{"title":"进行实验:一般原则","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/9781108557191.007","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"When scientists speak of experiments, in the strictest sense of the word, they mean a true experiment [1], where the scientist controls [2] all of the factors and conditions. Real world observations, and case studies [3], should be referred to as observational research [4], rather than experiments. For example, observing animals in the wild is not a true experiment, because it does not isolate and manipulate an independent variable [5].","PeriodicalId":305916,"journal":{"name":"Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology","volume":"84 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2018-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Conducting an Experiment: General Principles\",\"authors\":\"\",\"doi\":\"10.1017/9781108557191.007\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"When scientists speak of experiments, in the strictest sense of the word, they mean a true experiment [1], where the scientist controls [2] all of the factors and conditions. Real world observations, and case studies [3], should be referred to as observational research [4], rather than experiments. For example, observing animals in the wild is not a true experiment, because it does not isolate and manipulate an independent variable [5].\",\"PeriodicalId\":305916,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology\",\"volume\":\"84 1\",\"pages\":\"0\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2018-11-22\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108557191.007\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108557191.007","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
When scientists speak of experiments, in the strictest sense of the word, they mean a true experiment [1], where the scientist controls [2] all of the factors and conditions. Real world observations, and case studies [3], should be referred to as observational research [4], rather than experiments. For example, observing animals in the wild is not a true experiment, because it does not isolate and manipulate an independent variable [5].