Performance at a Precognitive Remote Viewing Task, with and without Ganzfeld Stimulation: Three Experiments

Q2 Psychology Journal of Parapsychology Pub Date : 2020-04-01 DOI:10.30891/jopar.2020.01.06
C. Roe, C. E. Cooper, L. Hickinbotham, Andrew Hodrien, L. Kirkwood, H. Martin
{"title":"Performance at a Precognitive Remote Viewing Task, with and without Ganzfeld Stimulation: Three Experiments","authors":"C. Roe, C. E. Cooper, L. Hickinbotham, Andrew Hodrien, L. Kirkwood, H. Martin","doi":"10.30891/jopar.2020.01.06","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Recent research by the lead author has sought to incorporate ganzfeld stimulation as part of a remote viewing protocol. An initial exploratory experiment (Roe & Flint, 2007) suggested that novice participants can successfully describe a randomly selected target location while in the ganzfeld context but did not make a direct comparison with performance in a waking state. This paper describes a series of three subsequent experiments that compared performance at a remote viewing task in a waking condition with a ganzfeld stimulation condition using a counterbalanced repeated measures design. There were only minor variations in design across the three experiments to enable combination of data in a summary analysis. In total, 110 participants produced 43 hits in the ganzfeld stimulation condition (39%), giving a highly significant positive deviation from chance expectation (sum of ranks = 225, p = .000012), whereas in the waking RV condition they achieved 30 hits (27.5%), which is marginally better than chance expectation (sum of ranks = 253, p = .034). The difference in z scores for target ratings in the two conditions approached significance (t[39] = 1.86, p = .065). In experiment 1, individual difference measures identified as predictors of psi performance were unrelated to target ratings. Participants completed Pekala’s (1991) Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI) in order to gauge their responsiveness to the ganzfeld protocol and of the 12 sub-dimensions tested, ganzfeld performance correlated significantly with greater absorption in their subjective experience, lower arousal, and less internal dialogue. In experiments 2 and 3 individual differences measure were replaced by measures of transliminality, openness to experience, and dissociative experiences, but these were unrelated to task success. Data from experiment 2 did not confirm the findings using the PCI from experiment 1, though a significant association was found with the time sense dimension. In experiment 3 no PCI dimensions correlated with task performance, a pattern that was confirmed when data were combined across all three experiments. Remote viewing (RV) can be defined as “the ability to perceive and to be able to describe what would be experienced if one were at some specified distant location” (after Hansel, 1989, p. 160). Al1 We should like to thank the Perrott-Warrick Fund, the Society for Psychical Research Research Grants Committee and the Parapsychological Association Research Endowment (PARE) Fund for their kind financial support of the experiments included in this series. Address correspondence to: Prof. Chris Roe, Faculty of Health, Education & Society, The University of Northampton, University Drive, , Northampton NN1 5PH, UK. © 2020 Roe et al. http://doi.org/10.30891/jopar2020.01.06 Journal of Parapsychology 2020, Vol. 84, No. 1, 38-65 Open Access This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Open Access 39 PRECOGNITIVE REMOTE VIEWING though the method can vary in practice (cf. Schwartz, 2015; Utts & May, 2003), experimental work typically involves a protocol in which the sender visits a randomly selected remote location and actively engages with the target material by attending to the features of the site and participating in activities appropriate to it (see Targ, 1994, for a more detailed description). Meanwhile, the receiver is led through a series of visualization techniques while in an ordinary waking state of consciousness by an experimenter who, masked to the identity of the target, directs them to describe particular features of the site using an interview format (Baptista, Derakshani, & Tressoldi, 2015). From its inception at SRI as a means of testing for ESP with Ingo Swann and its first published formal testing with Pat Price (Targ & Puthoff, 1974, 2005), the method seems to have been remarkably successful; so much so that when Utts (1996) was asked to review the evidence accumulated under the SRI and SAIC programs, she asserted: “Using the standards applied to any other area of science it is concluded that psychic functioning has been well established” (p. 3). Some of the early work at SRI has been criticized (Marks & Kamman, 1980), particularly with respect to potential problems with the randomization and editing of transcripts, which might have left cues to the order in which sites served as targets. These concerns were challenged by Tart, Puthoff, and Targ (1980), who demonstrated that when cues were removed a new independent judge was still able to match transcripts to target sites to a highly significant degree. Later, successful replications (e.g., Schlitz & Gruber, 1980, 1981; Schlitz & Haight, 1984), similarly took great care to ensure that neither the order of target selection nor of the transcripts could be inferred from material they contained. However, part of that solution involves either editing the transcripts, which itself can be grounds for criticism (e.g., Marks & Kamman, 1980, p. 16), or deferring feedback about target identities until the end of the series, which may be de-motivating (see, e.g., Tart, 2007). Of course, these concerns only apply to studies in which the same participant serves as viewer for a number of trials in the series, and thus is potentially able to refer in their transcripts to earlier targets and later planned sessions — this would not be possible if one were to adopt a design in which a larger number of participants contributed just one trial each. Militating against the use of a larger sample of participants is the difficulty in finding a sufficient number of able persons; for example, Utts (1996) estimated that only around 1% of those screened were suitable for RV work. This might be overcome if an induction procedure could be identified that facilitated the performance of novice participants. One candidate is the ganzfeld induction procedure. Although the ganzfeld does not necessarily induce a hypnagogic state (Wackermann et al., 2002), it does seem to share properties with other psi-conducive states that distinguish it from a “standard” RV protocol, such as systematically reducing external sensory stimulation and passively shifting the participants’ attention to internal sources of information (Braud & Braud, 1973; Honorton, 1977; Parker, 1975). There is some evidence to suggest that novice participants may be able to succeed at a free response ESP task under laboratory conditions where it incorporates a ganzfeld-induced altered state of consciousness (ASC; e.g., Baptista et al., 2015; Storm et al., 2010).2 The lead author conducted a pilot study (Roe & Flint, 2007) to test the speculation that ganzfeld stimulation might enable novice participants to succeed at a remote viewing task. Fourteen sender-receiver pairs of novice participants each contributed one remote viewing trial. Receivers underwent a 2 This is not to say that unselected participants are necessarily able to perform at similar levels as selected participants, but rather to note that unselected participants may be able to perform above chance expectation when conditions are conducive. 40 ROE, COOPER, HICKINBOTHAM, HODRIEN, KIRKWOOD & MARTIN progressive relaxation induction procedure followed by ganzfeld stimulation, during which they reported their sensory experiences, with the intention of describing a randomly-selected target site to which their sender partner had been sent. On completion of the trial the sender returned to provide feedback about the nature of the target. An independent judge ranked all 8 possible locations against each mentation, producing 12 binary hits across the 14 trials and a combined sum of ranks that was significant (SOR = 42, p = .008), suggesting that this approach might overcome the weaknesses just outlined. Although the study was successful, it was not clear whether this was a consequence of incorporating a ganzfeld protocol for our novice participants, since we did not have a comparison condition in which those participants attempted to generate impressions about a target location without the assistance of ganzfeld stimulation. The current experimental series was designed to address this shortcoming. Recruiting a wide range of participants allows researchers to explore various individual differences factors (such as personality, belief, and prior experiences) to determine whether they are associated with task success. Given that extant remote viewing research had paid relatively little attention to individual differences, we took our lead from other free response literature. We speculated (after Honorton, 1997; Roe, Jones & Maddern, 2007) that performance might be related to practice of a mental discipline, personal psi experience, paranormal belief, Feeling-Perceiving personality type as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, extraversion, and self-rated creativity (see Cardeña & Marcusson-Clavertz, 2015, for a more thorough review of individual differences variables associated with psi). Additionally, working with a range of participants allows us to consider individual differences in responsiveness to ganzfeld stimulation. The lead author has been a vocal advocate (e.g., Roe, 2009) of Stanford’s criticism of ganzfeld researchers for implicitly assuming that this induction procedure elicits a uniform response from all participants. In practice it is clear that some participants experience no shift at all from their ordinary waking state so that they will not benefit from any psi-conducive properties it supposedly confers. We therefore planned to investigate whether subjective shifts in state of consciousness, as measured using Pekala’s (1991) Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI) were associated with better performance at the remote viewing task. Finally, it is di","PeriodicalId":39641,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Parapsychology","volume":"84 1","pages":"38-65"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2020-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"5","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Parapsychology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.30891/jopar.2020.01.06","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"Psychology","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5

Abstract

Recent research by the lead author has sought to incorporate ganzfeld stimulation as part of a remote viewing protocol. An initial exploratory experiment (Roe & Flint, 2007) suggested that novice participants can successfully describe a randomly selected target location while in the ganzfeld context but did not make a direct comparison with performance in a waking state. This paper describes a series of three subsequent experiments that compared performance at a remote viewing task in a waking condition with a ganzfeld stimulation condition using a counterbalanced repeated measures design. There were only minor variations in design across the three experiments to enable combination of data in a summary analysis. In total, 110 participants produced 43 hits in the ganzfeld stimulation condition (39%), giving a highly significant positive deviation from chance expectation (sum of ranks = 225, p = .000012), whereas in the waking RV condition they achieved 30 hits (27.5%), which is marginally better than chance expectation (sum of ranks = 253, p = .034). The difference in z scores for target ratings in the two conditions approached significance (t[39] = 1.86, p = .065). In experiment 1, individual difference measures identified as predictors of psi performance were unrelated to target ratings. Participants completed Pekala’s (1991) Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI) in order to gauge their responsiveness to the ganzfeld protocol and of the 12 sub-dimensions tested, ganzfeld performance correlated significantly with greater absorption in their subjective experience, lower arousal, and less internal dialogue. In experiments 2 and 3 individual differences measure were replaced by measures of transliminality, openness to experience, and dissociative experiences, but these were unrelated to task success. Data from experiment 2 did not confirm the findings using the PCI from experiment 1, though a significant association was found with the time sense dimension. In experiment 3 no PCI dimensions correlated with task performance, a pattern that was confirmed when data were combined across all three experiments. Remote viewing (RV) can be defined as “the ability to perceive and to be able to describe what would be experienced if one were at some specified distant location” (after Hansel, 1989, p. 160). Al1 We should like to thank the Perrott-Warrick Fund, the Society for Psychical Research Research Grants Committee and the Parapsychological Association Research Endowment (PARE) Fund for their kind financial support of the experiments included in this series. Address correspondence to: Prof. Chris Roe, Faculty of Health, Education & Society, The University of Northampton, University Drive, , Northampton NN1 5PH, UK. © 2020 Roe et al. http://doi.org/10.30891/jopar2020.01.06 Journal of Parapsychology 2020, Vol. 84, No. 1, 38-65 Open Access This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Open Access 39 PRECOGNITIVE REMOTE VIEWING though the method can vary in practice (cf. Schwartz, 2015; Utts & May, 2003), experimental work typically involves a protocol in which the sender visits a randomly selected remote location and actively engages with the target material by attending to the features of the site and participating in activities appropriate to it (see Targ, 1994, for a more detailed description). Meanwhile, the receiver is led through a series of visualization techniques while in an ordinary waking state of consciousness by an experimenter who, masked to the identity of the target, directs them to describe particular features of the site using an interview format (Baptista, Derakshani, & Tressoldi, 2015). From its inception at SRI as a means of testing for ESP with Ingo Swann and its first published formal testing with Pat Price (Targ & Puthoff, 1974, 2005), the method seems to have been remarkably successful; so much so that when Utts (1996) was asked to review the evidence accumulated under the SRI and SAIC programs, she asserted: “Using the standards applied to any other area of science it is concluded that psychic functioning has been well established” (p. 3). Some of the early work at SRI has been criticized (Marks & Kamman, 1980), particularly with respect to potential problems with the randomization and editing of transcripts, which might have left cues to the order in which sites served as targets. These concerns were challenged by Tart, Puthoff, and Targ (1980), who demonstrated that when cues were removed a new independent judge was still able to match transcripts to target sites to a highly significant degree. Later, successful replications (e.g., Schlitz & Gruber, 1980, 1981; Schlitz & Haight, 1984), similarly took great care to ensure that neither the order of target selection nor of the transcripts could be inferred from material they contained. However, part of that solution involves either editing the transcripts, which itself can be grounds for criticism (e.g., Marks & Kamman, 1980, p. 16), or deferring feedback about target identities until the end of the series, which may be de-motivating (see, e.g., Tart, 2007). Of course, these concerns only apply to studies in which the same participant serves as viewer for a number of trials in the series, and thus is potentially able to refer in their transcripts to earlier targets and later planned sessions — this would not be possible if one were to adopt a design in which a larger number of participants contributed just one trial each. Militating against the use of a larger sample of participants is the difficulty in finding a sufficient number of able persons; for example, Utts (1996) estimated that only around 1% of those screened were suitable for RV work. This might be overcome if an induction procedure could be identified that facilitated the performance of novice participants. One candidate is the ganzfeld induction procedure. Although the ganzfeld does not necessarily induce a hypnagogic state (Wackermann et al., 2002), it does seem to share properties with other psi-conducive states that distinguish it from a “standard” RV protocol, such as systematically reducing external sensory stimulation and passively shifting the participants’ attention to internal sources of information (Braud & Braud, 1973; Honorton, 1977; Parker, 1975). There is some evidence to suggest that novice participants may be able to succeed at a free response ESP task under laboratory conditions where it incorporates a ganzfeld-induced altered state of consciousness (ASC; e.g., Baptista et al., 2015; Storm et al., 2010).2 The lead author conducted a pilot study (Roe & Flint, 2007) to test the speculation that ganzfeld stimulation might enable novice participants to succeed at a remote viewing task. Fourteen sender-receiver pairs of novice participants each contributed one remote viewing trial. Receivers underwent a 2 This is not to say that unselected participants are necessarily able to perform at similar levels as selected participants, but rather to note that unselected participants may be able to perform above chance expectation when conditions are conducive. 40 ROE, COOPER, HICKINBOTHAM, HODRIEN, KIRKWOOD & MARTIN progressive relaxation induction procedure followed by ganzfeld stimulation, during which they reported their sensory experiences, with the intention of describing a randomly-selected target site to which their sender partner had been sent. On completion of the trial the sender returned to provide feedback about the nature of the target. An independent judge ranked all 8 possible locations against each mentation, producing 12 binary hits across the 14 trials and a combined sum of ranks that was significant (SOR = 42, p = .008), suggesting that this approach might overcome the weaknesses just outlined. Although the study was successful, it was not clear whether this was a consequence of incorporating a ganzfeld protocol for our novice participants, since we did not have a comparison condition in which those participants attempted to generate impressions about a target location without the assistance of ganzfeld stimulation. The current experimental series was designed to address this shortcoming. Recruiting a wide range of participants allows researchers to explore various individual differences factors (such as personality, belief, and prior experiences) to determine whether they are associated with task success. Given that extant remote viewing research had paid relatively little attention to individual differences, we took our lead from other free response literature. We speculated (after Honorton, 1997; Roe, Jones & Maddern, 2007) that performance might be related to practice of a mental discipline, personal psi experience, paranormal belief, Feeling-Perceiving personality type as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, extraversion, and self-rated creativity (see Cardeña & Marcusson-Clavertz, 2015, for a more thorough review of individual differences variables associated with psi). Additionally, working with a range of participants allows us to consider individual differences in responsiveness to ganzfeld stimulation. The lead author has been a vocal advocate (e.g., Roe, 2009) of Stanford’s criticism of ganzfeld researchers for implicitly assuming that this induction procedure elicits a uniform response from all participants. In practice it is clear that some participants experience no shift at all from their ordinary waking state so that they will not benefit from any psi-conducive properties it supposedly confers. We therefore planned to investigate whether subjective shifts in state of consciousness, as measured using Pekala’s (1991) Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI) were associated with better performance at the remote viewing task. Finally, it is di
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
在有和没有Ganzfeld刺激的情况下,感知前远程观察任务的性能:三个实验
主要作者最近的研究试图将ganzfeld刺激作为远程观看协议的一部分。一项初步的探索性实验(Roe&Flint,2007)表明,新手参与者在ganzfeld环境中可以成功描述随机选择的目标位置,但没有与清醒状态下的表现进行直接比较。本文描述了一系列随后的三个实验,这些实验使用平衡重复测量设计,比较了清醒条件下远程观看任务和ganzfeld刺激条件下的性能。三个实验的设计只有微小的变化,以便在总结分析中结合数据。总的来说,110名参与者在甘茨菲尔德刺激条件下打出了43支安打(39%),与机会预期有非常显著的正偏差(秩和=225,p=0.00012),而在清醒的RV条件下,他们打出了30支安打,(27.5%),这略好于机会预期(秩和=253,p=.034)。两种情况下目标评分的z分差异接近显著性(t[39]=1.86,p=.065)。在实验1中,被确定为psi表现预测因素的个体差异测量与目标评分无关。参与者完成了Pekala(1991)的意识现象量表(PCI),以衡量他们对ganzfeld方案的反应,在测试的12个子维度中,ganzfelt表现与他们对主观体验的更大吸收、更低的唤醒和更少的内部对话显著相关。在实验2和3中,个体差异测量被越界性、对经验的开放性和游离经验的测量所取代,但这些与任务成功无关。来自实验2的数据没有证实使用来自实验1的PCI的发现,尽管发现与时间感维度显著相关。在实验3中,没有PCI维度与任务性能相关,当将所有三个实验的数据组合在一起时,这一模式得到了证实。远程观看(RV)可以被定义为“感知并能够描述如果一个人在某个特定的遥远位置会经历什么的能力”(根据Hansel,1989,第160页)。Al1我们要感谢佩罗特-沃里克基金会、心理研究学会研究资助委员会和副心理协会研究基金会(PARE)对本系列实验的资助。通信地址:Chris Roe教授,北安普顿大学健康、教育与社会学院,英国北安普顿NN1 5PH大学路。©2020 Roe等人。http://doi.org/10.30891/jopar2020.01.06《心理学杂志2020》,第84卷,第1期,38-65开放获取这是一篇根据知识共享署名许可证条款分发的开放获取文章,该许可证允许在任何媒体上不受限制地使用、分发和复制,前提是原始作者和来源可信。Open Access 39 PRECOGNITIVE REMOTE VIEVING尽管该方法在实践中可能有所不同(参见Schwartz,2015;Utts&May,2003),实验工作通常涉及一种协议,在该协议中,发送者访问随机选择的远程位置,并通过关注网站的特征和参与适合它的活动来积极参与目标材料(参见Targ,1994,了解更详细的描述)。与此同时,在普通清醒的意识状态下,实验者会引导接受者通过一系列可视化技术,并对目标的身份进行掩盖,引导他们使用访谈形式描述网站的特定特征(Baptista,Derakshani,&Tressoldi,2015)。从与Ingo Swann在SRI开始作为ESP测试手段,到与Pat Price首次发表正式测试(Targ&Puthoff,19742005),该方法似乎非常成功;以至于当Utts(1996)被要求审查SRI和SAIC项目下积累的证据时,她断言:“使用适用于任何其他科学领域的标准,可以得出结论,心理功能已经得到了很好的确立”(第3页)。SRI的一些早期工作受到了批评(Marks&Kamman,1980),特别是在转录本的随机化和编辑方面的潜在问题,这可能会给位点作为靶标的顺序留下线索。Tart、Puthoff和Targ(1980)对这些担忧提出了质疑,他们证明,当线索被删除时,新的独立法官仍然能够在很大程度上将转录本与目标位点进行匹配。后来,成功的复制(例如。 因此,我们计划调查意识状态的主观变化是否与远程观看任务的更好表现有关,这是使用Pekala(1991)的意识量表现象学(PCI)测量的。最后,是di
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Journal of Parapsychology
Journal of Parapsychology Psychology-Psychology (miscellaneous)
CiteScore
1.30
自引率
0.00%
发文量
11
期刊介绍: The Journal of Parapsychology is published quarterly in March, June, September, and December by the Parapsychology Press, a subsidiary of the Foundation for Research on the Nature of Man. The Journal is devoted mainly to original reports of experimental research in parapsychology. It also publishes research reviews, theoretical and methodological articles that are closely linked to the empirical findings in the field, book reviews, news, comments, letters and abstracts.
期刊最新文献
SSE-PA Breakthrough, 2022: Abstracts of the Combined Conference of the Parapsychological Association and the Society for Scientific Exploration Near-Death Experiences and Psychological Wellbeing: A Quantitative Analysis Conference Abstracts: SSE/PA Connections, 2021 In Memoriam: Rex Stanford (1938–2022): A Personal Tribute to an Intellectual Giant in the Science of Parapsychology Book Reviews: A Splendid Adventure - J.B. Rhine. Letters 1923–1939: ESP and the Foundations of Parapsychology
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1