Commentary on Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick, Implicit Bias ≠ Bias on Implicit Measures

IF 7.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Psychological Inquiry Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI:10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106761
M. Olson, L. Gill
{"title":"Commentary on Gawronski, Ledgerwood, and Eastwick, Implicit Bias ≠ Bias on Implicit Measures","authors":"M. Olson, L. Gill","doi":"10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106761","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The authors of the target article offer a definition of implicit bias as “unconscious effects of social category cues” (Gawronski, Ledgerwood, & Eastwick, this issue, p. 140), and, so defined, make a case for examining its causes, effects, and possible amelioration. We support this pursuit and offer some suggestions on how that might be accomplished. For years we have argued that however implicit bias might be defined, to equate it to the output of a measure that one happens to also call “implicit” or is a bad idea (e.g., Fazio & Olson, 2003; Olson & Fazio, 2009; Olson & Gill, 2022; Olson & Zabel, 2016). Indeed, we wrote nearly twenty years ago, “We would encourage researchers not to equate an implicitly measured construct with an unconscious one” (Fazio & Olson, 2003, p. 303). Since then, evidence has accumulated that bias on implicit measures is not implicit in the sense of being inaccessible to consciousness. For example, in 2007 we showed that implicitly measured self-esteem was consciously accessible and hence reportable on explicit measures when respondents were implored to be honest (Olson, Fazio, & Hermann, 2007). Similarly, implicitly-assessed antiBlack bias correlates with explicitly-assessed anti-Black bias under conditions of honesty and anonymity (Phillips & Olson, 2014; see also Hahn, Judd, Hirsh, & Blair, 2014). Nevertheless, we see the need for the authors’ treatise on the problems of conflating implicit bias with bias on an implicit measure, as prominent researchers in these domains persist in equating the two (Greenwald et al., 2022). However, and despite social scientists’ proliferation of near-synonyms, we also want to make a case that a focus on automaticity over implicitness with regard to implicit measures (and, as we will see, implicit bias) has a strong theoretical foundation and empirical support. Before the term “implicit” was popularized and applied to prejudice or attitude measurement, Fazio and colleagues (e.g., Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986) were investigating the automatic activation of attitudes. The evaluative priming measure they developed is probably the second most-used implicit measure, after the IAT. On a given trial in a priming task, a prime (the attitude object, usually in image form) is presented briefly, followed by a clearly valenced target adjective (e.g., wonderful) participants are tasked with identifying as either good or bad by pressing one of two corresponding keys as quickly as possible. This seminal work found that for particularly strong attitudes, primes facilitate the identification of valence-congruent target adjectives: cake primes facilitated identification of positive targets, and death primes facilitated identification of negative targets (inhibition of valence incongruent prime-target pairs was also observed). This facilitation effect is automatic because the activation of participants’ attitudes occurred despite their goal to identify the valence of the targets, not the primes; it was thus unintentional but also efficient insofar as it happened very quickly. Fazio and colleagues replicated this essential effect with respect to racial prejudice in a later paper (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995). A few years later, Greenwald and colleagues published their first article on the IAT (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). Much has been written about qualities of automaticity as applied to the IAT, but our sense is that IAT effects are not so much about the automatic activation of attitudes themselves as much as the controllability of those attitudes with regards to the forced categorization of two objects whose valence differ (e.g., for many Americans, Black and positive) but yet are assigned to the same response key. Nevertheless, and despite our suspicion that they tap somewhat different aspects of automaticity, priming and the IAT correlate substantially under specific conditions (see Olson & Zabel, 2016, for a review). Phillips and Olson (2014) also demonstrated that race bias as revealed on the IAT is experienced as spontaneously activated affect. Thus, at least with respect to the most commonly employed of them, implicit measures appear to reveal automatic properties of attitudes, which is why we have an affinity for the term “automatic prejudice” or, in keeping with fashion, “automatic bias.” Evidence of automatic activation of attitudes stimulated theoretical work that culminated in the MODE model of attitude-behavior relations, which articulates when and how attitudes with automatic properties exert impact—including impact about which people might be unaware—on judgments and behavior (Fazio, 1990; Fazio & Olson, 2014). According to the MODE model, and as demonstrated by researching employing the priming measure, attitudes of sufficient strength are likely to be automatically-activate upon perception of the attitude object (e.g., a member of a minoritized group). Once activated, such attitudes set in motion a cascade of processes that can culminate in a spontaneous attitudebehavior relation: an individual who harbors anti-Black bias","PeriodicalId":48327,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Inquiry","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.2000,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Psychological Inquiry","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106761","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

The authors of the target article offer a definition of implicit bias as “unconscious effects of social category cues” (Gawronski, Ledgerwood, & Eastwick, this issue, p. 140), and, so defined, make a case for examining its causes, effects, and possible amelioration. We support this pursuit and offer some suggestions on how that might be accomplished. For years we have argued that however implicit bias might be defined, to equate it to the output of a measure that one happens to also call “implicit” or is a bad idea (e.g., Fazio & Olson, 2003; Olson & Fazio, 2009; Olson & Gill, 2022; Olson & Zabel, 2016). Indeed, we wrote nearly twenty years ago, “We would encourage researchers not to equate an implicitly measured construct with an unconscious one” (Fazio & Olson, 2003, p. 303). Since then, evidence has accumulated that bias on implicit measures is not implicit in the sense of being inaccessible to consciousness. For example, in 2007 we showed that implicitly measured self-esteem was consciously accessible and hence reportable on explicit measures when respondents were implored to be honest (Olson, Fazio, & Hermann, 2007). Similarly, implicitly-assessed antiBlack bias correlates with explicitly-assessed anti-Black bias under conditions of honesty and anonymity (Phillips & Olson, 2014; see also Hahn, Judd, Hirsh, & Blair, 2014). Nevertheless, we see the need for the authors’ treatise on the problems of conflating implicit bias with bias on an implicit measure, as prominent researchers in these domains persist in equating the two (Greenwald et al., 2022). However, and despite social scientists’ proliferation of near-synonyms, we also want to make a case that a focus on automaticity over implicitness with regard to implicit measures (and, as we will see, implicit bias) has a strong theoretical foundation and empirical support. Before the term “implicit” was popularized and applied to prejudice or attitude measurement, Fazio and colleagues (e.g., Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986) were investigating the automatic activation of attitudes. The evaluative priming measure they developed is probably the second most-used implicit measure, after the IAT. On a given trial in a priming task, a prime (the attitude object, usually in image form) is presented briefly, followed by a clearly valenced target adjective (e.g., wonderful) participants are tasked with identifying as either good or bad by pressing one of two corresponding keys as quickly as possible. This seminal work found that for particularly strong attitudes, primes facilitate the identification of valence-congruent target adjectives: cake primes facilitated identification of positive targets, and death primes facilitated identification of negative targets (inhibition of valence incongruent prime-target pairs was also observed). This facilitation effect is automatic because the activation of participants’ attitudes occurred despite their goal to identify the valence of the targets, not the primes; it was thus unintentional but also efficient insofar as it happened very quickly. Fazio and colleagues replicated this essential effect with respect to racial prejudice in a later paper (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995). A few years later, Greenwald and colleagues published their first article on the IAT (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). Much has been written about qualities of automaticity as applied to the IAT, but our sense is that IAT effects are not so much about the automatic activation of attitudes themselves as much as the controllability of those attitudes with regards to the forced categorization of two objects whose valence differ (e.g., for many Americans, Black and positive) but yet are assigned to the same response key. Nevertheless, and despite our suspicion that they tap somewhat different aspects of automaticity, priming and the IAT correlate substantially under specific conditions (see Olson & Zabel, 2016, for a review). Phillips and Olson (2014) also demonstrated that race bias as revealed on the IAT is experienced as spontaneously activated affect. Thus, at least with respect to the most commonly employed of them, implicit measures appear to reveal automatic properties of attitudes, which is why we have an affinity for the term “automatic prejudice” or, in keeping with fashion, “automatic bias.” Evidence of automatic activation of attitudes stimulated theoretical work that culminated in the MODE model of attitude-behavior relations, which articulates when and how attitudes with automatic properties exert impact—including impact about which people might be unaware—on judgments and behavior (Fazio, 1990; Fazio & Olson, 2014). According to the MODE model, and as demonstrated by researching employing the priming measure, attitudes of sufficient strength are likely to be automatically-activate upon perception of the attitude object (e.g., a member of a minoritized group). Once activated, such attitudes set in motion a cascade of processes that can culminate in a spontaneous attitudebehavior relation: an individual who harbors anti-Black bias
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Gawronski、Ledgerwood和Eastwick的评论,隐性偏见 ≠ 隐性测量的偏差
这种态度一旦被激活,就会引发一连串的过程,最终形成一种自发的态度-行为关系:一个怀有反黑人偏见的人
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Psychological Inquiry
Psychological Inquiry PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY-
CiteScore
10.30
自引率
1.10%
发文量
31
期刊介绍: Psychological Inquiry serves as an international journal dedicated to the advancement of psychological theory. Each edition features an extensive target article exploring a controversial or provocative topic, accompanied by peer commentaries and a response from the target author(s). Proposals for target articles must be submitted using the Target Article Proposal Form, and only approved proposals undergo peer review by at least three reviewers. Authors are invited to submit their full articles after the proposal has received approval from the Editor.
期刊最新文献
How Prevalent is Social Projection? The Future of Social Perception Models: Further Directions for Theoretical Development of the Inductive Reasoning Model Social Projection and Cognitive Differentiation Co-Explain Self-Enhancement and in-Group Favoritism Three Pokes into the Comfort Zone of the Inductive Reasoning Model Inductive Reasoning Renewed: A Reply to Commentators
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1