Introduction to Special Thematic Issue, part 2 "Microsaccades: Empirical Research and Methodological Advances".

IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Journal of Eye Movement Research Pub Date : 2023-03-25 eCollection Date: 2020-01-01 DOI:10.16910/jemr.13.5.1
Rudolf Groner
{"title":"Introduction to Special Thematic Issue, part 2 \"Microsaccades: Empirical Research and Methodological Advances\".","authors":"Rudolf Groner","doi":"10.16910/jemr.13.5.1","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Microsaccades are at the interface between basic oculomotor phenomena and complex processes of cognitive functioning, and they also have been a challenge for subtle experimentation and adequate statistical analysis. In the second part of the special thematic issue (for the first part see  4) the authors present a series of articles which demonstrate that microsaccades are still an interesting and rewarding area of scientific research the forefront of research in many areas of sensory, perceptual, and cognitive processes.. In their article \"Pupillary and microsaccadic responses to cognitive effort and emotional arousal during complex decision making\" Krejtz, Żurawska, Duchowski, & Wichary (1) investigate pupillary and microsaccadic responses to information processing during multi-attribute decision making under affective priming. The participants were randomly assigned into three affective priming conditions (neutral, aversive, and erotic) and instructed to make discriminative decisions. As hypothesized by the authors, the results showed microsaccadic rate inhibition and pupillary dilation, depending on cognitive effort prior to decision and moderated by affective priming. Aversive priming increased pupillary and microsaccadic responses to information processing effort. The results indicate that pupillary response is more influenced by affective priming than microsaccadic rate. The results are discussed in the light of neuropsychological mechanisms of pupillary and microsaccadic behavior. In the article \"Microsaccadic rate signatures correlate under monocular and binocular stimulation conditions\" Essig, Leube, Rifai, & Wahl (2020) investigate microsaccades with respect to their directional distribution and rate under monocular and binocular conditions. In both stimulation conditions participants fixated a Gabor patch presented randomly in orientation of 45° or 135° over a wide range of spatial frequencies. Microsaccades were mostly horizontally oriented regardless of the spatial frequency of the grating. This outcome was consistent between both stimulation conditions. This study found that the microsaccadic rate signature curve correlates between both stimulation conditions, therefore extending the use of microsaccades to clinical applications, since parameters as contrast sensitivity, have frequently been measured monocularly in the clinical studies. The study \"Microsaccades during high speed continuous visual search\" by Martin, Davis, Riesenhuber, & Thorpe (3) provides an analysis of the microsaccades occurring during visual search, targeting to small faces pasted either into cluttered background photos or into a simple gray background.  Participants were instructed to target singular 3-degree upright or inverted faces in changing scenes.  As soon as the participant's gaze reached the target face, a new face was displayed in a different random location.  Regardless of the experimental context (e.g. background scene, no background scene), or target eccentricity (from 4 to 20 degrees of visual angle), The authors found that the microsaccade rate dropped to near zero levels within 12 milliseconds.  There were almost never any microsaccades after stimulus onset and before the first saccade to the face. In about 20% of the trials, there was a single microsaccade that occurred almost immediately after the preceding saccade's offset.  The authors argue that a single feedforward pass through the visual hierarchy of processing a stimulus is needed to effectuate prolonged continuous visual search and provide evidence that microsaccades can serve perceptual functions like correcting saccades or effectuating task-oriented goals during continuous visual search. While many studies have characterized the eye movements during visual fixation, including microsaccades, in most cases only horizontal and vertical components have been recorded and analyzed. Little is known about the torsional component of microsaccades. In the study \"Torsional component of microsaccades during fixation and quick phases during optokinetic stimulation\" Sadeghpour & Otero-Millan (5) recorded eye movements around the three axes of rotation during fixation and torsional optokinetic stimulus. The authors found that the average amplitude of the torsional component of microsaccades during fixation was 0.34 ± 0.07 degrees with velocities following a main sequence with a slope comparable to the horizontal and vertical components. The size of the torsional displacement during microsaccades was correlated with the horizontal but not the vertical component. In the presence of an optokinetic stimulus a nystagmus was induced producing  more frequent and larger torsional quick phases compared to microsaccades produced during fixation of a stationary stimulus. The torsional component and the vertical vergence component of quick phases increased with higher velocities. In previous research, microsaccades have been interpreted as psychophysiological indicators of task load. So far, it is still under debate how different types of task demands are influencing microsaccade rate. In their article \"The interplay between task difficulty and microsaccade rate: Evidence for the critical role of visual load\" Schneider et al. (6) examined the relation between visual load, mental load and microsaccade rate. The participants carried out a continuous performance task (n-back) in which visual task load (letters vs. abstract figures) and mental task load (1-back to 4-back) were manipulated as within-subjects variables. Eye tracking data, performance data as well as subjective workload were recorded. Data analysis revealed an increased level of microsaccade rate for stimuli of high visual demand (i.e. abstract figures), while mental demand (n-back-level) did not modulate microsaccade rate. The authors concluded that microsaccade rate reflects visual load of a task rather than its mental load. This conclusion is in accordance with the proposition of Krueger et al. (2) \"Microsaccades distinguish looking from seeing\", linking sensory with cognitive phenomena. The present special thematic issue adds several new interesting facets to the research landscape around microsaccades. They still remain an attractive focus of interdisciplinary research and transdisciplinary applications. Thus, as already noted in the first part of this special thematic issue, research on microsaccades will not only endure, but keep evolving as the knowledge base expands.</p>","PeriodicalId":15813,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Eye Movement Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3000,"publicationDate":"2023-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11185420/pdf/","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Eye Movement Research","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.5.1","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"2020/1/1 0:00:00","PubModel":"eCollection","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"OPHTHALMOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Microsaccades are at the interface between basic oculomotor phenomena and complex processes of cognitive functioning, and they also have been a challenge for subtle experimentation and adequate statistical analysis. In the second part of the special thematic issue (for the first part see  4) the authors present a series of articles which demonstrate that microsaccades are still an interesting and rewarding area of scientific research the forefront of research in many areas of sensory, perceptual, and cognitive processes.. In their article "Pupillary and microsaccadic responses to cognitive effort and emotional arousal during complex decision making" Krejtz, Żurawska, Duchowski, & Wichary (1) investigate pupillary and microsaccadic responses to information processing during multi-attribute decision making under affective priming. The participants were randomly assigned into three affective priming conditions (neutral, aversive, and erotic) and instructed to make discriminative decisions. As hypothesized by the authors, the results showed microsaccadic rate inhibition and pupillary dilation, depending on cognitive effort prior to decision and moderated by affective priming. Aversive priming increased pupillary and microsaccadic responses to information processing effort. The results indicate that pupillary response is more influenced by affective priming than microsaccadic rate. The results are discussed in the light of neuropsychological mechanisms of pupillary and microsaccadic behavior. In the article "Microsaccadic rate signatures correlate under monocular and binocular stimulation conditions" Essig, Leube, Rifai, & Wahl (2020) investigate microsaccades with respect to their directional distribution and rate under monocular and binocular conditions. In both stimulation conditions participants fixated a Gabor patch presented randomly in orientation of 45° or 135° over a wide range of spatial frequencies. Microsaccades were mostly horizontally oriented regardless of the spatial frequency of the grating. This outcome was consistent between both stimulation conditions. This study found that the microsaccadic rate signature curve correlates between both stimulation conditions, therefore extending the use of microsaccades to clinical applications, since parameters as contrast sensitivity, have frequently been measured monocularly in the clinical studies. The study "Microsaccades during high speed continuous visual search" by Martin, Davis, Riesenhuber, & Thorpe (3) provides an analysis of the microsaccades occurring during visual search, targeting to small faces pasted either into cluttered background photos or into a simple gray background.  Participants were instructed to target singular 3-degree upright or inverted faces in changing scenes.  As soon as the participant's gaze reached the target face, a new face was displayed in a different random location.  Regardless of the experimental context (e.g. background scene, no background scene), or target eccentricity (from 4 to 20 degrees of visual angle), The authors found that the microsaccade rate dropped to near zero levels within 12 milliseconds.  There were almost never any microsaccades after stimulus onset and before the first saccade to the face. In about 20% of the trials, there was a single microsaccade that occurred almost immediately after the preceding saccade's offset.  The authors argue that a single feedforward pass through the visual hierarchy of processing a stimulus is needed to effectuate prolonged continuous visual search and provide evidence that microsaccades can serve perceptual functions like correcting saccades or effectuating task-oriented goals during continuous visual search. While many studies have characterized the eye movements during visual fixation, including microsaccades, in most cases only horizontal and vertical components have been recorded and analyzed. Little is known about the torsional component of microsaccades. In the study "Torsional component of microsaccades during fixation and quick phases during optokinetic stimulation" Sadeghpour & Otero-Millan (5) recorded eye movements around the three axes of rotation during fixation and torsional optokinetic stimulus. The authors found that the average amplitude of the torsional component of microsaccades during fixation was 0.34 ± 0.07 degrees with velocities following a main sequence with a slope comparable to the horizontal and vertical components. The size of the torsional displacement during microsaccades was correlated with the horizontal but not the vertical component. In the presence of an optokinetic stimulus a nystagmus was induced producing  more frequent and larger torsional quick phases compared to microsaccades produced during fixation of a stationary stimulus. The torsional component and the vertical vergence component of quick phases increased with higher velocities. In previous research, microsaccades have been interpreted as psychophysiological indicators of task load. So far, it is still under debate how different types of task demands are influencing microsaccade rate. In their article "The interplay between task difficulty and microsaccade rate: Evidence for the critical role of visual load" Schneider et al. (6) examined the relation between visual load, mental load and microsaccade rate. The participants carried out a continuous performance task (n-back) in which visual task load (letters vs. abstract figures) and mental task load (1-back to 4-back) were manipulated as within-subjects variables. Eye tracking data, performance data as well as subjective workload were recorded. Data analysis revealed an increased level of microsaccade rate for stimuli of high visual demand (i.e. abstract figures), while mental demand (n-back-level) did not modulate microsaccade rate. The authors concluded that microsaccade rate reflects visual load of a task rather than its mental load. This conclusion is in accordance with the proposition of Krueger et al. (2) "Microsaccades distinguish looking from seeing", linking sensory with cognitive phenomena. The present special thematic issue adds several new interesting facets to the research landscape around microsaccades. They still remain an attractive focus of interdisciplinary research and transdisciplinary applications. Thus, as already noted in the first part of this special thematic issue, research on microsaccades will not only endure, but keep evolving as the knowledge base expands.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
专题特刊第2部分“微观发展:实证研究与方法论进展”导言
微扫视处于基本的动眼力现象和复杂的认知功能过程之间的界面,也是精细实验和充分统计分析的挑战。在专题问题的第二部分(第一部分见Martinez-Conde, Engbert, & Groner, 2020),作者提出了一系列文章,这些文章表明微跳仍然是一个有趣且有益的科学研究领域,是感官,知觉和认知过程的许多领域的研究前沿。Krejtz, Żurawska, Duchowski, & Wichary(2020)在他们的文章“复杂决策过程中对认知努力和情绪唤醒的瞳孔和微眼动反应”中研究了情感启动下多属性决策过程中瞳孔和微眼动对信息处理的反应。参与者被随机分配到三种情感启动条件下(中性、厌恶和色情),并被指示做出歧视性的决定。正如作者假设的那样,结果显示微眼球速率抑制和瞳孔扩张取决于决策前的认知努力,并受到情感启动的调节。厌恶启动增加了瞳孔和微眼球对信息处理努力的反应。结果表明,情感启动对瞳孔反应的影响大于微跳率。从瞳孔和微眼球行为的神经心理学机制角度讨论了这些结果。Essig, Leube, Rifai, & Wahl(2020)在文章“微跳频特征在单眼和双眼刺激条件下相关”中研究了微跳频在单眼和双眼条件下的方向分布和速率。在这两种刺激条件下,参与者都盯着一个Gabor贴片,该贴片在广泛的空间频率范围内以45°或135°的方向随机呈现。无论光栅的空间频率如何,微跳波大多是水平取向的。这一结果在两种刺激条件下是一致的。本研究发现,微跳率特征曲线在两种刺激条件之间存在相关性,因此将微跳的使用扩展到临床应用,因为在临床研究中,作为对比灵敏度的参数经常是单目测量的。Martin, Davis, Riesenhuber, & Thorpe(2020)的研究“高速连续视觉搜索期间的微眼跳”分析了视觉搜索期间发生的微眼跳,目标是粘贴在杂乱背景照片或简单灰色背景中的小面孔。参与者被要求在不断变化的场景中瞄准单一的3度直立或倒置的脸。一旦参与者的目光到达目标脸,一张新脸就会在不同的随机位置显示出来。无论实验环境如何(例如背景场景,没有背景场景),或目标偏心(从4到20度的视角),作者发现微跳率在12毫秒内下降到接近零的水平。在刺激开始后和第一次扫视到面部之前,几乎没有任何微扫视。在大约20%的试验中,在前一次眼跳偏移后几乎立即出现了一次微眼跳。作者认为,需要通过处理刺激的视觉层次的单一前馈传递来实现长时间的连续视觉搜索,并提供了微扫视在连续视觉搜索过程中具有纠正扫视或实现任务导向目标等感知功能的证据。虽然许多研究描述了眼球在注视过程中的运动,包括微跳,但在大多数情况下,只记录和分析了水平和垂直分量。人们对微跃动的扭转成分知之甚少。Sadeghpour & Otero-Millan(2020)在“注视和光动力刺激下的快速阶段微跳的扭转成分”研究中记录了在注视和扭转光动力刺激期间围绕三个旋转轴的眼球运动。作者发现,固定期间微眼跳扭转分量的平均振幅为0.34±0.07度,速度遵循主序列,斜率与水平和垂直分量相当。微跳时的扭转位移大小与水平分量相关,而与垂直分量无关。与固定刺激时产生的微跳相比,在光动力刺激下眼球震颤产生更频繁和更大的扭转快速相。快速相位的扭转分量和垂直辐合分量随速度的增加而增大。 在以往的研究中,微跳被解释为任务负荷的心理生理指标。到目前为止,不同类型的任务需求是如何影响微扫视率的还存在争议。Schneider et al.(1921)在他们的文章《任务难度和微跳率之间的相互作用:视觉负荷关键作用的证据》中研究了视觉负荷、心理负荷和微跳率之间的关系。参与者进行了一个连续的表现任务(n-back),其中视觉任务负荷(字母与抽象图形)和心理任务负荷(1-back到4-back)被操纵为受试者内部变量。记录眼球追踪数据、工作表现数据以及主观工作量。数据分析表明,高视觉需求刺激(如抽象图形)会增加微跳速,而心理需求(n-back水平)对微跳速没有调节作用。作者得出结论,微扫视率反映的是一项任务的视觉负荷,而不是精神负荷。这一结论与Krueger等人(2019)的命题“Microsaccades区分了看和看”(Microsaccades区分了感觉和认知现象)相一致。当前的专题专题为围绕微跳跃的研究领域增加了几个新的有趣的方面。它们仍然是跨学科研究和跨学科应用的一个有吸引力的焦点。因此,正如本专题第一部分已经指出的那样,对微跳的研究不仅会持续下去,而且会随着知识库的扩展而不断发展。参考文献Krejtz, K., Żurawska, J., Duchowski, A.和Wichary, S.(2020)。复杂决策过程中对认知努力和情绪唤醒的瞳孔和微眼球反应。眼动研究,13(5)。https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.5.2 Krueger, E., Schneider, A., Sawyer, B., Chavaillaz, A., Sonderegger, A., Groner, R., & Hancock, P.(2019)。微眼跳区分了看和看。眼动研究,12(6)。https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.6.2 Martin, j.g., Davis, c.e., Riesenhuber, M., & Thorpe, s.j.(2020)。高速连续视觉搜索时的微跳动。眼动研究,13(5)。https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.5.4 Martinez-Conde, S., Engbert, R., & Groner, R.(2020)。微观发展:实证研究与方法论进展——专题特刊第一部分导言。眼动研究,12(6)。https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.6.1 Sadeghpour, S., & Otero-Millan, J.(2020)。光动力刺激下固定和快速阶段微眼跳的扭转成分。眼动研究,13(5)。https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.5.5 Schneider, A., Sonderegger, A., Krueger, E., Meteier, Q., Luethold, P., & Chavaillaz, A.(2021)。任务难度与微跳率之间的相互作用:视觉负荷关键作用的证据。眼动研究,13(5)。https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.5.6
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
2.90
自引率
33.30%
发文量
10
审稿时长
10 weeks
期刊介绍: The Journal of Eye Movement Research is an open-access, peer-reviewed scientific periodical devoted to all aspects of oculomotor functioning including methodology of eye recording, neurophysiological and cognitive models, attention, reading, as well as applications in neurology, ergonomy, media research and other areas,
期刊最新文献
The level of skills involved in an observation-based gait analysis. Persistence of primitive reflexes associated with asymmetries in fixation and ocular motility values. Influence of complexity and Gestalt principles on aesthetic preferences for building façades: An eye tracking study Classification framework to identify similar visual scan paths using multiple similarity metrics Gender selection dilemma in FMCG advertising: Insights from eye-tracking research
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1