亲子互动的薄片取样定量评价。

IF 1.2 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI:10.1007/s10919-022-00420-7
Romana Burgess, Ilaria Costantini, Marc H Bornstein, Amy Campbell, Miguel A Cordero Vega, Iryna Culpin, Hayley Dingsdale, Rosalind M John, Mari-Rose Kennedy, Hannah R Tyson, Rebecca M Pearson, Ian Nabney
{"title":"亲子互动的薄片取样定量评价。","authors":"Romana Burgess,&nbsp;Ilaria Costantini,&nbsp;Marc H Bornstein,&nbsp;Amy Campbell,&nbsp;Miguel A Cordero Vega,&nbsp;Iryna Culpin,&nbsp;Hayley Dingsdale,&nbsp;Rosalind M John,&nbsp;Mari-Rose Kennedy,&nbsp;Hannah R Tyson,&nbsp;Rebecca M Pearson,&nbsp;Ian Nabney","doi":"10.1007/s10919-022-00420-7","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Behavioural coding is time-intensive and laborious. Thin slice sampling provides an alternative approach, aiming to alleviate the coding burden. However, little is understood about whether different behaviours coded over thin slices are comparable to those same behaviours over entire interactions. To provide quantitative evidence for the value of thin slice sampling for a variety of behaviours. We used data from three populations of parent-infant interactions: mother-infant dyads from the Grown in Wales (GiW) cohort (<i>n</i> = 31), mother-infant dyads from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort (<i>n</i> = 14), and father-infant dyads from the ALSPAC cohort (<i>n</i> = 11). Mean infant ages were 13.8, 6.8, and 7.1 months, respectively. Interactions were coded using a comprehensive coding scheme comprised of 11-14 behavioural groups, with each group comprised of 3-13 mutually exclusive behaviours. We calculated frequencies of verbal and non-verbal behaviours, transition matrices (probability of transitioning between behaviours, e.g., from looking at the infant to looking at a distraction) and stationary distributions (long-term proportion of time spent within behavioural states) for 15 thin slices of full, 5-min interactions. Measures drawn from the full sessions were compared to those from 1-, 2-, 3- and 4-min slices. We identified many instances where thin slice sampling (i.e., < 5 min) was an appropriate coding method, although we observed significant variation across different behaviours. We thereby used this information to provide detailed guidance to researchers regarding how long to code for each behaviour depending on their objectives.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2000,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10163135/pdf/","citationCount":"2","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"A Quantitative Evaluation of Thin Slice Sampling for Parent-Infant Interactions.\",\"authors\":\"Romana Burgess,&nbsp;Ilaria Costantini,&nbsp;Marc H Bornstein,&nbsp;Amy Campbell,&nbsp;Miguel A Cordero Vega,&nbsp;Iryna Culpin,&nbsp;Hayley Dingsdale,&nbsp;Rosalind M John,&nbsp;Mari-Rose Kennedy,&nbsp;Hannah R Tyson,&nbsp;Rebecca M Pearson,&nbsp;Ian Nabney\",\"doi\":\"10.1007/s10919-022-00420-7\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p><p>Behavioural coding is time-intensive and laborious. Thin slice sampling provides an alternative approach, aiming to alleviate the coding burden. However, little is understood about whether different behaviours coded over thin slices are comparable to those same behaviours over entire interactions. To provide quantitative evidence for the value of thin slice sampling for a variety of behaviours. We used data from three populations of parent-infant interactions: mother-infant dyads from the Grown in Wales (GiW) cohort (<i>n</i> = 31), mother-infant dyads from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort (<i>n</i> = 14), and father-infant dyads from the ALSPAC cohort (<i>n</i> = 11). Mean infant ages were 13.8, 6.8, and 7.1 months, respectively. Interactions were coded using a comprehensive coding scheme comprised of 11-14 behavioural groups, with each group comprised of 3-13 mutually exclusive behaviours. We calculated frequencies of verbal and non-verbal behaviours, transition matrices (probability of transitioning between behaviours, e.g., from looking at the infant to looking at a distraction) and stationary distributions (long-term proportion of time spent within behavioural states) for 15 thin slices of full, 5-min interactions. Measures drawn from the full sessions were compared to those from 1-, 2-, 3- and 4-min slices. We identified many instances where thin slice sampling (i.e., < 5 min) was an appropriate coding method, although we observed significant variation across different behaviours. We thereby used this information to provide detailed guidance to researchers regarding how long to code for each behaviour depending on their objectives.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":47747,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.2000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-01-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10163135/pdf/\",\"citationCount\":\"2\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00420-7\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q4\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00420-7","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2

摘要

行为编码是费时费力的。薄切片采样提供了另一种方法,旨在减轻编码负担。然而,对于在薄片上编码的不同行为是否与在整个交互过程中编码的相同行为具有可比性,人们知之甚少。为薄切片取样对多种行为的价值提供定量证据。我们使用了来自三个亲子互动群体的数据:来自威尔士生长(GiW)队列的母婴双体(n = 31),来自雅芳父母与儿童纵向研究(ALSPAC)队列的母婴双体(n = 14),以及来自ALSPAC队列的父子双体(n = 11)。婴儿平均年龄分别为13.8个月、6.8个月和7.1个月。使用由11-14个行为组组成的综合编码方案对交互进行编码,每组由3-13个相互排斥的行为组成。我们计算了15个5分钟完整互动的薄片的语言和非语言行为的频率、转换矩阵(行为之间转换的概率,例如,从看婴儿到看分心物)和平稳分布(在行为状态中花费的长期时间比例)。研究人员将整段时间的测量结果与1分钟、2分钟、3分钟和4分钟的测量结果进行了比较。我们发现了许多例子,其中薄切片采样(即,
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。

摘要图片

摘要图片

摘要图片

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
A Quantitative Evaluation of Thin Slice Sampling for Parent-Infant Interactions.

Behavioural coding is time-intensive and laborious. Thin slice sampling provides an alternative approach, aiming to alleviate the coding burden. However, little is understood about whether different behaviours coded over thin slices are comparable to those same behaviours over entire interactions. To provide quantitative evidence for the value of thin slice sampling for a variety of behaviours. We used data from three populations of parent-infant interactions: mother-infant dyads from the Grown in Wales (GiW) cohort (n = 31), mother-infant dyads from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort (n = 14), and father-infant dyads from the ALSPAC cohort (n = 11). Mean infant ages were 13.8, 6.8, and 7.1 months, respectively. Interactions were coded using a comprehensive coding scheme comprised of 11-14 behavioural groups, with each group comprised of 3-13 mutually exclusive behaviours. We calculated frequencies of verbal and non-verbal behaviours, transition matrices (probability of transitioning between behaviours, e.g., from looking at the infant to looking at a distraction) and stationary distributions (long-term proportion of time spent within behavioural states) for 15 thin slices of full, 5-min interactions. Measures drawn from the full sessions were compared to those from 1-, 2-, 3- and 4-min slices. We identified many instances where thin slice sampling (i.e., < 5 min) was an appropriate coding method, although we observed significant variation across different behaviours. We thereby used this information to provide detailed guidance to researchers regarding how long to code for each behaviour depending on their objectives.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL-
CiteScore
4.80
自引率
9.50%
发文量
27
期刊介绍: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior presents peer-reviewed original theoretical and empirical research on all major areas of nonverbal behavior. Specific topics include paralanguage, proxemics, facial expressions, eye contact, face-to-face interaction, and nonverbal emotional expression, as well as other subjects which contribute to the scientific understanding of nonverbal processes and behavior.
期刊最新文献
Machine Learning Predicts Accuracy in Eyewitnesses’ Voices The Expression of Vocal Emotions in Cognitively Healthy Adult Speakers: Impact of Emotion Category, Gender, and Age The Effect of Face Masks and Sunglasses on Emotion Perception over Two Years of the COVID-19 Pandemic The Digital Witness: Exploring Gestural Misinformation in Tele-Forensic Interviews with 5-8-Year-Old Children Perceptions of mate poaching predict jealousy towards higher-pitched women’s voices
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1