What the obesity epidemic does not need: A cancel culture

Q3 Medicine Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.) Pub Date : 2021-02-26 DOI:10.1002/lim2.27
Thomas Wood, Sue Kenneally, Fraser Birrell
{"title":"What the obesity epidemic does not need: A cancel culture","authors":"Thomas Wood,&nbsp;Sue Kenneally,&nbsp;Fraser Birrell","doi":"10.1002/lim2.27","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>A paper recently published in <i>Lifestyle Medicine</i> highlights the importance of informed and respectful debate as part of the scientific endeavour. Appearing in the first edition of the journal, Jacob et al.<span><sup>1</sup></span> examined data from 7403 participants from the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS), a nationally representative survey of the English adult population conducted by the National Centre for Social Research and Leicester University.<span><sup>2</sup></span> They found that there was a negative association between Verbal IQ, estimated using the National Adult Reading Test (NART), and obesity. During peer review, the manuscript was praised for its clear and robust statistical analyses. However, due to the nature of the topic, editorial review included assessing the manuscript for inappropriate or discriminatory language or conclusions, as well as ensuring both scientific and analytical merit in line with the journal's scope. Perhaps unsurprisingly, the paper still resulted in significant vocal debate on social media (Twitter), including calls for the paper's retraction. As Twitter does not uniformly allow for reasoned discussions, formal letters to the editor were solicited by Wiley to highlight any significant issues and allow for formal response by the original authors. This process is still an important aspect of moving scientific research forward, and as a journal we follow both International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE)<span><sup>3</sup></span> and the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) publishing principles,<span><sup>4</sup></span> which include giving a platform for rebuttal to published articles and encouraging logical and reasoned scientific debate. Both the letter to the editor by Redsell et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> and the response by Jacob et al.<span><sup>6</sup></span> are included in this issue. While we stand by the publication of the manuscript – as further outlined below – we believe that it is the job of the scientific community at large to iteratively move a field forward based on discussions such as those highlighted here. Therefore, to some extent, each reader can and will make up their own minds based on the evidence, following scientific principles to do so. We hope this commentary assists that process.</p><p>Using weight or normative estimators of body composition, such as the body mass index (BMI), as predictors of individual health or health outcomes is both controversial and highly emotive.<span><sup>7</sup></span> This is clear from the tone of both the letter by Redsell et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> and the response by Jacob et al.<span><sup>6</sup></span> One thing that we feel is absolutely necessary in order to move discussions in this field forward is the separation of our personal feelings about a topic from the scientific arguments, as much as that is possible. In line with that, both the letter writers and article authors were given opportunities to tone down their language, and both articles have been lightly edited (in language but not content) to more constructively moderate the discourse.</p><p>A discrete focus on the scientific arguments and process of a manuscript is important because Redsell and co-authors present unsubstantiated accusations such as that the manuscript is ‘ill-advised (at best)’, and include veiled threats that ‘this paper is also likely to cause harm, which may risk reputational damage’. The latter assertion was largely focused on how the findings may be portrayed by the media. We absolutely agree that accurate reporting of scientific findings in the media – as well as ensuring that research is not discriminatory – is an essential part of science publication and communication. However, the broader implication in the letter appears to be that research into populations at particular risk of, or potential downstream (health) consequences of, obesity should be avoided because it may be inaccurately portrayed in the media and used to propagate weight stigma. This argument also includes <i>non-sequiturs</i> in the assumptions that risk factors for obesity usually regarded as non-modifiable are not of interest for potential targeting of modifiable risk factors, and that patients and the media will necessarily fuel negative stereotypes. Patients, including the patient representative who helped write the editorial in our last issue,<span><sup>8</sup></span> are clear that they want to see the evidence for themselves, which the open access model supports. Therefore, it would be a curious approach to sanitise the content due to potential for misinterpretation rather than accept submitted articles based on their scientific quality.</p><p>Redsell et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> also suggest that the original study suffered from a number of methodological flaws and breached two ethical principles (beneficence and justice). Though there is some discussion regarding the utility of the National Adult Reading Test in this setting, no clear methodological flaws – either in the analysis or the interpretation – were raised. We agree that the quality of a data used to examine a specific question can almost always be improved. For instance, it must be recognised that societal inequities are significant drivers of both obesity and 'performance' in normative tests such as those for intelligence quotient (IQ)<span><sup>9</sup></span> with the history, use, and interpretation of the original IQ tests grounded in both sexism and racism.<span><sup>10, 11</sup></span> Jacob et al. performed multiple sensitivity analyses to adjust for individual background and environmental factors that might influence the outcome, but the direction and magnitude of the effect remained consistent. Importantly, though the authors could have been more explicit about other unknown confounders, as well as the fact that socioeconomic parameters such as race, income, and education are often proxies for much greater social inequities and a wide range of lived experiences,<span><sup>12</sup></span> they performed appropriate adjustments with the available data and were clear about the limitations. This does not preclude the possibility that different results will be seen in the future if datasets containing a broader range of equitable cognitive tests and accurate body composition measurements are collated and analysed. Advances in epidemiology like other branches of science is iterative and while no clinical research study is perfect, constructive criticism is a key driver of improving study design and conduct.</p><p>The intended goals of public health epidemiology, and what can and cannot be concluded from a particular dataset or analysis, is a crucial aspect of evidence-based medicine. Redsell et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> do rightly state that the original manuscript only ‘explores the association between IQ and obesity, not causation’, while also accusing the authors of implying causation. The former is absolutely true, as is the case with any epidemiological study, but Jacob et al<span><sup>6</sup></span> clearly do not attempt to assume causality, presenting previous work that could drive causality (if present) in either direction. Multiple factors may explain the results, from unknown confounding by socioeconomic and environmental factors to an inflammatory phenotype associated with visceral adipose tissue that may impact the brain,<span><sup>9, 13</sup></span> and while these questions remain it is imperative that objective research continue. The way the results are written by Jacob et al. is largely driven by the requirements of multivariate statistical analyses, where a response variable (in this case obesity as a binary outcome) must be chosen, along with associated predictor variables. The outcomes of these models include an odds ratio for obesity, but do not imply that the predictors (e.g., Verbal IQ) are directly causative.</p><p>Redsell et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> do also highlight papers that support the potential for better metrics and assessments for use when studying this question in the future, though one might argue that their representation of cited literature differs from that stated in the papers themselves. For instance, citing McGurn et al. as evidence that the performance of the NART was only modest when that paper describes a strong enough correlation to validate the use of the test.<span><sup>14</sup></span> As with any standardised test, we agree with Redsell et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> that the variance in NART scores (as well as BMI) due to other factors such as material and social deprivation is both possible and likely, but this is at least partly addressed by the statistical adjustments and does not invalidate the results as presented. We also agree that self-reported BMI does not perfectly reflect clinical measurements,<span><sup>15</sup></span> as also acknowledged by Jacob et al.<span><sup>6</sup></span> in the original manuscript, although self-report is generally regarded as a valid approach. If anything, self-report is likely to lead to an under-estimate of BMI so is likely to have underestimated the prevalence of obesity in this dataset and as such diluted any associations.</p><p>Correct attribution and citation is important, so the letter includes full references inserted at the proofing stage, which clarifies which publications (especially newspapers) may be more likely to misinterpret the science and create headlines, albeit that the references are not recent or relating to policy in the country of publication. We do operate in an international environment, so global awareness is important too, both in not bowing to excessive perceived pressure from the approach of certain UK newspapers nor being influenced by censoring in more totalitarian regimes. There is a balance to strike here: recent events in the Washington Capitol and social media's involvement and subsequent repudiation of longstanding approaches show this is a developing area.</p><p>We do strongly believe that every person has the right to feel safe, comfortable, and happy in their body, and multiple studies have indeed shown that damaging weight stigma is incredibly prevalent in the popular media, contributing significantly to society's negative viewpoints around body weight.<span><sup>16</sup></span> Work by the “health at every size” (HAES) movement is both important and growing, often highlighting the fact that societal weight stigma and diet culture can significantly negatively impact the health and quality of life of certain targeted individuals,<span><sup>17-20</sup></span> as well as the fact that BMI or body composition may not be deterministic with respect to long-term health outcomes.<span><sup>7, 21, 22</sup></span> These are important issues must be addressed head-on, for instance by tackling dangerous aspects of diet culture and media misrepresentation of weight and health, as well as clearly separating out epidemiological work designed to improve risk stratification and policy at the societal level from the nuanced, holistic, and personalised approaches that should be taken to improve an individual's health trajectory with their consent and concordance, which will most often include interventions that are weight neutral. These societal issues should not, however, prevent obesity research at the population and mechanistic levels being performed to identify those at risk, or potential interventions, particularly within systems and institutions. Identifying at-risk populations to help institute systemic change, rather than place health solely as the responsibility of the individual<span><sup>23, 24</sup></span> is an important goal of research using population datasets such as that presented by Jacob et al.,<span><sup>1</sup></span> and these studies are never intended to be used to make statements about individuals. However, there is compelling evidence that obesity is a strong driver of poor health outcomes, more powerful for many than physical inactivity.<span><sup>25</sup></span> Depending on the criteria used, 7–50% of those with obesity are metabolically healthy,<span><sup>26</sup></span> but even metabolically healthy obesity appears to be associated with an intermediate risk profile for outcomes such as cardiovascular disease.<span><sup>27</sup></span> Recent systematic reviews have also suggested that (i) weight loss interventions may improve health-related quality of life, with the effect potentially correlated with magnitude of weight loss,<span><sup>28</sup></span> and (ii) that behavioural weight management interventions result in improvements in a number of mental health outcomes, including body image concerns and self-efficacy,<span><sup>29</sup></span> without negatively impacting overall mental health. None of this negates an individual's experience of weight and weight stigma, but instead highlights the importance of objective assessment of the evidence at a higher level coupled with individualised patient-centric approaches. Therefore, those who want to help those impacted may be better served by encouraging the use of valid tools for both population and individual risk assessment, as this can inform empowering choices by the individuals, when shared in appropriate ways, which will vary by country, setting and individual.<span><sup>30</sup></span></p><p>In summary, with respect to the original manuscript and subsequent communications, we respectfully maintain that the original manuscript meets our criteria for publication. Retraction is not justified on the basis of the arguments presented. However, we welcome rational scientific debate and hope that by publishing both the letter and reply we can encourage a constructive collaborative approach to destigmatising obesity rather than shaming those who do not avoid addressing difficult and complex issues.</p><p>FB is Editor-in-Chief for the Wiley open access journal <i>Lifestyle Medicine</i> and Director of Science and Research for the British Society of Lifestyle Medicine. SK is a bariatric physician, Regional Director for South Wales &amp; on Nutrition Interface Group for the British Society of Lifestyle Medicine. TW is an Associate Editor for the Wiley open access journal <i>Lifestyle Medicine</i> and is a Trustee of British Society of Lifestyle Medicine.</p>","PeriodicalId":74076,"journal":{"name":"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2021-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1002/lim2.27","citationCount":"2","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/lim2.27","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"Medicine","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2

Abstract

A paper recently published in Lifestyle Medicine highlights the importance of informed and respectful debate as part of the scientific endeavour. Appearing in the first edition of the journal, Jacob et al.1 examined data from 7403 participants from the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS), a nationally representative survey of the English adult population conducted by the National Centre for Social Research and Leicester University.2 They found that there was a negative association between Verbal IQ, estimated using the National Adult Reading Test (NART), and obesity. During peer review, the manuscript was praised for its clear and robust statistical analyses. However, due to the nature of the topic, editorial review included assessing the manuscript for inappropriate or discriminatory language or conclusions, as well as ensuring both scientific and analytical merit in line with the journal's scope. Perhaps unsurprisingly, the paper still resulted in significant vocal debate on social media (Twitter), including calls for the paper's retraction. As Twitter does not uniformly allow for reasoned discussions, formal letters to the editor were solicited by Wiley to highlight any significant issues and allow for formal response by the original authors. This process is still an important aspect of moving scientific research forward, and as a journal we follow both International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE)3 and the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) publishing principles,4 which include giving a platform for rebuttal to published articles and encouraging logical and reasoned scientific debate. Both the letter to the editor by Redsell et al.5 and the response by Jacob et al.6 are included in this issue. While we stand by the publication of the manuscript – as further outlined below – we believe that it is the job of the scientific community at large to iteratively move a field forward based on discussions such as those highlighted here. Therefore, to some extent, each reader can and will make up their own minds based on the evidence, following scientific principles to do so. We hope this commentary assists that process.

Using weight or normative estimators of body composition, such as the body mass index (BMI), as predictors of individual health or health outcomes is both controversial and highly emotive.7 This is clear from the tone of both the letter by Redsell et al.5 and the response by Jacob et al.6 One thing that we feel is absolutely necessary in order to move discussions in this field forward is the separation of our personal feelings about a topic from the scientific arguments, as much as that is possible. In line with that, both the letter writers and article authors were given opportunities to tone down their language, and both articles have been lightly edited (in language but not content) to more constructively moderate the discourse.

A discrete focus on the scientific arguments and process of a manuscript is important because Redsell and co-authors present unsubstantiated accusations such as that the manuscript is ‘ill-advised (at best)’, and include veiled threats that ‘this paper is also likely to cause harm, which may risk reputational damage’. The latter assertion was largely focused on how the findings may be portrayed by the media. We absolutely agree that accurate reporting of scientific findings in the media – as well as ensuring that research is not discriminatory – is an essential part of science publication and communication. However, the broader implication in the letter appears to be that research into populations at particular risk of, or potential downstream (health) consequences of, obesity should be avoided because it may be inaccurately portrayed in the media and used to propagate weight stigma. This argument also includes non-sequiturs in the assumptions that risk factors for obesity usually regarded as non-modifiable are not of interest for potential targeting of modifiable risk factors, and that patients and the media will necessarily fuel negative stereotypes. Patients, including the patient representative who helped write the editorial in our last issue,8 are clear that they want to see the evidence for themselves, which the open access model supports. Therefore, it would be a curious approach to sanitise the content due to potential for misinterpretation rather than accept submitted articles based on their scientific quality.

Redsell et al.5 also suggest that the original study suffered from a number of methodological flaws and breached two ethical principles (beneficence and justice). Though there is some discussion regarding the utility of the National Adult Reading Test in this setting, no clear methodological flaws – either in the analysis or the interpretation – were raised. We agree that the quality of a data used to examine a specific question can almost always be improved. For instance, it must be recognised that societal inequities are significant drivers of both obesity and 'performance' in normative tests such as those for intelligence quotient (IQ)9 with the history, use, and interpretation of the original IQ tests grounded in both sexism and racism.10, 11 Jacob et al. performed multiple sensitivity analyses to adjust for individual background and environmental factors that might influence the outcome, but the direction and magnitude of the effect remained consistent. Importantly, though the authors could have been more explicit about other unknown confounders, as well as the fact that socioeconomic parameters such as race, income, and education are often proxies for much greater social inequities and a wide range of lived experiences,12 they performed appropriate adjustments with the available data and were clear about the limitations. This does not preclude the possibility that different results will be seen in the future if datasets containing a broader range of equitable cognitive tests and accurate body composition measurements are collated and analysed. Advances in epidemiology like other branches of science is iterative and while no clinical research study is perfect, constructive criticism is a key driver of improving study design and conduct.

The intended goals of public health epidemiology, and what can and cannot be concluded from a particular dataset or analysis, is a crucial aspect of evidence-based medicine. Redsell et al.5 do rightly state that the original manuscript only ‘explores the association between IQ and obesity, not causation’, while also accusing the authors of implying causation. The former is absolutely true, as is the case with any epidemiological study, but Jacob et al6 clearly do not attempt to assume causality, presenting previous work that could drive causality (if present) in either direction. Multiple factors may explain the results, from unknown confounding by socioeconomic and environmental factors to an inflammatory phenotype associated with visceral adipose tissue that may impact the brain,9, 13 and while these questions remain it is imperative that objective research continue. The way the results are written by Jacob et al. is largely driven by the requirements of multivariate statistical analyses, where a response variable (in this case obesity as a binary outcome) must be chosen, along with associated predictor variables. The outcomes of these models include an odds ratio for obesity, but do not imply that the predictors (e.g., Verbal IQ) are directly causative.

Redsell et al.5 do also highlight papers that support the potential for better metrics and assessments for use when studying this question in the future, though one might argue that their representation of cited literature differs from that stated in the papers themselves. For instance, citing McGurn et al. as evidence that the performance of the NART was only modest when that paper describes a strong enough correlation to validate the use of the test.14 As with any standardised test, we agree with Redsell et al.5 that the variance in NART scores (as well as BMI) due to other factors such as material and social deprivation is both possible and likely, but this is at least partly addressed by the statistical adjustments and does not invalidate the results as presented. We also agree that self-reported BMI does not perfectly reflect clinical measurements,15 as also acknowledged by Jacob et al.6 in the original manuscript, although self-report is generally regarded as a valid approach. If anything, self-report is likely to lead to an under-estimate of BMI so is likely to have underestimated the prevalence of obesity in this dataset and as such diluted any associations.

Correct attribution and citation is important, so the letter includes full references inserted at the proofing stage, which clarifies which publications (especially newspapers) may be more likely to misinterpret the science and create headlines, albeit that the references are not recent or relating to policy in the country of publication. We do operate in an international environment, so global awareness is important too, both in not bowing to excessive perceived pressure from the approach of certain UK newspapers nor being influenced by censoring in more totalitarian regimes. There is a balance to strike here: recent events in the Washington Capitol and social media's involvement and subsequent repudiation of longstanding approaches show this is a developing area.

We do strongly believe that every person has the right to feel safe, comfortable, and happy in their body, and multiple studies have indeed shown that damaging weight stigma is incredibly prevalent in the popular media, contributing significantly to society's negative viewpoints around body weight.16 Work by the “health at every size” (HAES) movement is both important and growing, often highlighting the fact that societal weight stigma and diet culture can significantly negatively impact the health and quality of life of certain targeted individuals,17-20 as well as the fact that BMI or body composition may not be deterministic with respect to long-term health outcomes.7, 21, 22 These are important issues must be addressed head-on, for instance by tackling dangerous aspects of diet culture and media misrepresentation of weight and health, as well as clearly separating out epidemiological work designed to improve risk stratification and policy at the societal level from the nuanced, holistic, and personalised approaches that should be taken to improve an individual's health trajectory with their consent and concordance, which will most often include interventions that are weight neutral. These societal issues should not, however, prevent obesity research at the population and mechanistic levels being performed to identify those at risk, or potential interventions, particularly within systems and institutions. Identifying at-risk populations to help institute systemic change, rather than place health solely as the responsibility of the individual23, 24 is an important goal of research using population datasets such as that presented by Jacob et al.,1 and these studies are never intended to be used to make statements about individuals. However, there is compelling evidence that obesity is a strong driver of poor health outcomes, more powerful for many than physical inactivity.25 Depending on the criteria used, 7–50% of those with obesity are metabolically healthy,26 but even metabolically healthy obesity appears to be associated with an intermediate risk profile for outcomes such as cardiovascular disease.27 Recent systematic reviews have also suggested that (i) weight loss interventions may improve health-related quality of life, with the effect potentially correlated with magnitude of weight loss,28 and (ii) that behavioural weight management interventions result in improvements in a number of mental health outcomes, including body image concerns and self-efficacy,29 without negatively impacting overall mental health. None of this negates an individual's experience of weight and weight stigma, but instead highlights the importance of objective assessment of the evidence at a higher level coupled with individualised patient-centric approaches. Therefore, those who want to help those impacted may be better served by encouraging the use of valid tools for both population and individual risk assessment, as this can inform empowering choices by the individuals, when shared in appropriate ways, which will vary by country, setting and individual.30

In summary, with respect to the original manuscript and subsequent communications, we respectfully maintain that the original manuscript meets our criteria for publication. Retraction is not justified on the basis of the arguments presented. However, we welcome rational scientific debate and hope that by publishing both the letter and reply we can encourage a constructive collaborative approach to destigmatising obesity rather than shaming those who do not avoid addressing difficult and complex issues.

FB is Editor-in-Chief for the Wiley open access journal Lifestyle Medicine and Director of Science and Research for the British Society of Lifestyle Medicine. SK is a bariatric physician, Regional Director for South Wales & on Nutrition Interface Group for the British Society of Lifestyle Medicine. TW is an Associate Editor for the Wiley open access journal Lifestyle Medicine and is a Trustee of British Society of Lifestyle Medicine.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
肥胖流行不需要的是:取消文化
最近发表在《生活方式医学》上的一篇论文强调了作为科学努力的一部分,知情和尊重的辩论的重要性。Jacob et al.研究了2007年成人精神病发病率调查(APMS)的7403名参与者的数据,该调查是由国家社会研究中心和莱斯特大学进行的一项具有全国代表性的英国成年人调查。他们发现,使用国家成人阅读测试(NART)估算的语言智商与肥胖之间存在负相关。在同行评议期间,该手稿因其清晰而有力的统计分析而受到称赞。然而,由于该主题的性质,编辑审查包括评估手稿中不恰当或歧视性的语言或结论,以及确保科学和分析价值符合期刊的范围。不出所料,这篇论文仍然在社交媒体(Twitter)上引发了激烈的争论,包括要求撤回这篇论文的呼声。由于Twitter不允许统一的理性讨论,Wiley要求给编辑写正式的信,以强调任何重要的问题,并允许原作者正式回复。这一过程仍然是推动科学研究向前发展的一个重要方面,作为一本期刊,我们遵循国际医学期刊编辑委员会(ICMJE)3和出版伦理委员会(COPE)的出版原则4,其中包括提供一个反驳已发表文章的平台,并鼓励合乎逻辑和理性的科学辩论。Redsell等人给编辑的信5和Jacob等人的回复6都包含在这一期中。虽然我们支持手稿的发表——正如下面进一步概述的那样——但我们认为,在这里强调的讨论的基础上,迭代地推动一个领域向前发展是整个科学界的工作。因此,在某种程度上,每个读者都可以并且将会根据证据,遵循科学原则来做出自己的决定。我们希望这篇评论有助于这一进程。7 .使用体重或身体组成的标准估计值,如身体质量指数(BMI),作为个人健康或健康结果的预测指标,既存在争议,也非常情绪化从Redsell等人的信和Jacob等人的回应的语气中可以清楚地看出这一点。我们认为,为了推动这一领域的讨论向前发展,有一件事是绝对必要的,那就是尽可能地将我们对一个主题的个人感受与科学论证分离开来。与此相一致的是,信件作者和文章作者都有机会缓和他们的语言,两篇文章都被轻微编辑(语言而不是内容),以更有建设性地缓和话语。对一篇论文的科学论证和过程的独立关注是很重要的,因为Redsell和合著者提出了未经证实的指控,比如该论文“(充其量)是不明智的”,并包含了“这篇论文也可能造成伤害,可能会冒着声誉受损的风险”的隐晦威胁。后一种说法主要集中在媒体如何描述这些发现上。我们绝对同意媒体对科学发现的准确报道——以及确保研究不具有歧视性——是科学出版和传播的一个重要组成部分。然而,这封信中更广泛的含义似乎是,应该避免对肥胖具有特定风险或潜在下游(健康)后果的人群进行研究,因为它可能被媒体不准确地描述并用于传播体重耻辱。这一论点还包括假设中的不合理推论,即肥胖的风险因素通常被认为是不可改变的,对可改变的风险因素的潜在目标不感兴趣,并且患者和媒体必然会助长消极的刻板印象。患者,包括在我们上一期杂志中帮助撰写社论的患者代表,都清楚地表示,他们希望亲眼看到开放获取模式所支持的证据。因此,这将是一种奇怪的方法,因为内容可能被误解,而不是根据其科学质量接受提交的文章。Redsell等人5还指出,最初的研究存在许多方法上的缺陷,违反了两个伦理原则(仁慈和正义)。虽然有一些关于全国成人阅读测试在这种情况下的效用的讨论,但没有明确的方法缺陷-无论是在分析还是解释-被提出。我们同意,用于检查特定问题的数据的质量几乎总是可以改进的。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
2.10
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
审稿时长
7 weeks
期刊最新文献
Issue Information Meeting Abstract Issue Information Analysis of Canadian Physician Obituaries Between 2000 and 2023 to Investigate Trends in Death Between Specialties: A Retrospective Cross-Sectional Study Defining the Traditional Mediterranean Lifestyle: Joint International Consensus Statement
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1