第一语言和第二语言的精确数字表示

Helga Klein
{"title":"第一语言和第二语言的精确数字表示","authors":"Helga Klein","doi":"10.1515/9783110661941-010","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"One of the major questions in the field of cognitive psychology is the extent to which our thought is dependent on, or formed by, the language we speak. In the mid-1900s, proponents of the linguistic relativity principle claimed that different languages with distinct grammatical properties and lexicons would have a major impact on the way the native speakers of that language perceived reality. This idea was based on the work of the anthropologists Sapir (1949), and Whorf (1956), and named the “Sapir-Whorf-Hypothesis” by Hoijer (1971). The opposite view is expressed by the theory of cultural universality (Au, 1983), meaning that basic concepts innate to human beings can be found in every culture irrespective of linguistic differences. The concept of number seems to be a good example for a theory of cultural universality at first sight, as all known cultures have developed at least some number words, and even pre-verbal infants and animals are able to single out the larger of two sets based on the respective number of items. The term “numerosity” was used by Dehaene (1997) for the awareness of quantity. Yet, it is still not clear whether nature has provided us with the concept of exact number or if this is a cultural acquirement based on the acquisition of verbal counting procedures. This chapter will review evidence supporting the language relativity hypothesis for the instance of exact number representations in a small number range (up to 10); other chapters in this book focus on the linguistic specificities of multi-digit number word systems and other aspects of mathematics Bahnmüller, this volume; Dowker, this volume). Presenting studies from different fields, this chapter will propose that the concept of exact numerosity is based on natural language, and furthermore that linguistic specificities even put constraints on the form of exact numerosity representations. The first focus is on the finding that grammatical properties shape the development of the concepts for one versus two, three, and more. Second, studies that describe a representational change in adults who learn a new number word system (including symbols for numerosities higher than four or five) will be presented. Third, differences in arithmetic fact retrieval in both first and second language will be reviewed. These findings will be discussed in the light of the “access-deficit-hypothesis” regarding developmental dyscalculia, suggesting that children with mathematical difficulties may have a problem in accessing number magnitude from symbols (e.g., presenting with longer response times","PeriodicalId":345296,"journal":{"name":"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning","volume":"24 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2021-05-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Exact number representations in first and second language\",\"authors\":\"Helga Klein\",\"doi\":\"10.1515/9783110661941-010\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"One of the major questions in the field of cognitive psychology is the extent to which our thought is dependent on, or formed by, the language we speak. In the mid-1900s, proponents of the linguistic relativity principle claimed that different languages with distinct grammatical properties and lexicons would have a major impact on the way the native speakers of that language perceived reality. This idea was based on the work of the anthropologists Sapir (1949), and Whorf (1956), and named the “Sapir-Whorf-Hypothesis” by Hoijer (1971). The opposite view is expressed by the theory of cultural universality (Au, 1983), meaning that basic concepts innate to human beings can be found in every culture irrespective of linguistic differences. The concept of number seems to be a good example for a theory of cultural universality at first sight, as all known cultures have developed at least some number words, and even pre-verbal infants and animals are able to single out the larger of two sets based on the respective number of items. The term “numerosity” was used by Dehaene (1997) for the awareness of quantity. Yet, it is still not clear whether nature has provided us with the concept of exact number or if this is a cultural acquirement based on the acquisition of verbal counting procedures. This chapter will review evidence supporting the language relativity hypothesis for the instance of exact number representations in a small number range (up to 10); other chapters in this book focus on the linguistic specificities of multi-digit number word systems and other aspects of mathematics Bahnmüller, this volume; Dowker, this volume). Presenting studies from different fields, this chapter will propose that the concept of exact numerosity is based on natural language, and furthermore that linguistic specificities even put constraints on the form of exact numerosity representations. The first focus is on the finding that grammatical properties shape the development of the concepts for one versus two, three, and more. Second, studies that describe a representational change in adults who learn a new number word system (including symbols for numerosities higher than four or five) will be presented. Third, differences in arithmetic fact retrieval in both first and second language will be reviewed. These findings will be discussed in the light of the “access-deficit-hypothesis” regarding developmental dyscalculia, suggesting that children with mathematical difficulties may have a problem in accessing number magnitude from symbols (e.g., presenting with longer response times\",\"PeriodicalId\":345296,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning\",\"volume\":\"24 1\",\"pages\":\"0\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2021-05-25\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110661941-010\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Diversity Dimensions in Mathematics and Language Learning","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110661941-010","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

认知心理学领域的一个主要问题是,我们的思想在多大程度上依赖于我们所说的语言,或由我们所说的语言形成。在20世纪中期,语言相对性原理的支持者声称,具有不同语法特性和词汇的不同语言,会对以该语言为母语的人感知现实的方式产生重大影响。这一观点是基于人类学家Sapir(1949)和Whorf(1956)的研究成果,Hoijer(1971)将其命名为“Sapir-Whorf假说”。文化普遍性理论(Au, 1983)表达了相反的观点,这意味着无论语言差异如何,人类固有的基本概念都可以在每种文化中找到。乍一看,数字的概念似乎是文化普遍性理论的一个很好的例子,因为所有已知的文化都至少发展了一些数字词,甚至没有语言的婴儿和动物都能够根据各自的项目数量从两组中挑出较大的一个。Dehaene(1997)使用“numerosity”一词来表示对数量的认识。然而,我们仍然不清楚究竟是大自然赋予了我们精确数字的概念,还是这是一种基于语言计数程序习得的文化习得。本章将回顾支持语言相对性假设的证据,以在小范围内(最多10个)的精确数字表示为例;本书的其他章节侧重于多数字词系统和数学的其他方面的语言特性bahnmller,本卷;道克,本卷)。通过介绍不同领域的研究,本章将提出精确数字的概念是基于自然语言的,而且语言的特殊性甚至限制了精确数字表示的形式。第一个重点是发现语法属性塑造了“一”与“二”、“三”等概念的发展。第二,研究描述了学习新的数字单词系统(包括高于4或5的数字符号)的成年人的表征变化。第三,回顾第一语言和第二语言在算术事实检索方面的差异。这些发现将在发展性计算障碍的“获取缺陷假说”的基础上进行讨论,该假说认为,有数学困难的儿童可能在从符号获取数字大小方面存在问题(例如,表现出较长的反应时间)
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Exact number representations in first and second language
One of the major questions in the field of cognitive psychology is the extent to which our thought is dependent on, or formed by, the language we speak. In the mid-1900s, proponents of the linguistic relativity principle claimed that different languages with distinct grammatical properties and lexicons would have a major impact on the way the native speakers of that language perceived reality. This idea was based on the work of the anthropologists Sapir (1949), and Whorf (1956), and named the “Sapir-Whorf-Hypothesis” by Hoijer (1971). The opposite view is expressed by the theory of cultural universality (Au, 1983), meaning that basic concepts innate to human beings can be found in every culture irrespective of linguistic differences. The concept of number seems to be a good example for a theory of cultural universality at first sight, as all known cultures have developed at least some number words, and even pre-verbal infants and animals are able to single out the larger of two sets based on the respective number of items. The term “numerosity” was used by Dehaene (1997) for the awareness of quantity. Yet, it is still not clear whether nature has provided us with the concept of exact number or if this is a cultural acquirement based on the acquisition of verbal counting procedures. This chapter will review evidence supporting the language relativity hypothesis for the instance of exact number representations in a small number range (up to 10); other chapters in this book focus on the linguistic specificities of multi-digit number word systems and other aspects of mathematics Bahnmüller, this volume; Dowker, this volume). Presenting studies from different fields, this chapter will propose that the concept of exact numerosity is based on natural language, and furthermore that linguistic specificities even put constraints on the form of exact numerosity representations. The first focus is on the finding that grammatical properties shape the development of the concepts for one versus two, three, and more. Second, studies that describe a representational change in adults who learn a new number word system (including symbols for numerosities higher than four or five) will be presented. Third, differences in arithmetic fact retrieval in both first and second language will be reviewed. These findings will be discussed in the light of the “access-deficit-hypothesis” regarding developmental dyscalculia, suggesting that children with mathematical difficulties may have a problem in accessing number magnitude from symbols (e.g., presenting with longer response times
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
Disentangling the relationship between mathematical learning disability and second-language acquisition Culture and language: How do these influence arithmetic? Language issues in mathematics word problems for English learners Blindness and deafness: A window to study the visual and verbal basis of the number sense Exact number representations in first and second language
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1