Revisiting Lenneberg's Hypotheses About Early Developmental Plasticity: Language Organization After Left-Hemisphere Perinatal Stroke.

IF 0.6 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Biolinguistics Pub Date : 2017-12-31 DOI:10.5964/bioling.9105
E. Newport, B. Landau, Anna Seydell-Greenwald, P. Turkeltaub, Catherine E. Chambers, A. Dromerick, J. Carpenter, M. Berl, W. Gaillard
{"title":"Revisiting Lenneberg's Hypotheses About Early Developmental Plasticity: Language Organization After Left-Hemisphere Perinatal Stroke.","authors":"E. Newport, B. Landau, Anna Seydell-Greenwald, P. Turkeltaub, Catherine E. Chambers, A. Dromerick, J. Carpenter, M. Berl, W. Gaillard","doi":"10.5964/bioling.9105","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"A prominent theme in the literature on brain injury and recovery has been the notion of early developmental plasticity (Kennard 1940, Kolb et al. 2000). This has been a particular focus in work on language. In healthy adults, language is virtually always lateralized to the left hemisphere (LH; Broca 1861, Gazzaniga & Sperry 1967). However, Basser (1962) and Lenneberg (1967) compiled published case studies, their own patient histories, and available medical records of children and adults with left and right hemisphere lesions or hemispherectomy to determine whether there were systematic effects of hemisphere and age of insult on the development or recovery of language. From these data, Lenneberg (1967) concluded that, when even massive injuries to one hemisphere occurred before age 2, most children developed language normally or with only some delay; and these outcomes were the same regardless of which hemisphere was affected. This led him to argue that initially, before cerebral dominance was fully established, the two hemispheres were equipotential for language. This was less true for older children and was definitively no longer true for adults, who showed strong LH specificity for language interference and some recovery from mild aphasias, but did not recover completely from severe aphasias or left hemispherectomies. Using the Wada test (briefly anesthetizing one hemisphere and then the other; see Loring et al. 1992) to determine which hemisphere controls speech, Rasmussen & Milner (1977) showed that in children, depending on the age at injury, speech that is ordinarily in the left hemisphere could be controlled successfully by the right hemisphere or by an alternate region of the damaged left hemisphere. Similar reorganization was not observed in adults, even decades after injury. These generalizations have long formed the classic picture of recovery of language function. \n \nHowever, recent research on organization after early injury in children has not always found such consistent outcomes. Some studies have found good language abilities after focal brain injury in children, but others have not (Banich et al. 1990, Ballantyne et al. 2007, Levine et al. 2005, Moesch, Max, & Tranel 2005, Montour-Proulx et al. 2004, Stiles et al. 2012, Westmacott et al. 2010). Relatively few studies of neural reorganization have been done with children, also with somewhat inconsistent outcomes (see, e.g., Mbwana et al. 2009, Rosenberger et al. 2009, and You et al. 2011 for language reorganization with epilepsy, and Booth et al. 2000, Dick et al. 2013, Fair et al. 2006, 2010, Jacola et al. 2006, Liegeois et al. 2004, Raja et al. 2010, Staudt et al. 2002, 2007, and Tillema et al. 2008 on perinatal stroke). This variation of outcomes may be due to true variation among children, or to the inclusion of children with a variety of types and causes of focal brain injuries (e.g., periventricular leukomalacia, moya moya, vasculitis, tumors, and hemorrhagic or arterial ischemic strokes) or the effects of other medical problems that are often comorbid with stroke in children (e.g., seizures and seizure medications, heart disease and reduced cortical perfusion, or sickle cell anemia). It might also be due to variation in the ages at which participants were evaluated (see Bates et al. 2001, showing that children with focal brain injuries may show developmental delays but later reach normal levels of performance). \n \nThere has also been little consistency in investigators’ views of the principles governing developmental plasticity for language. Only a few researchers have proposed hypotheses about what areas or networks in the brain are capable of subserving language in the face of early brain injury, and these proposals are in sharp conflict. Vargha-Khadem et al. (1985) suggested that the left hemisphere is uniquely suited for language and that successful reorganization of language is limited to LH brain areas. (See also Raja et al. 2010, who have argued that the remaining left hemisphere voxel activity correlates best with language proficiency after left hemisphere perinatal stroke). Staudt (2002) and Gaillard and colleagues (Gaillard et al. 2007, Berl et al. 2014, Mbwana et al. 2009) have argued that left hemisphere areas or their precise right hemisphere homologues can subserve language when there are early left hemisphere abnormalities. In contrast, Bates et al. (1997) have suggested that the young brain is highly plastic; they argue that “the human capacity for language is not localized at birth,” implying that reasonably normal language skills might be able to develop in numerous other brain regions. Bedny et al. (2011) have argued that congenitally blind individuals utilize even occipital cortex (including V1) during spoken language processing. Can this wide range of brain areas indeed support language? In our ongoing work we seek to understand the forces that lead language to develop in only certain brain areas in the healthy child and also to understand what areas can support language after early brain injury. \n \nAn important literature is the work of the Gaillard lab (Gaillard et al. 2007, Berl et al. 2014, Mbwana et al. 2009) using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the organization of language over development and how it is affected by early and continuing epilepsy (and the brain abnormalities that cause them). In response to chronic epilepsy, cortical processing of language is frequently restructured, with some or all language function shifted to the right hemisphere. Their work has shown a limited number of ways in which language is organized across a very large number of children: in the usual left hemisphere areas, in the precisely homotopic right hemisphere areas, or in the usual left hemisphere temporal areas combined with the homotopic right hemisphere frontal areas. No other patterns of language organization appear in their subjects. \n \nHowever, while chronic seizures can be clinically devastating for children, they apparently exert relatively mild effects on cortical organization: 75 % of children with early chronic seizures retain the typical left hemisphere pattern of language organization. To examine language after very early damage to the brain, we are focusing on perinatal arterial ischemic stroke, a relatively rare neurological event but one whose characteristics may provide an excellent model for examining the neural organization of language after early brain injury and for gaining insight into important principles of neural plasticity for language. In perinatal stroke, the injuries are typically much larger than in pediatric epilepsy but are relatively stereotyped in anatomy; approximate time of onset is clear; and in most patients there are not continuing seizures or long periods of time on antiepileptic medications. This makes our perinatal population an important contrast to Gaillard et al.’s work on epilepsy.","PeriodicalId":54041,"journal":{"name":"Biolinguistics","volume":"11 1","pages":"407-422"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6000,"publicationDate":"2017-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"55","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Biolinguistics","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.5964/bioling.9105","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"0","JCRName":"LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 55

Abstract

A prominent theme in the literature on brain injury and recovery has been the notion of early developmental plasticity (Kennard 1940, Kolb et al. 2000). This has been a particular focus in work on language. In healthy adults, language is virtually always lateralized to the left hemisphere (LH; Broca 1861, Gazzaniga & Sperry 1967). However, Basser (1962) and Lenneberg (1967) compiled published case studies, their own patient histories, and available medical records of children and adults with left and right hemisphere lesions or hemispherectomy to determine whether there were systematic effects of hemisphere and age of insult on the development or recovery of language. From these data, Lenneberg (1967) concluded that, when even massive injuries to one hemisphere occurred before age 2, most children developed language normally or with only some delay; and these outcomes were the same regardless of which hemisphere was affected. This led him to argue that initially, before cerebral dominance was fully established, the two hemispheres were equipotential for language. This was less true for older children and was definitively no longer true for adults, who showed strong LH specificity for language interference and some recovery from mild aphasias, but did not recover completely from severe aphasias or left hemispherectomies. Using the Wada test (briefly anesthetizing one hemisphere and then the other; see Loring et al. 1992) to determine which hemisphere controls speech, Rasmussen & Milner (1977) showed that in children, depending on the age at injury, speech that is ordinarily in the left hemisphere could be controlled successfully by the right hemisphere or by an alternate region of the damaged left hemisphere. Similar reorganization was not observed in adults, even decades after injury. These generalizations have long formed the classic picture of recovery of language function. However, recent research on organization after early injury in children has not always found such consistent outcomes. Some studies have found good language abilities after focal brain injury in children, but others have not (Banich et al. 1990, Ballantyne et al. 2007, Levine et al. 2005, Moesch, Max, & Tranel 2005, Montour-Proulx et al. 2004, Stiles et al. 2012, Westmacott et al. 2010). Relatively few studies of neural reorganization have been done with children, also with somewhat inconsistent outcomes (see, e.g., Mbwana et al. 2009, Rosenberger et al. 2009, and You et al. 2011 for language reorganization with epilepsy, and Booth et al. 2000, Dick et al. 2013, Fair et al. 2006, 2010, Jacola et al. 2006, Liegeois et al. 2004, Raja et al. 2010, Staudt et al. 2002, 2007, and Tillema et al. 2008 on perinatal stroke). This variation of outcomes may be due to true variation among children, or to the inclusion of children with a variety of types and causes of focal brain injuries (e.g., periventricular leukomalacia, moya moya, vasculitis, tumors, and hemorrhagic or arterial ischemic strokes) or the effects of other medical problems that are often comorbid with stroke in children (e.g., seizures and seizure medications, heart disease and reduced cortical perfusion, or sickle cell anemia). It might also be due to variation in the ages at which participants were evaluated (see Bates et al. 2001, showing that children with focal brain injuries may show developmental delays but later reach normal levels of performance). There has also been little consistency in investigators’ views of the principles governing developmental plasticity for language. Only a few researchers have proposed hypotheses about what areas or networks in the brain are capable of subserving language in the face of early brain injury, and these proposals are in sharp conflict. Vargha-Khadem et al. (1985) suggested that the left hemisphere is uniquely suited for language and that successful reorganization of language is limited to LH brain areas. (See also Raja et al. 2010, who have argued that the remaining left hemisphere voxel activity correlates best with language proficiency after left hemisphere perinatal stroke). Staudt (2002) and Gaillard and colleagues (Gaillard et al. 2007, Berl et al. 2014, Mbwana et al. 2009) have argued that left hemisphere areas or their precise right hemisphere homologues can subserve language when there are early left hemisphere abnormalities. In contrast, Bates et al. (1997) have suggested that the young brain is highly plastic; they argue that “the human capacity for language is not localized at birth,” implying that reasonably normal language skills might be able to develop in numerous other brain regions. Bedny et al. (2011) have argued that congenitally blind individuals utilize even occipital cortex (including V1) during spoken language processing. Can this wide range of brain areas indeed support language? In our ongoing work we seek to understand the forces that lead language to develop in only certain brain areas in the healthy child and also to understand what areas can support language after early brain injury. An important literature is the work of the Gaillard lab (Gaillard et al. 2007, Berl et al. 2014, Mbwana et al. 2009) using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the organization of language over development and how it is affected by early and continuing epilepsy (and the brain abnormalities that cause them). In response to chronic epilepsy, cortical processing of language is frequently restructured, with some or all language function shifted to the right hemisphere. Their work has shown a limited number of ways in which language is organized across a very large number of children: in the usual left hemisphere areas, in the precisely homotopic right hemisphere areas, or in the usual left hemisphere temporal areas combined with the homotopic right hemisphere frontal areas. No other patterns of language organization appear in their subjects. However, while chronic seizures can be clinically devastating for children, they apparently exert relatively mild effects on cortical organization: 75 % of children with early chronic seizures retain the typical left hemisphere pattern of language organization. To examine language after very early damage to the brain, we are focusing on perinatal arterial ischemic stroke, a relatively rare neurological event but one whose characteristics may provide an excellent model for examining the neural organization of language after early brain injury and for gaining insight into important principles of neural plasticity for language. In perinatal stroke, the injuries are typically much larger than in pediatric epilepsy but are relatively stereotyped in anatomy; approximate time of onset is clear; and in most patients there are not continuing seizures or long periods of time on antiepileptic medications. This makes our perinatal population an important contrast to Gaillard et al.’s work on epilepsy.
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
重新审视Lenneberg关于早期发育可塑性的假设:左半球围产期中风后的语言组织。
关于脑损伤和恢复的文献中一个突出的主题是早期发育可塑性的概念(Kennard 1940,Kolb等人,2000)。这一直是语言工作中的一个特别关注点。在健康成年人中,语言几乎总是偏向左半球(LH;Broca 1861,Gazzaniga和Sperry 1967)。然而,Basser(1962)和Lenneberg(1967)汇编了已发表的案例研究、他们自己的病史以及患有左右半球病变或半球切除术的儿童和成人的可用医疗记录,以确定半球和侮辱年龄是否对语言的发展或恢复产生系统性影响。根据这些数据,Lenneberg(1967)得出结论,即使一个半球在2岁之前发生大规模损伤,大多数儿童的语言发展正常或只有一些延迟;无论哪个半球受到影响,这些结果都是相同的。这使他认为,最初,在大脑支配地位完全确立之前,两个半球对语言是等电位的。对于年龄较大的儿童来说,情况并非如此,对于成年人来说,情况也不再如此,他们对语言干扰表现出强烈的LH特异性,并从轻度失语症中恢复了一些,但从严重失语症或左半球切除术中并没有完全恢复。Rasmussen和Milner(1977)使用Wada测试(短暂麻醉一个半球,然后麻醉另一个半球;见Loring等人1992)来确定哪个半球控制言语,结果表明,根据受伤年龄的不同,儿童通常在左半球的言语可以通过右半球或受损左半球的另一个区域成功控制。即使在受伤几十年后,也没有在成年人身上观察到类似的重组。这些概括早已形成了语言功能恢复的经典图景。然而,最近关于儿童早期损伤后组织的研究并不总是发现如此一致的结果。一些研究发现,儿童局灶性脑损伤后语言能力良好,但其他研究则不然(Banich等人1990,Ballantyne等人2007,Levine等人2005,Moesch,Max,&Tranel 2005,Montour Proulx等人2004,Stiles等人2012,Westmacott等人2010)。对儿童进行的神经重组研究相对较少,也有一些不一致的结果(例如,参见Mbwana等人2009、Rosenberger等人2009和You等人2011关于癫痫的语言重组,以及Booth等人2000、Dick等人2013、Fair等人2006、2010、Jacoba等人2006、Liegeois等人2004、Raja等人2010、Staudt等人2002、2007和Tillema等人2008关于围产期中风)。这种结果的变化可能是由于儿童之间的真实变化,或包括患有多种类型和原因的局灶性脑损伤的儿童(例如,室周白质软化症、烟雾症、血管炎、肿瘤和出血性或动脉缺血性中风)或其他医学问题的影响,这些问题通常与儿童中风共病(例如,癫痫发作和癫痫药物、心脏病和皮质灌注减少或镰状细胞贫血)。这也可能是由于评估参与者的年龄变化(见Bates等人,2001年,研究表明,局灶性脑损伤的儿童可能表现出发育迟缓,但后来达到了正常的表现水平)。研究人员对语言发展可塑性原则的看法也几乎没有一致性。只有少数研究人员提出了关于大脑中哪些区域或网络能够在早期脑损伤时为语言服务的假设,而这些建议存在着尖锐的冲突。Vargha Khadem等人(1985)认为,左半球是唯一适合语言的区域,语言的成功重组仅限于LH大脑区域。(另见Raja等人,2010年,他们认为左半球围产期中风后剩余的左半球体素活动与语言熟练度相关性最好)。Staudt(2002)和Gaillard及其同事(Gaillard等人,2007年,Berl等人,2014年,Mbwana等人,2009年)认为,当存在早期左半球异常时,左半球区域或其精确的右半球同源物可以辅助语言。相比之下,Bates等人(1997)提出,年轻的大脑具有高度可塑性;他们认为,“人类的语言能力在出生时就没有局限性”,这意味着合理正常的语言技能可能能够在许多其他大脑区域发展。Bedny等人(2011)认为,先天性盲人在口语处理过程中甚至会利用枕叶皮层(包括V1)。 这种广泛的大脑区域真的能支持语言吗?在我们正在进行的工作中,我们试图了解导致语言只在健康儿童的某些大脑区域发展的力量,并了解早期脑损伤后哪些区域可以支持语言。一个重要的文献是Gaillard实验室的工作(Gaillard等人,2007年,Berl等人,2014年,Mbwana等人,2009年),使用功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)来检查语言过度发育的组织,以及它如何受到早期和持续癫痫的影响(以及导致癫痫的大脑异常)。作为对慢性癫痫的反应,大脑皮层对语言的处理经常发生重组,部分或全部语言功能转移到右半球。他们的工作表明,在大量儿童中,语言的组织方式有限:在通常的左半球区域,在精确的同位右半球区域,或者在通常的左侧半球时间区域与同位右侧半球额叶区域相结合。在他们的研究对象中没有出现其他的语言组织模式。然而,尽管慢性癫痫发作对儿童来说可能具有临床破坏性,但它们显然对皮层组织产生了相对温和的影响:75%的早期慢性癫痫发作儿童保留了典型的左半球语言组织模式。为了检查大脑早期损伤后的语言,我们将重点放在围产期动脉缺血性中风上,这是一种相对罕见的神经事件,但其特征可能为检查早期脑损伤后语言的神经组织和深入了解语言神经可塑性的重要原理提供了一个极好的模型。围产期中风的损伤通常比儿童癫痫大得多,但在解剖学上相对定型;发病的大致时间是明确的;大多数患者没有持续的癫痫发作或长时间服用抗癫痫药物。这使得我们的围产期人群与Gaillard等人关于癫痫的研究形成了重要对比。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Biolinguistics
Biolinguistics LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS-
CiteScore
1.50
自引率
0.00%
发文量
5
审稿时长
12 weeks
期刊最新文献
Biolinguistics end-of-year notice 2023 Why large language models are poor theories of human linguistic cognition: A reply to Piantadosi Social evolution and commitment: Bridging the gap between formal linguistic theories and language evolution research A future without a past: Philosophical consequences of Merge Eademne sunt?
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1