Heart's tale of trauma: Fear-conditioned heart rate changes in post-traumatic stress disorder

IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 PSYCHIATRY Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Pub Date : 2023-08-07 DOI:10.1111/acps.13602
Simone Battaglia, C. Nazzi, J. F. Thayer
{"title":"Heart's tale of trauma: Fear-conditioned heart rate changes in post-traumatic stress disorder","authors":"Simone Battaglia, C. Nazzi, J. F. Thayer","doi":"10.1111/acps.13602","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The study of emotional memories in neuroscience is now more than ever linked to the use of fear conditioning paradigms, which represent a direct and controlled way to study this phenomenon in humans. Repeated pairings of a neutral stimulus with a negative consequence leads to the creation of a transiently threatening entity, which can be used to assess explicit and implicit ratings of fear. In fact, the use of psychophysiological indices (i.e., skin conductance responses, fear potentiated startle, pupillary responses) represents a less biased tool compared with verbal ratings, as they are harder to control voluntarily. Among those, the use of heart rate (HR) responses is on the rise, as it reveals the presence of fear-induced bradycardia, a phenomenon by which a conditioned stimulus elicits a brief cardiac deceleration upon its presentation. This stems from an intricate interplay among the central and the peripheral nervous systems, by means of the vagus nerve which extends to the heart. The neurovisceral integration model of fear (NVI-f) proposes that high level cognitive structures (i.e., prefrontal cortex) influence both the amygdala and the hippocampus, generating neurovisceral responses via projections that govern heart behaviour. Compromised functioning of those high-level structures can, thus, engender aberrant fear conditioning, which is known to lead to the development of different psychiatric disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). As these populations of patients show differences from the norm in their conditioned responses when measured by more widely used techniques, it is possible to assume that the same would be true for HR responses. This hypothesis has been investigated with a particular focus on PTSD patients (see Table 1 for a summary). Here, we aim at discussing in a single work the research endeavours made in this field. Moreover, our goal is to provide an interpretation of the various results taken together. PTSD is tightly related to second-order conditioning, a phenomenon by which a conditioned stimulus (CS) can acquire the properties of an unconditioned stimulus (US) and become a US by itself. Moreover, the affective properties of the CS can be further transferred to a second stimulus. This is actually relevant for these patients since cues related to the traumatic event can serve as a US and negatively impact on everyday life. Accordingly, to study this phenomenon, Wessa and Flor recruited trauma exposed individuals, some of which suffered from PTSD, and healthy controls. They had them undergo a fear conditioning paradigm during which one geometrical shape was followed by a picture of an airplane crash, while the other never was. Crucially, HR results showed no differences between the two CSs, in none of the groups, revealing how fear bradycardia does not emerge in situations where the US causes cognitive distress instead of a painful physical sensation. Similarly, Orr et al recruited trauma exposed individuals to partake in a fear conditioning experiment. Some of the participants satisfied the criteria for PTSD, while others did not develop the condition. Differently from the study by Wessa and Flor, the US consisted of a painful physical sensation, namely a shock delivered to the hand. The results of this experiment revealed that PTSD patients showed greater differential responses to the two CS, while non-PTSD patients did not. More specifically, PTSD patients displayed faster HR to the CS+. The authors suggest that this higher conditionability constitutes an explanation for the development of this disorder. Valuable insights can stem even from studies that consider populations with PTSD-related problems instead of the fully fledged condition. Notably, Costanzo et al focused their study not on patients but on war veterans with subthreshold PTSD. Authors hypothesised that given the characteristics of the disorder, higher symptom severity would be associated with greater conditioned responses during both acquisition and extinction. Therefore, they recruited war veterans and split them in two groups based on symptom levels, forming a low and a high symptom group. They underwent a conditional discrimination paradigm, where three different visual stimuli were presented in pairs to participants. During acquisition, the ‘threat’ pairs were presented with an air blast, and the ‘safety’ pair represented the CS . During the inhibition phase, a pair comprised of a stimulus from both previous pairs was presented to assess the ability to associate a safety signal to a threatening cue. Finally, during the extinction phase, both ‘threat’ and ‘safety’ pairs were presented unreinforced. Results revealed that during the acquisition phase, the high symptom group Received: 8 May 2023 Revised: 7 July 2023 Accepted: 23 July 2023","PeriodicalId":108,"journal":{"name":"Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica","volume":"148 5","pages":"463-466"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3000,"publicationDate":"2023-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"5","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.13602","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHIATRY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5

Abstract

The study of emotional memories in neuroscience is now more than ever linked to the use of fear conditioning paradigms, which represent a direct and controlled way to study this phenomenon in humans. Repeated pairings of a neutral stimulus with a negative consequence leads to the creation of a transiently threatening entity, which can be used to assess explicit and implicit ratings of fear. In fact, the use of psychophysiological indices (i.e., skin conductance responses, fear potentiated startle, pupillary responses) represents a less biased tool compared with verbal ratings, as they are harder to control voluntarily. Among those, the use of heart rate (HR) responses is on the rise, as it reveals the presence of fear-induced bradycardia, a phenomenon by which a conditioned stimulus elicits a brief cardiac deceleration upon its presentation. This stems from an intricate interplay among the central and the peripheral nervous systems, by means of the vagus nerve which extends to the heart. The neurovisceral integration model of fear (NVI-f) proposes that high level cognitive structures (i.e., prefrontal cortex) influence both the amygdala and the hippocampus, generating neurovisceral responses via projections that govern heart behaviour. Compromised functioning of those high-level structures can, thus, engender aberrant fear conditioning, which is known to lead to the development of different psychiatric disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). As these populations of patients show differences from the norm in their conditioned responses when measured by more widely used techniques, it is possible to assume that the same would be true for HR responses. This hypothesis has been investigated with a particular focus on PTSD patients (see Table 1 for a summary). Here, we aim at discussing in a single work the research endeavours made in this field. Moreover, our goal is to provide an interpretation of the various results taken together. PTSD is tightly related to second-order conditioning, a phenomenon by which a conditioned stimulus (CS) can acquire the properties of an unconditioned stimulus (US) and become a US by itself. Moreover, the affective properties of the CS can be further transferred to a second stimulus. This is actually relevant for these patients since cues related to the traumatic event can serve as a US and negatively impact on everyday life. Accordingly, to study this phenomenon, Wessa and Flor recruited trauma exposed individuals, some of which suffered from PTSD, and healthy controls. They had them undergo a fear conditioning paradigm during which one geometrical shape was followed by a picture of an airplane crash, while the other never was. Crucially, HR results showed no differences between the two CSs, in none of the groups, revealing how fear bradycardia does not emerge in situations where the US causes cognitive distress instead of a painful physical sensation. Similarly, Orr et al recruited trauma exposed individuals to partake in a fear conditioning experiment. Some of the participants satisfied the criteria for PTSD, while others did not develop the condition. Differently from the study by Wessa and Flor, the US consisted of a painful physical sensation, namely a shock delivered to the hand. The results of this experiment revealed that PTSD patients showed greater differential responses to the two CS, while non-PTSD patients did not. More specifically, PTSD patients displayed faster HR to the CS+. The authors suggest that this higher conditionability constitutes an explanation for the development of this disorder. Valuable insights can stem even from studies that consider populations with PTSD-related problems instead of the fully fledged condition. Notably, Costanzo et al focused their study not on patients but on war veterans with subthreshold PTSD. Authors hypothesised that given the characteristics of the disorder, higher symptom severity would be associated with greater conditioned responses during both acquisition and extinction. Therefore, they recruited war veterans and split them in two groups based on symptom levels, forming a low and a high symptom group. They underwent a conditional discrimination paradigm, where three different visual stimuli were presented in pairs to participants. During acquisition, the ‘threat’ pairs were presented with an air blast, and the ‘safety’ pair represented the CS . During the inhibition phase, a pair comprised of a stimulus from both previous pairs was presented to assess the ability to associate a safety signal to a threatening cue. Finally, during the extinction phase, both ‘threat’ and ‘safety’ pairs were presented unreinforced. Results revealed that during the acquisition phase, the high symptom group Received: 8 May 2023 Revised: 7 July 2023 Accepted: 23 July 2023

Abstract Image

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
心脏创伤的故事:创伤后应激障碍中恐惧条件下的心率变化。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 医学-精神病学
CiteScore
11.20
自引率
3.00%
发文量
135
审稿时长
6-12 weeks
期刊介绍: Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica acts as an international forum for the dissemination of information advancing the science and practice of psychiatry. In particular we focus on communicating frontline research to clinical psychiatrists and psychiatric researchers. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica has traditionally been and remains a journal focusing predominantly on clinical psychiatry, but translational psychiatry is a topic of growing importance to our readers. Therefore, the journal welcomes submission of manuscripts based on both clinical- and more translational (e.g. preclinical and epidemiological) research. When preparing manuscripts based on translational studies for submission to Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, the authors should place emphasis on the clinical significance of the research question and the findings. Manuscripts based solely on preclinical research (e.g. animal models) are normally not considered for publication in the Journal.
期刊最新文献
Letter to the Editor Concerning "Glucagon-Like Peptide Agonists for Weight Management in Antipsychotic-Induced Weight Gain: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis". Prediction of electroconvulsive therapy outcome: A network analysis approach. How to treat antipsychotic-related weight gain and metabolic disturbances: Is there a role for GLP-1 receptor agonists? Issue Information Variation of subclinical psychosis as a function of population density across different European settings: Findings from the multi-national EU-GEI study.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1