Getting in Touch – Social status predicts physical interaction in classrooms

S. J. Eder
{"title":"Getting in Touch – Social status predicts physical interaction in classrooms","authors":"S. J. Eder","doi":"10.22330/HE/34/159-172","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Social interactions and hierarchical structures in classrooms are studied in a number of scientific disciplines, yet the complexity of such systems makes them hard to investigate. In the present study we explore the relationship between social status and bodily interaction, since non-verbal communication and touch play a role in most social systems, yet are poorly understood in school settings. We developed a novel approach to assess social status in grammar school students by way of measuring the presence in others’ minds: Classmates assessed their peers in intellectual, social and physical domains. Additionally, we measured the amount and nature of physical interactions among classmates during breaks in the classroom. These interactions were tracked with the help of older, trained and regularly supervised students from the same school. This peer-to-peer method generated large amounts of data over a period of two months, during which 168 students were observed repeatedly. Results show that touching behavior is modulated by social status and sex: The amount of physical interaction with classmates increases significantly with social status. Same sex touching of intimate zones such as breasts, lap and buttocks occur more frequently among individuals of similar status as compared to touching the intimate zones of the opposite sex. The latter involves extremely high and low ranked individuals more often than same-sex interactions. This study helps to understand formative interactions within classrooms and gives rise to new questions on the establishment and maintenance of hierarchies in peer groups. Keywords: social interaction, classroom hierarchy, social touch, physical interaction, social status. _________________________________________________________\u2028 * Paper presented at XXIV Biennial Conference of Human Ethology 2018, in Santiago, Chile. \u2029 Eder, S.J. & Oberzaucher, E. (2019). Getting in Touch – Social status predicts physical interaction in classrooms. Human Ethology, 34, 159-172. https://doi.org/ 10.22330/he/34/159-172 submitted: 31.Oct. 2018; revised: 12. Aug. 2019; accepted: 20.Aug. 2019 Eder, S.J. & Oberzaucher, E. (2019). Getting in Touch Human Ethology, 34, 159-172 TOUCH, HIERARCHY AND SOCIAL INTERACTIONS IN CLASSROOMS The ambivalence and ambiguity of touches, especially when status differences and dependencies are involved, are publicly debated (Kantor, 2018; Astor, 2018). Recently, this topic has gained attention in the context of classrooms through the initiative of a US-school to ban touches from schools altogether (The Associated Press, 2007). Other schools are merely restricting ‘inappropriate’ touches ( Jones, 2011). It is of little surprise that measures are taken to outlaw unwanted physical interactions, but this might lead to depriving us from essential parts of our social lives: The many functions of touch not only include expression of dominance, but also affiliative interactions, such as social grooming (Lehmann, Korstjens & Dunbar, 2007; Dunbar, 2012). Physical interaction serves a communicative purpose in social interactions. Not only can touch convey emotions (Hertenstein, Holmes, McCullough, & Keltner, 2009; Hertenstein, Keltner, App, Bulleit & Jaskolka, 2006) and reflect social bonds (Suvilehto et al, 2019), it also increases persuasiveness and has positive effects on compliance (Gueguen, Jacob & Boulbry, 2007; Gueguen & Fischer-Lokou, 2003; Guéguen, 2002). Social touch is known to have various physiological benefits such as a decreased heart rate and increased oxytocin levels (Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, & Ehlert, 2003; Henricson, Berglund, Maatta, Ekman & Segesten, 2008; Light, Grewen & Amico, 2005), and it can counteract the negative effects induced by stress, as measured with physiological and biochemical markers such as blood pressure and salivary cortisol (Grewen, Anderson, Girdler, & Light, 2003; Ditzen, Neumann, Bodenmann, von Dawans, Turner, Ehlert & Heinrichs, 2007). This nonverbal comforting effect of touch is more effective than the benefits obtained from verbal social support (Holt-Lunstad, Birmingham & Light, 2008). It appears to be an innate mechanism, as lowered cortisol levels resulting from physical touch can not only be observed in adults, but also in neonatal premature infants (Neu, Laudenslager, & Robinson, 2009). Empirical findings indicate that the positive effects of touch affect both the toucher and the one being touched (Neu et al., 2009). Healthy touching interactions are a sign of secure dyadic attachment: Infants of depressed mothers receive less physical attention and compensate for this with selfstimulation (Herrera, Reissland, & Shepherd, 2004). Ferber and colleagues identified affectionate, stimulating and instrumental types of caregiving touch in humans (Ferber, Feldman & Makhoul, 2008). Caregiving touch is essential for psychological and physiological development: Children deprived of touch show developmental delays i.e. in cognitive skills (MacLean, 2003; Nelson, 2007). Not only humans, but other mammals depend on touch for healthy development, too (Harlow,1958; Gonzalez, Lovic, Ward, Wainwright & Fleming, 2001): Early comparative studies emphasize the importance not only of mother-infant-interactions, but also of peer to peer contact in mammalian development (Harlow, Harlow, Dodsworth & Arling, 1966; Gonzalez et al., 2001). Later experimental studies confirmed that developmental delays can indeed result from missing tactile stimulation (Scafidi, Field, Schanberg, Bauer, Vega-Lahr, Garcia, Poirier, Nystrom & Kuhn, 1986; Gonzalez et al., 2001; Lovic & Fleming, 2004). Touch contributes to our healthy development and physical well-being, but can also be perceived as molesting. In much the same way, hierarchy and social status can be seen as both: an instrument to exploit group members and bring advantage to single individuals","PeriodicalId":91082,"journal":{"name":"Human ethology bulletin","volume":"22 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Human ethology bulletin","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.22330/HE/34/159-172","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Social interactions and hierarchical structures in classrooms are studied in a number of scientific disciplines, yet the complexity of such systems makes them hard to investigate. In the present study we explore the relationship between social status and bodily interaction, since non-verbal communication and touch play a role in most social systems, yet are poorly understood in school settings. We developed a novel approach to assess social status in grammar school students by way of measuring the presence in others’ minds: Classmates assessed their peers in intellectual, social and physical domains. Additionally, we measured the amount and nature of physical interactions among classmates during breaks in the classroom. These interactions were tracked with the help of older, trained and regularly supervised students from the same school. This peer-to-peer method generated large amounts of data over a period of two months, during which 168 students were observed repeatedly. Results show that touching behavior is modulated by social status and sex: The amount of physical interaction with classmates increases significantly with social status. Same sex touching of intimate zones such as breasts, lap and buttocks occur more frequently among individuals of similar status as compared to touching the intimate zones of the opposite sex. The latter involves extremely high and low ranked individuals more often than same-sex interactions. This study helps to understand formative interactions within classrooms and gives rise to new questions on the establishment and maintenance of hierarchies in peer groups. Keywords: social interaction, classroom hierarchy, social touch, physical interaction, social status. _________________________________________________________
 * Paper presented at XXIV Biennial Conference of Human Ethology 2018, in Santiago, Chile. 
 Eder, S.J. & Oberzaucher, E. (2019). Getting in Touch – Social status predicts physical interaction in classrooms. Human Ethology, 34, 159-172. https://doi.org/ 10.22330/he/34/159-172 submitted: 31.Oct. 2018; revised: 12. Aug. 2019; accepted: 20.Aug. 2019 Eder, S.J. & Oberzaucher, E. (2019). Getting in Touch Human Ethology, 34, 159-172 TOUCH, HIERARCHY AND SOCIAL INTERACTIONS IN CLASSROOMS The ambivalence and ambiguity of touches, especially when status differences and dependencies are involved, are publicly debated (Kantor, 2018; Astor, 2018). Recently, this topic has gained attention in the context of classrooms through the initiative of a US-school to ban touches from schools altogether (The Associated Press, 2007). Other schools are merely restricting ‘inappropriate’ touches ( Jones, 2011). It is of little surprise that measures are taken to outlaw unwanted physical interactions, but this might lead to depriving us from essential parts of our social lives: The many functions of touch not only include expression of dominance, but also affiliative interactions, such as social grooming (Lehmann, Korstjens & Dunbar, 2007; Dunbar, 2012). Physical interaction serves a communicative purpose in social interactions. Not only can touch convey emotions (Hertenstein, Holmes, McCullough, & Keltner, 2009; Hertenstein, Keltner, App, Bulleit & Jaskolka, 2006) and reflect social bonds (Suvilehto et al, 2019), it also increases persuasiveness and has positive effects on compliance (Gueguen, Jacob & Boulbry, 2007; Gueguen & Fischer-Lokou, 2003; Guéguen, 2002). Social touch is known to have various physiological benefits such as a decreased heart rate and increased oxytocin levels (Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, & Ehlert, 2003; Henricson, Berglund, Maatta, Ekman & Segesten, 2008; Light, Grewen & Amico, 2005), and it can counteract the negative effects induced by stress, as measured with physiological and biochemical markers such as blood pressure and salivary cortisol (Grewen, Anderson, Girdler, & Light, 2003; Ditzen, Neumann, Bodenmann, von Dawans, Turner, Ehlert & Heinrichs, 2007). This nonverbal comforting effect of touch is more effective than the benefits obtained from verbal social support (Holt-Lunstad, Birmingham & Light, 2008). It appears to be an innate mechanism, as lowered cortisol levels resulting from physical touch can not only be observed in adults, but also in neonatal premature infants (Neu, Laudenslager, & Robinson, 2009). Empirical findings indicate that the positive effects of touch affect both the toucher and the one being touched (Neu et al., 2009). Healthy touching interactions are a sign of secure dyadic attachment: Infants of depressed mothers receive less physical attention and compensate for this with selfstimulation (Herrera, Reissland, & Shepherd, 2004). Ferber and colleagues identified affectionate, stimulating and instrumental types of caregiving touch in humans (Ferber, Feldman & Makhoul, 2008). Caregiving touch is essential for psychological and physiological development: Children deprived of touch show developmental delays i.e. in cognitive skills (MacLean, 2003; Nelson, 2007). Not only humans, but other mammals depend on touch for healthy development, too (Harlow,1958; Gonzalez, Lovic, Ward, Wainwright & Fleming, 2001): Early comparative studies emphasize the importance not only of mother-infant-interactions, but also of peer to peer contact in mammalian development (Harlow, Harlow, Dodsworth & Arling, 1966; Gonzalez et al., 2001). Later experimental studies confirmed that developmental delays can indeed result from missing tactile stimulation (Scafidi, Field, Schanberg, Bauer, Vega-Lahr, Garcia, Poirier, Nystrom & Kuhn, 1986; Gonzalez et al., 2001; Lovic & Fleming, 2004). Touch contributes to our healthy development and physical well-being, but can also be perceived as molesting. In much the same way, hierarchy and social status can be seen as both: an instrument to exploit group members and bring advantage to single individuals
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
接触——社会地位预示着课堂上的身体互动
社会互动和课堂中的等级结构在许多科学学科中得到了研究,然而这些系统的复杂性使得它们难以调查。在本研究中,我们探讨了社会地位和身体互动之间的关系,因为非语言交流和触摸在大多数社会系统中发挥着作用,但在学校环境中却很少被理解。我们开发了一种新颖的方法来评估文法学校学生的社会地位,方法是测量在别人心中的存在:同学们在智力、社会和身体领域评估他们的同龄人。此外,我们还测量了课间休息时同学之间身体互动的数量和性质。这些互动是在来自同一所学校的年龄较大、受过训练并经常受到监督的学生的帮助下进行的。这种点对点的方法在两个月的时间里产生了大量的数据,在此期间,168名学生被反复观察。结果表明,触摸行为受社会地位和性别的调节:与同学的身体互动数量随着社会地位的增加而显著增加。与触摸异性的私密部位相比,同性之间触摸乳房、大腿和臀部等私密部位的行为在地位相似的个体中更为频繁。后者比同性之间的互动更多地涉及到地位极高和地位较低的个体。这项研究有助于理解课堂中的形成性互动,并提出了关于同龄人群体中等级制度的建立和维持的新问题。关键词:社会互动课堂等级社会接触身体互动社会地位_________________________________________________________
 * 论文发表于2018年人类行为学二十四两年一次的会议,在圣地亚哥,智利。
Eder, S.J. & Oberzaucher, E.(2019)。接触——社会地位预示着课堂上的身体互动。动物行为学,34,159-172。https://doi.org/ 10.22330/he/34/159-172提交:10月31日2018;修正:12。2019年8月;接受:20. 8月。2019 Eder, S.J. & Oberzaucher, E.(2019)。接触,人类行为学,34,159-172触摸,等级和课堂上的社会互动。触摸的矛盾心理和模糊性,特别是当涉及到地位差异和依赖时,是公开辩论的(Kantor, 2018;阿斯特,2018)。最近,这一话题在课堂上引起了人们的关注,因为一所美国学校倡议在学校里完全禁止触摸(美联社,2007)。其他学校只是限制“不适当的”触摸(Jones, 2011)。人们采取措施禁止不必要的身体互动,这不足为奇,但这可能会导致我们失去社交生活的重要部分:触摸的许多功能不仅包括支配地位的表达,还包括附属互动,如社交修饰(Lehmann, Korstjens & Dunbar, 2007;邓巴,2012)。身体互动在社会互动中起到沟通的作用。触摸不仅可以传达情感(Hertenstein, Holmes, McCullough, & Keltner, 2009;Hertenstein, Keltner, App, bulletit & Jaskolka, 2006)并反映了社会联系(Suvilehto等人,2019),它还增加了说服力,并对依从性有积极影响(Gueguen, Jacob & Boulbry, 2007;Gueguen & Fischer-Lokou, 2003;Gueguen, 2002)。众所周知,社交接触具有各种生理上的益处,例如降低心率和增加催产素水平(Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, & Ehlert, 2003;Henricson, Berglund, Maatta, Ekman & Segesten, 2008;Light, Grewen & Amico, 2005),通过测量血压和唾液皮质醇等生理生化指标,它可以抵消压力引起的负面影响(Grewen, Anderson, Girdler, & Light, 2003;迪岑,诺伊曼,博登曼,冯·道恩斯,特纳,埃勒特和海因里希斯,2007)。触摸的这种非语言安慰效果比语言社会支持更有效(Holt-Lunstad, Birmingham & Light, 2008)。这似乎是一种先天机制,因为身体接触导致的皮质醇水平降低不仅在成年人中可以观察到,而且在新生儿早产儿中也可以观察到(Neu, Laudenslager, & Robinson, 2009)。实证研究结果表明,触摸的积极效应会影响触摸者和被触摸者(Neu et al., 2009)。健康的触摸互动是安全的双重依恋的标志:抑郁母亲的婴儿得到较少的身体关注,并以自我刺激来弥补这一点(Herrera, Reissland, & Shepherd, 2004)。Ferber和他的同事们确定了人类关爱触摸的深情、刺激和工具性类型(Ferber, Feldman & Makhoul, 2008)。照料性触摸对心理和生理发展至关重要:被剥夺触摸的儿童表现出发育迟缓,即: 认知技能(MacLean, 2003;尼尔森,2007)。不仅是人类,其他哺乳动物也依靠触摸来促进健康发育(Harlow,1958;Gonzalez, Lovic, Ward, Wainwright & Fleming, 2001):早期的比较研究不仅强调了母婴互动的重要性,还强调了哺乳动物发育过程中同伴间接触的重要性(Harlow, Harlow, Dodsworth & Arling, 1966;Gonzalez et al., 2001)。后来的实验研究证实,缺失触觉刺激确实会导致发育迟缓(Scafidi, Field, Schanberg, Bauer, Vega-Lahr, Garcia, Poirier, Nystrom & Kuhn, 1986;Gonzalez et al., 2001;Lovic & Fleming, 2004)。触摸有助于我们的健康发展和身体健康,但也可以被视为骚扰。同样地,等级制度和社会地位可以被看作是两者兼而有之:一种剥削群体成员的工具,一种给个人带来好处的工具
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
ISHE Travels to Amazon: A Narrative of a Special Issue Based on the XXII ISHE Conference, 5-9 August 2014 in Belém, Brazil The Ethologist’s Corner The Trouble With Certainty in the Study of Human Evolution Women at the “Sight” of Evolution A Lost Idea in Psychology: Observation as Starting Point for the Scientific Investigation of Human Behavior
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1