Individual Differences in Dealing With Classroom Noise Disturbances

IF 1.9 4区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Mind Brain and Education Pub Date : 2022-04-27 DOI:10.1111/mbe.12322
Jessica Massonnié, D. Mareschal, N. Kirkham
{"title":"Individual Differences in Dealing With Classroom Noise Disturbances","authors":"Jessica Massonnié, D. Mareschal, N. Kirkham","doi":"10.1111/mbe.12322","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Classrooms are noisy: when children are engaged in solo work, they also hear background babble, noise from outdoor, and people moving around. Few studies investigating the effects of noise on academic tasks use naturalistic stimuli. Questions also remain regarding why some children are more impaired by noise than others. This study compared primary school children’s performance at three academic tasks (text recall, reading comprehension, mathematics) in silence, and while hearing irrelevant verbal noise (storytelling, n = 33) or mixed noise (outdoor noise, movement, babble, n = 31). We found that noise does not impair overall performance. Children might use compensatory strategies (e.g., re-reading) to reach the same level of performance in silence and noise. Individual differences in selective attention and working memory were not related to the impact of noise, with one exception: children with lower working memory were more impaired by noise when doing mathematics. Replication on a larger sample is needed. Classrooms are full of life and full of sounds, generated by discussions, movements, objects, and events occurring outdoors (e.g., road traffic). As far as instruction is concerned, any sound that is not related to the current learning objectives and is unwanted, nonmeaningful, distracting, and/or unpleasant can be defined as a noise. This study investigated (1) to what extent noise impacts on children’s performance on academic tasks and (2) potential individual differences in children’s performance when working with background noise, compared to silence. 1Institute of Education, University College London 2Birkbeck, University of London Address correspondence to Jessica Massonnié, School of Education and Sociology, Faculty of Humanities & Social Sciences, University of Portsmouth, St George’s Building, 141 High Street, Portsmouth PO1 2HY, United Kingdom; e-mail: jessica.massonnie@port.ac.uk According to current theories, noise can impact task performance via three main mechanisms: (1) order processing, (2) phonological and/or semantic processing, and (3) attentional capture (Hughes, Vachon, & Jones, 2007; Klatte, Bergström, & Lachmann, 2013, a summary of previous studies is in Appendix). According to the order processing account, background noise composed of a series of distinct, successive sounds, is perceived as ordered and interferes with tasks involving order processing, such as serial recall. This interpretation is supported by laboratory experiments in which adults (Jones & Macken, 1993; Jones, Macken, & Murray, 1993) and children (Elliott, 2002; Elliott et al., 2016; Elliott & Briganti, 2012; Klatte, Lachmann, Schlittmeier, & Hellbrück, 2010; Klatte, Meis, Sukowski, & Schick, 2007) remember series of items (e.g., letter, words) in the presence of various distracting sounds (e.g., series of digits, words, tones). It is hard to generalize results to naturalistic noise stimuli that are not explicitly segmented (e.g., full utterances or conversations with overlapping sources of noise) and to tasks beyond serial recall. This lack of generalization reduces the educational and practical relevance of the findings. The phonological processing account suggests that noise interference occurs in working memory, a system allowing for the maintenance, storage, and manipulation of information. In working memory, the phonological loop stores and rehearses phonological representations that are presented visually (e.g., when reading words) and auditorily (e.g., when hearing speech; Baddeley, 2003). When visual and auditory representations are processed at the same time, they interfere with each other. This account explains the negative impact of background speech on serial recall, text recall (Boman, 2004), mathematics, reading, and spelling (Dockrell & Shield, 2006); all of which involve the processing of phonological information in working memory. As shown in adult experiments, having a better working memory reduces the impact of noise on serial recall (Sörqvist, 2010), text recall (Sörqvist, Ljungberg, & Ljung, 2010), and reading comprehension (Sörqvist, Halin, & Hygge, 2010). © 2022 The Authors. Mind, Brain, and Education published by International Mind, Brain, and Education Society and Wiley Periodicals LLC. 1 This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Dealing With Classroom Noise Noise is expected to be less detrimental to task performance when its phonological features are less salient. This is the case when multiple people talk at the same time or when conversations overlap with environmental noise. These types of noise do not impact on primary school students’ mathematics performance (Dockrell & Shield, 2006) or on middle school students’ reading (Slater, 1968) and mathematics (Ljung, Sörqvist, & Hygge, 2009) performance. Neely and LeCompte (1999) suggested that it was competing semantic, and not phonological, processing that explained the amount of interference between the noise and the task at hand. Importantly, both of the phonological and semantic expanations focus on the speech-like properties of the distracting sounds. Some evidence runs counter to the phonological and semantic accounts of noise interference: (1) Kassinove (1972) found no impact of verbal noise on mathematics performance in primary and middle school students, (2) classroom noise without speech impairs children’s ability to recall a text (Klatte et al., 2010), and (3) background conversations overlapping with environmental noise can have a positive impact on reading, spelling (Dockrell & Shield, 2006), reading comprehension (Connolly et al., 2019), and mathematics (Zentall & Shaw, 1980). This is where the attentional capture account comes into play. It posits that noise captures attention and, in doing so, distracts participants from their main task (Hughes et al., 2007). According to Klatte et al. (2013), “auditory events that are salient (e.g., of personal significance), unexpected (e.g., slamming of a door), or deviant from the recent auditory context (e.g., change in voice in a speech stream) have a strong potential to capture attention.” (p. 3). The attentional capture account explains why verbal and classroom noise without speech both have a negative impact on memory: by redirecting participants’ attention away from the information to be remembered, it can lead them to “miss out” some items. This theory can also explain why, paradoxically, some types of noise, such as a mix of background conversations and environmental noise, have a positive impact on reading and mathematics. This could be due to: (1) attention being redirected away, and then back to the main task, involving a re-focus of attention (Dockrell & Shield, 2006), (2) attentional disruption favoring abstract processing and conceptual association, as suggested in the creativity literature (Mehta, Zhu, & Cheema, 2012). It is possible that, for these positive effects to occur, the noise should not contain salient phonological information that interferes with working memory. Few experiments have directly measured working memory and attentional processes to test the phonological processing and attentional capture accounts. Studies investigating the role of working memory in noise interference have only involved adults (Sörqvist, 2010; Sörqvist, Halin, & Hygge, 2010; Sörqvist, Ljungberg, & Ljung, 2010). Developmental studies have indirectly tested the role of attention by showing that children (whose attentional skills are still developing) generally have a greater noise-related impediment than adults (Elliott, 2002; Elliott et al., 2016; Joseph, Hughes, Sörqvist, & Marsh, 2018; Klatte et al., 2010). Massonnié, Rogers, Mareschal, and Kirkham (2019) showed that primary school children with poor selective attention were particularly vulnerable to mixed noise when completing a divergent thinking task. This effect was driven by children who were in their early primary school years (from 5 up to 8 years of age). Older children, between 8 and 11 years of age, did not perform differently in silence and noise irrespective of their selective attention skills. More work needs to be done to understand why children may struggle with noise and if this is related to general attention mechanisms (Erickson & Newman, 2017). Furthermore, more studies are needed to specifically replicate the positive impact of hearing a mix of background conversations and environmental noise on academic tasks, and connect this impact to attentional mechanisms. Study Aims The present study investigated whether individual differences in working memory and selective attention relate to the impact of noise on academic tasks. It focuses on children in upper primary school (Key stage 2 in the United Kingdom), an age at which foundational literacy and numeracy skills are in place, the focus being on utilizing these skills in the context of elaboration, problem solving, and comprehension skills (Department for Education, 2013). The additional reflective components of this higher-level work may be particularly vulnerable to the distracting effects of noise. Three outcome measures were selected: reading comprehension, mathematics (two compulsory national subjects), and text recall (a testing method used in schools, and a more naturalistic measure of memory than serial recall). Two types of noise were selected to allow for comparison with the literature: (1) verbal noise (e.g., someone telling a story) and (2) a mix of overlapping conversations and background noise (henceforth called “mixed noise”). Verbal noise was predicted to have a negative impact on all three tasks, due to phonological interference. Because phonological interference is hypothesized to take place in working memory, lower working memory was expected to relate to a higher impact of verbal noise.","PeriodicalId":51595,"journal":{"name":"Mind Brain and Education","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9000,"publicationDate":"2022-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"3","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Mind Brain and Education","FirstCategoryId":"95","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12322","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3

Abstract

Classrooms are noisy: when children are engaged in solo work, they also hear background babble, noise from outdoor, and people moving around. Few studies investigating the effects of noise on academic tasks use naturalistic stimuli. Questions also remain regarding why some children are more impaired by noise than others. This study compared primary school children’s performance at three academic tasks (text recall, reading comprehension, mathematics) in silence, and while hearing irrelevant verbal noise (storytelling, n = 33) or mixed noise (outdoor noise, movement, babble, n = 31). We found that noise does not impair overall performance. Children might use compensatory strategies (e.g., re-reading) to reach the same level of performance in silence and noise. Individual differences in selective attention and working memory were not related to the impact of noise, with one exception: children with lower working memory were more impaired by noise when doing mathematics. Replication on a larger sample is needed. Classrooms are full of life and full of sounds, generated by discussions, movements, objects, and events occurring outdoors (e.g., road traffic). As far as instruction is concerned, any sound that is not related to the current learning objectives and is unwanted, nonmeaningful, distracting, and/or unpleasant can be defined as a noise. This study investigated (1) to what extent noise impacts on children’s performance on academic tasks and (2) potential individual differences in children’s performance when working with background noise, compared to silence. 1Institute of Education, University College London 2Birkbeck, University of London Address correspondence to Jessica Massonnié, School of Education and Sociology, Faculty of Humanities & Social Sciences, University of Portsmouth, St George’s Building, 141 High Street, Portsmouth PO1 2HY, United Kingdom; e-mail: jessica.massonnie@port.ac.uk According to current theories, noise can impact task performance via three main mechanisms: (1) order processing, (2) phonological and/or semantic processing, and (3) attentional capture (Hughes, Vachon, & Jones, 2007; Klatte, Bergström, & Lachmann, 2013, a summary of previous studies is in Appendix). According to the order processing account, background noise composed of a series of distinct, successive sounds, is perceived as ordered and interferes with tasks involving order processing, such as serial recall. This interpretation is supported by laboratory experiments in which adults (Jones & Macken, 1993; Jones, Macken, & Murray, 1993) and children (Elliott, 2002; Elliott et al., 2016; Elliott & Briganti, 2012; Klatte, Lachmann, Schlittmeier, & Hellbrück, 2010; Klatte, Meis, Sukowski, & Schick, 2007) remember series of items (e.g., letter, words) in the presence of various distracting sounds (e.g., series of digits, words, tones). It is hard to generalize results to naturalistic noise stimuli that are not explicitly segmented (e.g., full utterances or conversations with overlapping sources of noise) and to tasks beyond serial recall. This lack of generalization reduces the educational and practical relevance of the findings. The phonological processing account suggests that noise interference occurs in working memory, a system allowing for the maintenance, storage, and manipulation of information. In working memory, the phonological loop stores and rehearses phonological representations that are presented visually (e.g., when reading words) and auditorily (e.g., when hearing speech; Baddeley, 2003). When visual and auditory representations are processed at the same time, they interfere with each other. This account explains the negative impact of background speech on serial recall, text recall (Boman, 2004), mathematics, reading, and spelling (Dockrell & Shield, 2006); all of which involve the processing of phonological information in working memory. As shown in adult experiments, having a better working memory reduces the impact of noise on serial recall (Sörqvist, 2010), text recall (Sörqvist, Ljungberg, & Ljung, 2010), and reading comprehension (Sörqvist, Halin, & Hygge, 2010). © 2022 The Authors. Mind, Brain, and Education published by International Mind, Brain, and Education Society and Wiley Periodicals LLC. 1 This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Dealing With Classroom Noise Noise is expected to be less detrimental to task performance when its phonological features are less salient. This is the case when multiple people talk at the same time or when conversations overlap with environmental noise. These types of noise do not impact on primary school students’ mathematics performance (Dockrell & Shield, 2006) or on middle school students’ reading (Slater, 1968) and mathematics (Ljung, Sörqvist, & Hygge, 2009) performance. Neely and LeCompte (1999) suggested that it was competing semantic, and not phonological, processing that explained the amount of interference between the noise and the task at hand. Importantly, both of the phonological and semantic expanations focus on the speech-like properties of the distracting sounds. Some evidence runs counter to the phonological and semantic accounts of noise interference: (1) Kassinove (1972) found no impact of verbal noise on mathematics performance in primary and middle school students, (2) classroom noise without speech impairs children’s ability to recall a text (Klatte et al., 2010), and (3) background conversations overlapping with environmental noise can have a positive impact on reading, spelling (Dockrell & Shield, 2006), reading comprehension (Connolly et al., 2019), and mathematics (Zentall & Shaw, 1980). This is where the attentional capture account comes into play. It posits that noise captures attention and, in doing so, distracts participants from their main task (Hughes et al., 2007). According to Klatte et al. (2013), “auditory events that are salient (e.g., of personal significance), unexpected (e.g., slamming of a door), or deviant from the recent auditory context (e.g., change in voice in a speech stream) have a strong potential to capture attention.” (p. 3). The attentional capture account explains why verbal and classroom noise without speech both have a negative impact on memory: by redirecting participants’ attention away from the information to be remembered, it can lead them to “miss out” some items. This theory can also explain why, paradoxically, some types of noise, such as a mix of background conversations and environmental noise, have a positive impact on reading and mathematics. This could be due to: (1) attention being redirected away, and then back to the main task, involving a re-focus of attention (Dockrell & Shield, 2006), (2) attentional disruption favoring abstract processing and conceptual association, as suggested in the creativity literature (Mehta, Zhu, & Cheema, 2012). It is possible that, for these positive effects to occur, the noise should not contain salient phonological information that interferes with working memory. Few experiments have directly measured working memory and attentional processes to test the phonological processing and attentional capture accounts. Studies investigating the role of working memory in noise interference have only involved adults (Sörqvist, 2010; Sörqvist, Halin, & Hygge, 2010; Sörqvist, Ljungberg, & Ljung, 2010). Developmental studies have indirectly tested the role of attention by showing that children (whose attentional skills are still developing) generally have a greater noise-related impediment than adults (Elliott, 2002; Elliott et al., 2016; Joseph, Hughes, Sörqvist, & Marsh, 2018; Klatte et al., 2010). Massonnié, Rogers, Mareschal, and Kirkham (2019) showed that primary school children with poor selective attention were particularly vulnerable to mixed noise when completing a divergent thinking task. This effect was driven by children who were in their early primary school years (from 5 up to 8 years of age). Older children, between 8 and 11 years of age, did not perform differently in silence and noise irrespective of their selective attention skills. More work needs to be done to understand why children may struggle with noise and if this is related to general attention mechanisms (Erickson & Newman, 2017). Furthermore, more studies are needed to specifically replicate the positive impact of hearing a mix of background conversations and environmental noise on academic tasks, and connect this impact to attentional mechanisms. Study Aims The present study investigated whether individual differences in working memory and selective attention relate to the impact of noise on academic tasks. It focuses on children in upper primary school (Key stage 2 in the United Kingdom), an age at which foundational literacy and numeracy skills are in place, the focus being on utilizing these skills in the context of elaboration, problem solving, and comprehension skills (Department for Education, 2013). The additional reflective components of this higher-level work may be particularly vulnerable to the distracting effects of noise. Three outcome measures were selected: reading comprehension, mathematics (two compulsory national subjects), and text recall (a testing method used in schools, and a more naturalistic measure of memory than serial recall). Two types of noise were selected to allow for comparison with the literature: (1) verbal noise (e.g., someone telling a story) and (2) a mix of overlapping conversations and background noise (henceforth called “mixed noise”). Verbal noise was predicted to have a negative impact on all three tasks, due to phonological interference. Because phonological interference is hypothesized to take place in working memory, lower working memory was expected to relate to a higher impact of verbal noise.
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
处理课堂噪音干扰的个体差异
教室里很吵:当孩子们独自作业时,他们也会听到背景的咿呀学语、室外的噪音和人们走动的声音。很少有研究使用自然刺激来调查噪音对学业任务的影响。关于为什么有些孩子比其他孩子更容易受到噪音的伤害,问题仍然存在。这项研究比较了小学生在沉默、听不相关的语言噪音(讲故事,n = 33)或混合噪音(户外噪音,运动,咿呀学语,n = 31)的情况下在三项学术任务(课文回忆,阅读理解,数学)中的表现。我们发现噪音不会影响整体性能。儿童可能会使用补偿策略(例如,重新阅读)来达到在安静和噪音中相同的表现水平。选择性注意和工作记忆的个体差异与噪音的影响无关,但有一个例外:工作记忆较差的儿童在做数学时更容易受到噪音的损害。需要在更大的样本上进行复制。教室充满了生命,充满了声音,这些声音是由讨论、运动、物体和发生在户外的事件(例如道路交通)产生的。就教学而言,任何与当前学习目标无关的、不需要的、无意义的、分散注意力的和/或不愉快的声音都可以被定义为噪音。本研究调查了(1)噪音对儿童学业表现的影响程度;(2)与静音相比,背景噪音对儿童学业表现的潜在个体差异。寄给朴茨茅斯大学人文与社会科学学院教育与社会学学院Jessica massonni<e:1>,英国朴茨茅斯PO1 2HY高街141号圣乔治大厦;根据目前的理论,噪音可以通过三个主要机制影响任务绩效:(1)顺序处理,(2)语音和/或语义处理,(3)注意力捕获(Hughes, Vachon, & Jones, 2007;Klatte, Bergström, & Lachmann, 2013,对以往研究的总结见附录)。根据顺序处理理论,背景噪音由一系列不同的、连续的声音组成,被认为是有序的,并干扰涉及顺序处理的任务,如连续回忆。这种解释得到了实验室实验的支持,在这些实验中,成年人(Jones & Macken, 1993;Jones, Macken, & Murray, 1993)和儿童(Elliott, 2002;Elliott et al., 2016;Elliott & Briganti, 2012;Klatte, Lachmann, Schlittmeier, & hellbr<e:1>, 2010;Klatte, Meis, Sukowski, & Schick, 2007)在各种分散注意力的声音(例如,一系列数字,单词,音调)存在的情况下记住一系列项目(例如,字母,单词)。很难将结果推广到没有明确分割的自然噪声刺激(例如,完整的话语或具有重叠噪声源的对话)和超出序列回忆的任务。这种泛化的缺乏降低了研究结果的教育和实际意义。语音加工理论表明,噪声干扰发生在工作记忆中,而工作记忆是一个维护、存储和操作信息的系统。在工作记忆中,语音回路储存和排练视觉上(如阅读单词时)和听觉上(如听演讲时;·巴德利,2003)。当视觉和听觉同时被处理时,它们会相互干扰。这一解释解释了背景言语对序列回忆、文本回忆(Boman, 2004)、数学、阅读和拼写(Dockrell & Shield, 2006)的负面影响;所有这些都涉及到工作记忆中语音信息的加工。如成人实验所示,拥有较好的工作记忆可以降低噪声对序列回忆(Sörqvist, 2010)、文本回忆(Sörqvist, Ljungberg, & Ljung, 2010)和阅读理解(Sörqvist, Halin, & Hygge, 2010)的影响。©2022作者。《心智、大脑与教育》由国际心智、大脑与教育协会和威利期刊有限责任公司出版。这是一篇基于知识共享署名许可的开放获取文章,该许可允许在任何媒体上使用、分发和复制,前提是原始作品被适当引用。当语音特征不太突出时,噪音对任务表现的危害就会小一些。当很多人同时说话,或者谈话与环境噪音重叠时,就会出现这种情况。这些类型的噪音不会影响小学生的数学成绩(Dockrell & Shield, 2006),也不会影响中学生的阅读(Slater, 1968)和数学(Ljung, Sörqvist, & Hygge, 2009)成绩。 Neely和LeCompte(1999)认为是语义上的竞争,而不是语音上的竞争,解释了噪音和手头任务之间的干扰程度。重要的是,语音和语义的扩展都集中在分心声音的语音特性上。一些证据与噪音干扰的音韵学和语义学解释背道而驰:(1) Kassinove(1972)发现语言噪音对中小学生的数学成绩没有影响;(2)没有言语的课堂噪音会损害儿童回忆课文的能力(Klatte等,2010);(3)背景对话与环境噪音重叠对阅读、拼写(Dockrell & Shield, 2006)、阅读理解(Connolly等,2019)和数学(Zentall & Shaw, 1980)有积极影响。这就是注意力捕捉理论发挥作用的地方。它假设噪音会吸引注意力,并在此过程中分散参与者的主要任务(Hughes et al., 2007)。根据Klatte等人(2013)的说法,“显著的(例如,对个人有重要意义的)、意外的(例如,摔门)或偏离最近的听觉环境的听觉事件(例如,语音流中的声音变化)具有很强的吸引注意力的潜力。(第3页)注意力捕获说解释了为什么口头和教室里没有说话的噪音都会对记忆产生负面影响:通过将参与者的注意力从要记住的信息上转移开,这会导致他们“错过”一些项目。这一理论也可以解释为什么某些类型的噪音,如背景谈话和环境噪音的混合,对阅读和数学有积极的影响,这是自相矛盾的。这可能是由于:(1)注意力被重新转移,然后回到主要任务,涉及到注意力的重新集中(Dockrell和Shield, 2006);(2)注意力中断有利于抽象处理和概念联想,正如创造力文献所建议的那样(Mehta, Zhu, & Cheema, 2012)。有可能的是,为了产生这些积极的影响,噪音不应该包含干扰工作记忆的显著语音信息。很少有实验直接测量工作记忆和注意过程来测试语音加工和注意捕获的说法。调查工作记忆在噪音干扰中的作用的研究只涉及成年人(Sörqvist, 2010;Sörqvist, Halin, & Hygge, 2010;Sörqvist, Ljungberg, & Ljung, 2010)。发展研究间接测试了注意力的作用,表明儿童(其注意力技能仍在发展中)通常比成人有更大的噪音相关障碍(Elliott, 2002;Elliott et al., 2016;约瑟夫,休斯,Sörqvist, & Marsh, 2018;Klatte et al., 2010)。massonni<e:1>, Rogers, Mareschal, and Kirkham(2019)表明,选择性注意力差的小学生在完成发散性思维任务时特别容易受到混合噪音的影响。这种影响是由小学早期阶段的儿童(从5岁到8岁)造成的。8到11岁的大一点的孩子,不管他们的选择性注意力能力如何,在安静和噪音环境下的表现并没有什么不同。需要做更多的工作来理解为什么儿童可能会与噪音作斗争,以及这是否与一般的注意力机制有关(Erickson & Newman, 2017)。此外,需要更多的研究来具体地复制听到背景对话和环境噪音混合对学术任务的积极影响,并将这种影响与注意力机制联系起来。本研究旨在探讨工作记忆和选择性注意的个体差异是否与噪声对学业任务的影响有关。它侧重于小学高年级(英国的关键阶段2)的儿童,这个年龄段的儿童具备基本的识字和算术技能,重点是在阐述、解决问题和理解技能的背景下利用这些技能(教育部,2013年)。这种高水平工作的额外反射成分可能特别容易受到噪音的干扰。选择了三个结果测量方法:阅读理解、数学(两门必修的国家科目)和文本回忆(一种在学校使用的测试方法,比连续回忆更自然地测量记忆)。我们选择了两种类型的噪音来与文献进行比较:(1)口头噪音(例如,有人讲故事)和(2)重叠对话和背景噪音的混合(从此称为“混合噪音”)。由于语音干扰,言语噪音预计会对所有三项任务产生负面影响。 由于语音干扰被假设发生在工作记忆中,较低的工作记忆被认为与言语噪音的较高影响有关。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
3.50
自引率
11.10%
发文量
29
期刊介绍: Mind, Brain, and Education (MBE), recognized as the 2007 Best New Journal in the Social Sciences & Humanities by the Association of American Publishers" Professional & Scholarly Publishing Division, provides a forum for the accessible presentation of basic and applied research on learning and development, including analyses from biology, cognitive science, and education. The journal grew out of the International Mind, Brain, and Education Society"s mission to create a new field of mind, brain and education, with educators and researchers expertly collaborating in integrating the variety of fields connecting mind, brain, and education in research, theory, and/or practice.
期刊最新文献
Need for Cognition, Neuromyths, and Knowledge about the Brain in Aspiring Teachers Sibling Effects on School Achievement: Evidence From Two Large French Cohorts Evidence, Policy, Education, and Neuroscience—The State of Play in the UK Underutilized Techniques and Underrepresented Samples in Educational Neuroscience Research: An Introduction to the Special Issue Fixation Disparity: A Possible Index of Visuospatial Cognition during Authentic Learning Tasks
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1