate of diverse ideas, and promoting a dialogue to clarify misunderstandings about theories, methods, or interpretation of core results. Notably, diversity of ideas is unlikely to emerge without reckoning with the possible blind spots and biases in our field. Though scholars have pointed out lack of diversity in participants in psychological studies (and thus models for human behavior) throughout twentieth century (e.g., Sears, 1986), only in the last decade the issue of limited diversity in studied participants came to the forefront of discussions about how to improve generalizability of our theories and phenomena. As Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan (2010) pointed out, for most psychological studies in the twentieth century, the modal participant in psychological research has been a white college student at an elite American college. A decade since their influential paper on how nine out of ten participants in psychological research came from Western, English-Speaking, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) countries, the issue with lack of sampling diversity remains (e.g., Cheon, Melani, & Hong, 2020; Hruschka, Medin, Rogoff, & Henrich, 2018). Platforms of online crowdworkers (e.g., Amazon Mechanical Turk or Prolific Academic) taking part in surveys and experiments for modest remuneration have helped to expand psychological research beyond college students, but also introduced structural inequalities and new limitations: such crowdworking platforms are less available in the Global South, with a few exceptions rely on higher English language proficiency, and restrict the scope of studied phenomena to those that can be administered online. Two related blind points that researchers pay insufficient attention to concern the situated nature of psychological phenomena in cultural (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Shweder, 1991), and historical contexts (Gergen, 1978; Muthukrishna, Henrich, & Slingerland, 2021; Varnum & Grossmann, 2021; Vygotsky, 1978). For many psychological scientists like myself, it appears self-evident that most psychological phenomena are constrained through culturallyand ecologically-mediated beliefs, habits, patterns of socialization and practices. And whenever culture changes, changes in the inner workings of phenomena often follow suit. Yet beyond lip-service to the importance of considering theoretical implications of these insights, much of our field appears more fascinated by phenomena that claim universality rather than cultural or temporal specificity. We often take phenomena that may be unique to a specific context (often the United States) and present their inner workings as psychological universals. And if we do entertain the idea of cultural context, it is as often as a “moderator” of phenomena—i.e., a variable that is theorized to be separate from the phenomenon one aims to explore rather than as part of the system it is embedded in (see Barrett, 2022, for an example why this approach may be misleading). Finally, consider discussions about topics such as social class and inequality, polarization, education, or mental health: A “WEIRD” account of a given psychological phenomenon often emerges as a dominant one, the one to compare other accounts against. As a result, non-WEIRD scholars have a harder time presenting equally valuable insights concerning different inner working of a given phenomenon in their cultural context. These observations about limited diversity in our field have implications for the advancement of theory in psychology. If the model of human behavior is based on an TRANSPARENCY AND INCLUSION IN PSYCHOLOGICAL INQUIRY 235","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.2000,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Psychological Inquiry","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2023.2172277","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

s of the target articles, first determining common topics and subsequently calculating relative weight of twelve most frequently mentioned topics over time. Figure 1 shows themes which have dominated the discourse so far. Constant features are the topics of motivation (incl. needs and goals) and self-control, cognitive processes and their metacognitive regulation, mental health and well-being, individual differences and social cognition, as well as theoretical issues concerning research methods in psychology. Dominance of some themes reflects the Zeitgeist. The topic of meta-science—present in the journal since the first issue—become especially prominent in the last decade of Open Science movement. On the other hand, the topic of mental health and well-being was pronounced around the time of the discussions about well-being and the subsequent emergence of the Positive Psychology field in late 1990searly 2000s. In the new millennium, cultural diversity and related societal issues became salient, with the trend continuing to this day. Further, judgment and decision-making made a big entry in the last 15 years, possibly due the Nobel Prize in economics to Kahneman in 2002, and greater focus on behavioral economics thereafter. Toward Greater Equity and Diversity of Submissions The original idea behind Psychological Inquiry—a dialogue through open peer exchange about contentious ideas and theories—remains as important today as it was over three decades ago. Interdisciplinary research is on the rise (Van Noorden, 2015). Therefore, concepts and theories have an opportunity to be enriched by perspectives coming from different fields of studies. At the same time, intellectual silos and cultural echo-chambers remain—while more scholars today work in interdisciplinary teams of specialists than before (“Why Interdisciplinary Research Matters,” 2015), focus on specialization can also produce intellectual silos within one’s discipline. Such silos are often not conducive to the cumulative advancement of science. Scientific silos may be especially damaging for psychology (Cacioppo, 2007), where theoretical approaches touch on many neighboring disciplines, from anthropology and economics, to biology, linguistics, and neuroscience, to philosophy and education, to sociology and political science, to health studies, and so on (Boyack, Klavans, & B€orner, 2005). Scholars connecting closer to one of the neighboring fields may diverge in their grand theories, favor methodological paradigms others may find peculiar or simply be unfamiliar with, and develop their own jargon, all contributing to confusion about the concepts, methods, and evaluation of the results. How can we combat such disciplinary isolationism? An idea pursued by Psychological Inquiry since its inception has been to provide scholars with an opportunity for a civil discussion and debate of diverse ideas, and promoting a dialogue to clarify misunderstandings about theories, methods, or interpretation of core results. Notably, diversity of ideas is unlikely to emerge without reckoning with the possible blind spots and biases in our field. Though scholars have pointed out lack of diversity in participants in psychological studies (and thus models for human behavior) throughout twentieth century (e.g., Sears, 1986), only in the last decade the issue of limited diversity in studied participants came to the forefront of discussions about how to improve generalizability of our theories and phenomena. As Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan (2010) pointed out, for most psychological studies in the twentieth century, the modal participant in psychological research has been a white college student at an elite American college. A decade since their influential paper on how nine out of ten participants in psychological research came from Western, English-Speaking, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) countries, the issue with lack of sampling diversity remains (e.g., Cheon, Melani, & Hong, 2020; Hruschka, Medin, Rogoff, & Henrich, 2018). Platforms of online crowdworkers (e.g., Amazon Mechanical Turk or Prolific Academic) taking part in surveys and experiments for modest remuneration have helped to expand psychological research beyond college students, but also introduced structural inequalities and new limitations: such crowdworking platforms are less available in the Global South, with a few exceptions rely on higher English language proficiency, and restrict the scope of studied phenomena to those that can be administered online. Two related blind points that researchers pay insufficient attention to concern the situated nature of psychological phenomena in cultural (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Shweder, 1991), and historical contexts (Gergen, 1978; Muthukrishna, Henrich, & Slingerland, 2021; Varnum & Grossmann, 2021; Vygotsky, 1978). For many psychological scientists like myself, it appears self-evident that most psychological phenomena are constrained through culturallyand ecologically-mediated beliefs, habits, patterns of socialization and practices. And whenever culture changes, changes in the inner workings of phenomena often follow suit. Yet beyond lip-service to the importance of considering theoretical implications of these insights, much of our field appears more fascinated by phenomena that claim universality rather than cultural or temporal specificity. We often take phenomena that may be unique to a specific context (often the United States) and present their inner workings as psychological universals. And if we do entertain the idea of cultural context, it is as often as a “moderator” of phenomena—i.e., a variable that is theorized to be separate from the phenomenon one aims to explore rather than as part of the system it is embedded in (see Barrett, 2022, for an example why this approach may be misleading). Finally, consider discussions about topics such as social class and inequality, polarization, education, or mental health: A “WEIRD” account of a given psychological phenomenon often emerges as a dominant one, the one to compare other accounts against. As a result, non-WEIRD scholars have a harder time presenting equally valuable insights concerning different inner working of a given phenomenon in their cultural context. These observations about limited diversity in our field have implications for the advancement of theory in psychology. If the model of human behavior is based on an TRANSPARENCY AND INCLUSION IN PSYCHOLOGICAL INQUIRY 235
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
心理探究的透明与包容:反思过去,拥抱现在,构建包容的未来
S的目标文章,首先确定常见主题,然后计算12个最常被提及的主题随时间的相对权重。图1显示了迄今为止主导话语的主题。不变的特征是动机(包括需求和目标)和自我控制,认知过程及其元认知调节,心理健康和福祉,个体差异和社会认知,以及有关心理学研究方法的理论问题。一些主题的主导反映了时代精神。元科学的话题从第一期开始就出现在杂志上,在开放科学运动的过去十年中变得尤为突出。另一方面,心理健康和幸福的话题是在关于幸福的讨论以及随后在20世纪90年代末至21世纪初积极心理学领域出现的时候提出的。在新的千年里,文化多样性和相关的社会问题变得突出,这种趋势一直持续到今天。此外,判断和决策在过去15年里取得了重大进展,可能是由于卡尼曼在2002年获得了诺贝尔经济学奖,以及此后对行为经济学的更多关注。心理学探究背后的最初理念——通过公开的同行交流来讨论有争议的观点和理论——在今天仍然和三十多年前一样重要。跨学科研究正在兴起(Van Noorden, 2015)。因此,概念和理论有机会被来自不同研究领域的观点所丰富。与此同时,知识孤岛和文化回声室仍然存在——尽管今天有更多的学者在跨学科的专家团队中工作(“为什么跨学科研究很重要”,2015),但专注于专业化也会在一个学科内产生知识孤岛。这样的竖井往往不利于科学的累积进步。科学的筒井可能对心理学尤其有害(Cacioppo, 2007),其中理论方法涉及许多邻近学科,从人类学和经济学,到生物学,语言学和神经科学,到哲学和教育学,到社会学和政治学,到健康研究等等(Boyack, Klavans, & B€orner, 2005)。与一个相邻领域联系更紧密的学者可能会在他们的大理论上产生分歧,倾向于其他人可能觉得奇怪或根本不熟悉的方法范式,并发展自己的行话,所有这些都导致了对概念、方法和结果评估的混淆。我们怎样才能反对这种学科孤立主义呢?《心理探究》自成立以来一直追求的一个理念是,为学者们提供一个对不同观点进行民间讨论和辩论的机会,并促进对话,以澄清对理论、方法或核心结果解释的误解。值得注意的是,如果不考虑我们领域中可能存在的盲点和偏见,就不可能出现思想的多样性。尽管学者们在整个二十世纪都指出心理学研究(以及人类行为模型)的参与者缺乏多样性(例如,Sears, 1986),但直到最近十年,研究参与者的有限多样性问题才成为讨论如何提高理论和现象的普遍性的前沿。正如Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan(2010)所指出的,在20世纪的大多数心理学研究中,心理学研究的模态参与者一直是美国精英大学的白人大学生。十年前,他们发表了一篇有影响力的论文,阐述了心理学研究中十分之九的参与者来自西方、英语、工业化、富裕和民主(WEIRD)国家,但缺乏抽样多样性的问题仍然存在(例如,Cheon, Melani, & Hong, 2020;Hruschka, Medin, Rogoff, & Henrich, 2018)。在线众包工作者平台(例如Amazon Mechanical Turk或多产学术)参与调查和实验,获得适度报酬,这有助于将心理学研究扩展到大学生之外,但也引入了结构性不平等和新的限制:这样的众筹平台在南半球很少出现,只有少数例外,它们依赖于更高的英语水平,并且将研究的范围限制在那些可以在线管理的现象上。研究人员关注不足的两个相关盲点是文化中心理现象的情境性(如Markus & Kitayama, 1991;Shweder, 1991)和历史背景(Gergen, 1978;Muthukrishna, Henrich, & Slingerland, 2021;瓦纳姆和格罗斯曼,2021;维果斯基,1978)。 对于许多像我这样的心理科学家来说,大多数心理现象都受到文化和生态介导的信仰、习惯、社会化模式和实践的约束,这似乎是不言而喻的。每当文化发生变化,现象的内部运作也会随之发生变化。然而,除了口头上强调考虑这些见解的理论含义的重要性之外,我们这个领域的许多人似乎更着迷于那些声称具有普遍性而不是文化或时间特殊性的现象。我们经常把可能是特定环境(通常是美国)特有的现象作为心理共性来呈现它们的内部运作。如果我们接受文化背景的概念,它通常是现象的“调节器”,即。,这是一个变量,从理论上讲,它与人们旨在探索的现象是分开的,而不是作为它所嵌入的系统的一部分(见巴雷特,2022,关于为什么这种方法可能会误导的例子)。最后,考虑一下关于社会阶级和不平等、两极分化、教育或心理健康等话题的讨论:对特定心理现象的“怪异”解释通常会作为主导解释出现,并与其他解释进行比较。因此,非weird学者很难就其文化背景下特定现象的不同内在运作提出同样有价值的见解。这些关于我们这个领域有限的多样性的观察对心理学理论的进步具有启示意义。如果人类行为的模型是建立在心理探究的透明和包容的基础上的话
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Psychological Inquiry
Psychological Inquiry PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY-
CiteScore
10.30
自引率
1.10%
发文量
31
期刊介绍: Psychological Inquiry serves as an international journal dedicated to the advancement of psychological theory. Each edition features an extensive target article exploring a controversial or provocative topic, accompanied by peer commentaries and a response from the target author(s). Proposals for target articles must be submitted using the Target Article Proposal Form, and only approved proposals undergo peer review by at least three reviewers. Authors are invited to submit their full articles after the proposal has received approval from the Editor.
期刊最新文献
How Prevalent is Social Projection? The Future of Social Perception Models: Further Directions for Theoretical Development of the Inductive Reasoning Model Social Projection and Cognitive Differentiation Co-Explain Self-Enhancement and in-Group Favoritism Three Pokes into the Comfort Zone of the Inductive Reasoning Model Inductive Reasoning Renewed: A Reply to Commentators
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1