{"title":"失明和失聪:研究数字感觉的视觉和语言基础的窗口","authors":"M. Buyle, C. Marlair, Virginie Crollen","doi":"10.1515/9783110661941-014","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"When children acquire numerical skills, they have to learn a variety of specific numerical tools. The most obvious are the numerical codes such as number words (one, two, three, etc.) or Arabic numerals (1, 2, 3, etc.). Other skills will be relatively more abstract: arithmetical facts (i.e., 4 × 2 = 8), arithmetical procedures (i.e., borrowing), or arithmetical laws (i.e., a + b = b + a). The acquisition of these numerical tools is complex and probably not facilitated by the fact that a numerical expression does not have a single meaning. Indeed, numbers can be used as a kind of label or proper name (i.e., Bus 51). They can also be part of a familiar fixed sequence (i.e., 51 comes immediately after 50 and before 52). They can be used to refer to continuous analogue quantities (i.e., 51,2 grams) (Butterworth, 2005; Fuson, 1988) and, most importantly, they can be used to denote the number of things in a set – the cardinality of the set. Children are able to understand the special meaning of cardinality because they possess a specific and innate capacity for dealing with quantities (Feigenson et al., 2004). Supporting the innate nature of the “number sense,” it has been found, for instance, that fetuses in the last trimester are already able to discriminate auditory numerical quantities (Schleger et al., 2014). A large set of behavioral studies using the classic method of habituation has also revealed sensitivity to small numerosities (e.g., Starkey & Cooper, 1980) in young children. In the study of Starkey and Cooper (1980), for example, slides with a fixed number of 2 dots were repeatedly presented to 4to 6-month-old infants until their looking time decreased, indicating habituation. At that point, a slide with a deviant number of 3 dots was presented and yielded significantly longer looking times, indicating dishabituation and therefore discrimination between the numerosities 2 and 3. This effect was replicated with newborns (Antell & Keating, 1983) and with various stimuli such as sets of realistic objects (Strauss & Curtis, 1981), targets in motion (Van Loosbroek & Smitsman, 1990; Wynn et al., 2002), twoand threesyllable words (Bijeljac-Babic et al., 1991), or puppet making two or three sequential jumps (Wood & Spelke, 2005; Wynn, 1996). However, it was not observed","PeriodicalId":345296,"journal":{"name":"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning","volume":"43 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2021-05-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"3","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Blindness and deafness: A window to study the visual and verbal basis of the number sense\",\"authors\":\"M. Buyle, C. Marlair, Virginie Crollen\",\"doi\":\"10.1515/9783110661941-014\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"When children acquire numerical skills, they have to learn a variety of specific numerical tools. The most obvious are the numerical codes such as number words (one, two, three, etc.) or Arabic numerals (1, 2, 3, etc.). Other skills will be relatively more abstract: arithmetical facts (i.e., 4 × 2 = 8), arithmetical procedures (i.e., borrowing), or arithmetical laws (i.e., a + b = b + a). The acquisition of these numerical tools is complex and probably not facilitated by the fact that a numerical expression does not have a single meaning. Indeed, numbers can be used as a kind of label or proper name (i.e., Bus 51). They can also be part of a familiar fixed sequence (i.e., 51 comes immediately after 50 and before 52). They can be used to refer to continuous analogue quantities (i.e., 51,2 grams) (Butterworth, 2005; Fuson, 1988) and, most importantly, they can be used to denote the number of things in a set – the cardinality of the set. Children are able to understand the special meaning of cardinality because they possess a specific and innate capacity for dealing with quantities (Feigenson et al., 2004). Supporting the innate nature of the “number sense,” it has been found, for instance, that fetuses in the last trimester are already able to discriminate auditory numerical quantities (Schleger et al., 2014). A large set of behavioral studies using the classic method of habituation has also revealed sensitivity to small numerosities (e.g., Starkey & Cooper, 1980) in young children. In the study of Starkey and Cooper (1980), for example, slides with a fixed number of 2 dots were repeatedly presented to 4to 6-month-old infants until their looking time decreased, indicating habituation. At that point, a slide with a deviant number of 3 dots was presented and yielded significantly longer looking times, indicating dishabituation and therefore discrimination between the numerosities 2 and 3. This effect was replicated with newborns (Antell & Keating, 1983) and with various stimuli such as sets of realistic objects (Strauss & Curtis, 1981), targets in motion (Van Loosbroek & Smitsman, 1990; Wynn et al., 2002), twoand threesyllable words (Bijeljac-Babic et al., 1991), or puppet making two or three sequential jumps (Wood & Spelke, 2005; Wynn, 1996). However, it was not observed\",\"PeriodicalId\":345296,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning\",\"volume\":\"43 1\",\"pages\":\"0\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2021-05-25\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"3\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110661941-014\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110661941-014","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
摘要
当孩子们获得数字技能时,他们必须学习各种具体的数字工具。最明显的是数字编码,如数字单词(1、2、3等)或阿拉伯数字(1、2、3等)。其他技能将相对更加抽象:算术事实(即4 × 2 = 8),算术过程(即借用)或算术定律(即a + b = b + a)。这些数值工具的获得是复杂的,并且可能不会因为数值表达式没有单一含义而变得容易。事实上,数字可以用作一种标签或专有名称(例如,总线51)。它们也可以是一个熟悉的固定序列的一部分(例如,51紧接在50之后,52之前)。它们可以用来指连续的模拟量(即51.2克)(Butterworth, 2005;Fuson, 1988),最重要的是,它们可以用来表示集合中事物的数量——集合的基数。儿童能够理解基数的特殊含义,因为他们拥有处理数量的特定和天生的能力(Feigenson et al., 2004)。例如,研究发现,在最后三个月的胎儿已经能够区分听觉数字数量,这支持了“数字感”的先天本质(Schleger et al., 2014)。大量使用经典习惯化方法的行为研究也揭示了幼儿对小数字的敏感性(例如,Starkey & Cooper, 1980)。例如,在Starkey和Cooper(1980)的研究中,他们反复向4 - 6个月大的婴儿展示固定数量的2个点的幻灯片,直到他们看的时间减少,这表明他们已经习惯了。在这一点上,出现了一个带有3个偏差点的幻灯片,并且产生了明显更长的观看时间,这表明不习惯,因此区分了数字2和3。这种效应在新生儿(Antell & Keating, 1983)和各种刺激(如一系列现实物体(Strauss & Curtis, 1981)、运动中的目标(Van Loosbroek & Smitsman, 1990;Wynn et al., 2002),二音节和三音节的单词(bijeljack - babic et al., 1991),或者木偶连续跳两到三次(Wood & Spelke, 2005;永利,1996)。然而,它没有被观察到
Blindness and deafness: A window to study the visual and verbal basis of the number sense
When children acquire numerical skills, they have to learn a variety of specific numerical tools. The most obvious are the numerical codes such as number words (one, two, three, etc.) or Arabic numerals (1, 2, 3, etc.). Other skills will be relatively more abstract: arithmetical facts (i.e., 4 × 2 = 8), arithmetical procedures (i.e., borrowing), or arithmetical laws (i.e., a + b = b + a). The acquisition of these numerical tools is complex and probably not facilitated by the fact that a numerical expression does not have a single meaning. Indeed, numbers can be used as a kind of label or proper name (i.e., Bus 51). They can also be part of a familiar fixed sequence (i.e., 51 comes immediately after 50 and before 52). They can be used to refer to continuous analogue quantities (i.e., 51,2 grams) (Butterworth, 2005; Fuson, 1988) and, most importantly, they can be used to denote the number of things in a set – the cardinality of the set. Children are able to understand the special meaning of cardinality because they possess a specific and innate capacity for dealing with quantities (Feigenson et al., 2004). Supporting the innate nature of the “number sense,” it has been found, for instance, that fetuses in the last trimester are already able to discriminate auditory numerical quantities (Schleger et al., 2014). A large set of behavioral studies using the classic method of habituation has also revealed sensitivity to small numerosities (e.g., Starkey & Cooper, 1980) in young children. In the study of Starkey and Cooper (1980), for example, slides with a fixed number of 2 dots were repeatedly presented to 4to 6-month-old infants until their looking time decreased, indicating habituation. At that point, a slide with a deviant number of 3 dots was presented and yielded significantly longer looking times, indicating dishabituation and therefore discrimination between the numerosities 2 and 3. This effect was replicated with newborns (Antell & Keating, 1983) and with various stimuli such as sets of realistic objects (Strauss & Curtis, 1981), targets in motion (Van Loosbroek & Smitsman, 1990; Wynn et al., 2002), twoand threesyllable words (Bijeljac-Babic et al., 1991), or puppet making two or three sequential jumps (Wood & Spelke, 2005; Wynn, 1996). However, it was not observed