成为一个知者:通过合作制造知识

IF 1.4 2区 哲学 Q1 HISTORY & PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE Social Epistemology Pub Date : 2023-11-09 DOI:10.1080/02691728.2023.2266716
Marie-Theres Fester-Seeger
{"title":"成为一个知者:通过合作制造知识","authors":"Marie-Theres Fester-Seeger","doi":"10.1080/02691728.2023.2266716","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACTThis paper takes a step back from considering expertise as a social phenomenon. One should investigate how people become knowers before assigning expertise to a person’s actions. Using a temporal-sensitive systemic ethnography, a case study shows how undergraduate students form a social system out of necessity as they fabricate knowledge around an empty wording like ‘conscious living’. Tracing the engagement with students and tutor to recursive moments of coaction, I argue that, through the subtleties of bodily movements, people incorporate the actions of others as they become knowers. Knowing for a person solidifies as they imbue concepts, terms, facts, etc. with their own understanding. While coaction refers to the interlocking of actions in a specific moment, the fabrication of knowledge resides in temporally distributed moments of coaction where students deliberately incorporate and build on past occurrences in a present moment. In so doing, people cannot be separated from their systemic embedding. Linking coaction with systemic cognition, people fabricate knowledge within wider systemic structures. Within these boundaries, knowers come to fabricate knowledge for themselves and a wider system. Thus, knowing must be seen as an active, embodied, dialogical and multiscalar activity.KEYWORDS: Languagingcoactionsystemic cognitionembodiment AcknowledgmentsThis paper benefited greatly from the comments of two anonymous reviewers. I would also like to thank Stephen Cowley for his very valuable comments and for the insightful conversations about this paper. I also would like to thank the two editors of this special issue, Sarah Bro Trasmundi and Charlie Lassiter, for their patience and kindness and for making this special issue happen. My special thanks go to Charlie Lassiter for his invaluable feedback and help on this paper. A heartfelt thanks go to the research participants who consented to this study. Without them, this paper could not have been written.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1. The written language bias emerges from Linell’s (Citation1982) observation that ‘[o]ur conception of linguistic behavior is biased by a tendency to treat processes, activities, and conditions on them in terms of object-like, static, autonomous and permanent structures, i.e. as if they shared such properties with written characters, words, texts, pictures and images’ (1). Pointing towards the bias of treating ‘natural’ language in terms of its written forms – that is, as ‘thing’-like entities – greatly excludes how human beings bring about language. Consequently, rather than acknowledging the heterogeneous character of language, linguists (and beyond) assign fixed and stable meanings to lexical items, view language as homogeneous systems, and think about language use in terms of acting with stable structures (Linell Citation2019). Hence, traditional linguistics dismiss how people, through gaze, gestures and other bodily dynamics, engage in linguistic activity together (see Cowley Citation2010; Thibault Citation2011). Thus, the written language bias as a notion reminds scholars not to view language as an autonomous system and stable entity in itself but as something that emerges, first and foremost, from human bodily engagement with other people and things in and with the world.2. As my concern broadly is with how the actions of others change personhood in the long run, I find much value in Wegner and Sparrow’s (Citation2007) notion of coaction as it grants attention to how people incorporate the actions of others in their actions. This leads to blurring the lines of authorship in one’s actions. For me, there is, therefore, a difference between ‘joint action’ and ‘coaction’. While ‘joint action’ might presuppose ‘coaction’, in my view, ‘joint action’ alludes to how people together achieve a common goal paired with we-intentions (Fiebich and Gallagher Citation2013). The notion, thus, focuses on the activity in itself and what happens between people but not on how people build on the actions of others and how it induces a change in a person.3. Emphasizing that language is embodied activity before it is symbolic, Cowley defines wordings as ‘readily repeated aspects of vocalizations that, for speakers of a community, carry historically derived information’ (Cowley Citation2011, 186). Wordings differ from ‘words’ as the notion highlights that in languaging, people phenomenologically draw on stabilized patterns of articulatory gestures rather than on abstract objects in a language system (as highlighted in the Written Language Bias) (Cowley and Harvey Citation2016). The notion of wording fits the multiscalar character of languaging: bodily dynamics occurring on fast bodily time scales mesh with the slower sociocultural scales of a community. A person’s past experience of engaging in a specific linguistic community enables them to react to and act upon specific patterns of articulatory movements.4. I am certain that many of us have stumbled upon an old textbook or folder from their past student days, as I have the other day. Looking at my old notes immediately evoked instances of talking and discussing in class with teachers and co-students. It brought about past moments of engaging with others, now reified in my own writing.5. Systemic cognition (Cowley and Vallée-Tourangeau Citation2013) highlights the role of a person – as a human cognitive agent (Giere Citation2004) – in wider systems. Giving due weight to how a person is embedded in and brings forth systemic structures, the view calls attention to a person’s actions within a system (e.g. in a work environment or a sports team). I will present the view in more detail later.6. The term ‘fabricating’ emerged during vivid discussions with Stephen Cowley on a first draft of the manuscript, who rightly saw that the students under investigation do not display expertise but engage in moments of constructing knowledge for themselves and others.7. From a biological view, Maturana uses ‘consensual’ to describe how organisms interacting with each other do so as ‘they are exposed to similar sensory stimuli in the same physical environment’ (Kravchenko Citation2007, 652). In doing so, Maturana emphasizes the role of the immediate environment on an organism’s actions.8. The pico-scale comprises bodily dynamics that last from milliseconds to tenths of seconds, the microscale of bodily dynamics traces ‘saying and doings’ that can be measured from tenths of seconds to seconds’ and, finally, the enchro-scale enables one to trace situated social events that can be measured in seconds, minutes, etc. (Thibault Citation2011, 2015–216).9. Järviletho (Citation1998) introduces the theory of organism-environment system to counteract a ‘two systems’ view that clearly separates the organism from the environment. In asking, when drinking from a cup of coffee, whether the cup belongs to the organism or the environment, Järviletho visualizes how organism and environment are inseparable. Thus, behavior belongs to the organism-environment system and cannot be treated as a mediator between two systems. An organism-environment system is the result of behavior. In emphasizing the inseparability of organism and environment, he stresses that ‘all parts of the system are active in relation to the result’ (330) – observable behavior. In this view, pressing a button of an elevator, for example, does not depend on responding to a perceived stimuli, thus cannot only be attributed to an individual mind but to a person engaging with their immediate environment and acting within wider supra-structural cultural constraints. Knowing when to press the button, what it does and its functions emerges not only from past engagement with others but also from one’s cultural embedding. Mental activity, therefore, cannot be located in an organism but must be attributed to the whole system. The theory informs systemic cognition as it allows for acknowledging how non-local constraints (e.g. culture, autobiographical events, etc.) affect human cognitive action.10. Built on constructivist ideas, this form of instruction leads with the premise ‘to construct knowledge by solving problems’ (Blumenfeld et al. Citation2000, 150). With the focus on ‘real-world problems’, students are given a driving question around a particular topic. Students learn and develop new ideas in groups and ‘represent knowledge around the driving question’ (Blumenfeld et al. Citation2000).11. The group consists of four people, while Paula, Gaby and Vincent formed the core group. Anna, as the fourth member, only irregularly joined the group meetings. The course took place twice a week, while the second day was allocated for group work. The group only managed to meet in class (although attempts were made to meet outside of class), and only twice during the 7 weeks of filming has the whole group met. Hence, the theme of ‘absence’ and ‘irregularity’ greatly determined the group’s work. The four people struggled to form a group as each person wanted to work alone and had different interests. Paula’s interest was in cooking, Gaby had an interest in Korean pop music, Vincent in creating YouTube videos and Anna in baking bread. They faced the challenge of combining all four themes.12. See detailed transcript in appendix.13. The recordings were made with several cameras in the classroom. All students were in class together to discuss their projects. Hence, sound overlaps of all the group discussions and the talk of each group is, therefore, partly unintelligible. Instances that were impossible to transcribe, I marked as ((unintelligible)).14. For Cowley and Harvey (Citation2016), wordings are nonce events ‘that are perceived and construed in relation to a person’s sociocultural experience’ (58). They stress that speakers cannot produce the same sounds, but rather they produce an acoustic pattern that is treated as ‘the same’. In this logic, everything we acoustically produce is a nonce event. Only when we phenomenological act on what we hear can these actions have a normative effect.15. Please find a more detailed transcript in the appendix.16. Kendon (Citation2004) distinguishes two gesture families of the open hand: Open Hand Supine (‘palm up’) and Open Hand Prone (palm down). These gestures take on a pragmatic function in conversations. For Kendon, the former (palm up) relates to contexts of negating or interrupting while the latter to contexts of serving or offering. As seen in the current instance, none of the two aspects are happening in this moment of coordination. Instead, the gestures take over a cognitive function: they allow the students to spatio-temporally anchor past events together as they establish a project and create knowledge for themselves.17. This is contrary to traditional linguistic approaches which relegate gestures and other bodily dynamics to paralinguistic cues and, thus, not essential to linguistics. In adopting the languaging perspective, this ethnography treats such paralinguistic cues as essential and being on the same level of importance as the analysis has shown.Additional informationFundingParticipating students in this study were given a 3% extra credit for this class and, additionally, received an extra $20 for their participation. The amount was supported by a grant from the National Natural Science Foundation of China [Grants No. 31571141 and No. 31628010] in collaboration with South China Normal University. The research has been approved as exempt by the Research Compliance Human Studies Program at the American host university where the data collection was conducted. My Ph.D. research was funded by the Department of Language and Communication at the University of Southern Denmark in Odense, Denmark. The writing of this article took place during my work as a postdoctoral fellow at the Postdoc Network Brandenburg, which granted me an individual grant for my research.Notes on contributorsMarie-Theres Fester-SeegerMarie-Theres Fester-Seeger is a postdoctoral fellow at the European University of Viadrina (Frankfurt (Oder), Germany). She received her PhD at the Department of Language and Communication at the University of Southern Denmark in 2021. Her research interests include distributed language/languaging, multiscalar temporal cognition, systemic views on cognition and dialogical approaches to language. She is particularly interested in how human engagement with a direct Other determines human perception, action and thinking. Interested in human lived experience and temporality, she investigates how people are able to perceive and act upon what is not directly present and how this contributes to human becoming. On the grounds of that, she developed the idea of human presencing in her PhD. She received an individual grant from the Postdoc Network Brandenburg and currently investigates human engagement with digital voice assistants in their home environments.","PeriodicalId":51614,"journal":{"name":"Social Epistemology","volume":" 5","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4000,"publicationDate":"2023-11-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Becoming a Knower: Fabricating Knowing Through Coaction\",\"authors\":\"Marie-Theres Fester-Seeger\",\"doi\":\"10.1080/02691728.2023.2266716\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"ABSTRACTThis paper takes a step back from considering expertise as a social phenomenon. One should investigate how people become knowers before assigning expertise to a person’s actions. Using a temporal-sensitive systemic ethnography, a case study shows how undergraduate students form a social system out of necessity as they fabricate knowledge around an empty wording like ‘conscious living’. Tracing the engagement with students and tutor to recursive moments of coaction, I argue that, through the subtleties of bodily movements, people incorporate the actions of others as they become knowers. Knowing for a person solidifies as they imbue concepts, terms, facts, etc. with their own understanding. While coaction refers to the interlocking of actions in a specific moment, the fabrication of knowledge resides in temporally distributed moments of coaction where students deliberately incorporate and build on past occurrences in a present moment. In so doing, people cannot be separated from their systemic embedding. Linking coaction with systemic cognition, people fabricate knowledge within wider systemic structures. Within these boundaries, knowers come to fabricate knowledge for themselves and a wider system. Thus, knowing must be seen as an active, embodied, dialogical and multiscalar activity.KEYWORDS: Languagingcoactionsystemic cognitionembodiment AcknowledgmentsThis paper benefited greatly from the comments of two anonymous reviewers. I would also like to thank Stephen Cowley for his very valuable comments and for the insightful conversations about this paper. I also would like to thank the two editors of this special issue, Sarah Bro Trasmundi and Charlie Lassiter, for their patience and kindness and for making this special issue happen. My special thanks go to Charlie Lassiter for his invaluable feedback and help on this paper. A heartfelt thanks go to the research participants who consented to this study. Without them, this paper could not have been written.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1. The written language bias emerges from Linell’s (Citation1982) observation that ‘[o]ur conception of linguistic behavior is biased by a tendency to treat processes, activities, and conditions on them in terms of object-like, static, autonomous and permanent structures, i.e. as if they shared such properties with written characters, words, texts, pictures and images’ (1). Pointing towards the bias of treating ‘natural’ language in terms of its written forms – that is, as ‘thing’-like entities – greatly excludes how human beings bring about language. Consequently, rather than acknowledging the heterogeneous character of language, linguists (and beyond) assign fixed and stable meanings to lexical items, view language as homogeneous systems, and think about language use in terms of acting with stable structures (Linell Citation2019). Hence, traditional linguistics dismiss how people, through gaze, gestures and other bodily dynamics, engage in linguistic activity together (see Cowley Citation2010; Thibault Citation2011). Thus, the written language bias as a notion reminds scholars not to view language as an autonomous system and stable entity in itself but as something that emerges, first and foremost, from human bodily engagement with other people and things in and with the world.2. As my concern broadly is with how the actions of others change personhood in the long run, I find much value in Wegner and Sparrow’s (Citation2007) notion of coaction as it grants attention to how people incorporate the actions of others in their actions. This leads to blurring the lines of authorship in one’s actions. For me, there is, therefore, a difference between ‘joint action’ and ‘coaction’. While ‘joint action’ might presuppose ‘coaction’, in my view, ‘joint action’ alludes to how people together achieve a common goal paired with we-intentions (Fiebich and Gallagher Citation2013). The notion, thus, focuses on the activity in itself and what happens between people but not on how people build on the actions of others and how it induces a change in a person.3. Emphasizing that language is embodied activity before it is symbolic, Cowley defines wordings as ‘readily repeated aspects of vocalizations that, for speakers of a community, carry historically derived information’ (Cowley Citation2011, 186). Wordings differ from ‘words’ as the notion highlights that in languaging, people phenomenologically draw on stabilized patterns of articulatory gestures rather than on abstract objects in a language system (as highlighted in the Written Language Bias) (Cowley and Harvey Citation2016). The notion of wording fits the multiscalar character of languaging: bodily dynamics occurring on fast bodily time scales mesh with the slower sociocultural scales of a community. A person’s past experience of engaging in a specific linguistic community enables them to react to and act upon specific patterns of articulatory movements.4. I am certain that many of us have stumbled upon an old textbook or folder from their past student days, as I have the other day. Looking at my old notes immediately evoked instances of talking and discussing in class with teachers and co-students. It brought about past moments of engaging with others, now reified in my own writing.5. Systemic cognition (Cowley and Vallée-Tourangeau Citation2013) highlights the role of a person – as a human cognitive agent (Giere Citation2004) – in wider systems. Giving due weight to how a person is embedded in and brings forth systemic structures, the view calls attention to a person’s actions within a system (e.g. in a work environment or a sports team). I will present the view in more detail later.6. The term ‘fabricating’ emerged during vivid discussions with Stephen Cowley on a first draft of the manuscript, who rightly saw that the students under investigation do not display expertise but engage in moments of constructing knowledge for themselves and others.7. From a biological view, Maturana uses ‘consensual’ to describe how organisms interacting with each other do so as ‘they are exposed to similar sensory stimuli in the same physical environment’ (Kravchenko Citation2007, 652). In doing so, Maturana emphasizes the role of the immediate environment on an organism’s actions.8. The pico-scale comprises bodily dynamics that last from milliseconds to tenths of seconds, the microscale of bodily dynamics traces ‘saying and doings’ that can be measured from tenths of seconds to seconds’ and, finally, the enchro-scale enables one to trace situated social events that can be measured in seconds, minutes, etc. (Thibault Citation2011, 2015–216).9. Järviletho (Citation1998) introduces the theory of organism-environment system to counteract a ‘two systems’ view that clearly separates the organism from the environment. In asking, when drinking from a cup of coffee, whether the cup belongs to the organism or the environment, Järviletho visualizes how organism and environment are inseparable. Thus, behavior belongs to the organism-environment system and cannot be treated as a mediator between two systems. An organism-environment system is the result of behavior. In emphasizing the inseparability of organism and environment, he stresses that ‘all parts of the system are active in relation to the result’ (330) – observable behavior. In this view, pressing a button of an elevator, for example, does not depend on responding to a perceived stimuli, thus cannot only be attributed to an individual mind but to a person engaging with their immediate environment and acting within wider supra-structural cultural constraints. Knowing when to press the button, what it does and its functions emerges not only from past engagement with others but also from one’s cultural embedding. Mental activity, therefore, cannot be located in an organism but must be attributed to the whole system. The theory informs systemic cognition as it allows for acknowledging how non-local constraints (e.g. culture, autobiographical events, etc.) affect human cognitive action.10. Built on constructivist ideas, this form of instruction leads with the premise ‘to construct knowledge by solving problems’ (Blumenfeld et al. Citation2000, 150). With the focus on ‘real-world problems’, students are given a driving question around a particular topic. Students learn and develop new ideas in groups and ‘represent knowledge around the driving question’ (Blumenfeld et al. Citation2000).11. The group consists of four people, while Paula, Gaby and Vincent formed the core group. Anna, as the fourth member, only irregularly joined the group meetings. The course took place twice a week, while the second day was allocated for group work. The group only managed to meet in class (although attempts were made to meet outside of class), and only twice during the 7 weeks of filming has the whole group met. Hence, the theme of ‘absence’ and ‘irregularity’ greatly determined the group’s work. The four people struggled to form a group as each person wanted to work alone and had different interests. Paula’s interest was in cooking, Gaby had an interest in Korean pop music, Vincent in creating YouTube videos and Anna in baking bread. They faced the challenge of combining all four themes.12. See detailed transcript in appendix.13. The recordings were made with several cameras in the classroom. All students were in class together to discuss their projects. Hence, sound overlaps of all the group discussions and the talk of each group is, therefore, partly unintelligible. Instances that were impossible to transcribe, I marked as ((unintelligible)).14. For Cowley and Harvey (Citation2016), wordings are nonce events ‘that are perceived and construed in relation to a person’s sociocultural experience’ (58). They stress that speakers cannot produce the same sounds, but rather they produce an acoustic pattern that is treated as ‘the same’. In this logic, everything we acoustically produce is a nonce event. Only when we phenomenological act on what we hear can these actions have a normative effect.15. Please find a more detailed transcript in the appendix.16. Kendon (Citation2004) distinguishes two gesture families of the open hand: Open Hand Supine (‘palm up’) and Open Hand Prone (palm down). These gestures take on a pragmatic function in conversations. For Kendon, the former (palm up) relates to contexts of negating or interrupting while the latter to contexts of serving or offering. As seen in the current instance, none of the two aspects are happening in this moment of coordination. Instead, the gestures take over a cognitive function: they allow the students to spatio-temporally anchor past events together as they establish a project and create knowledge for themselves.17. This is contrary to traditional linguistic approaches which relegate gestures and other bodily dynamics to paralinguistic cues and, thus, not essential to linguistics. In adopting the languaging perspective, this ethnography treats such paralinguistic cues as essential and being on the same level of importance as the analysis has shown.Additional informationFundingParticipating students in this study were given a 3% extra credit for this class and, additionally, received an extra $20 for their participation. The amount was supported by a grant from the National Natural Science Foundation of China [Grants No. 31571141 and No. 31628010] in collaboration with South China Normal University. The research has been approved as exempt by the Research Compliance Human Studies Program at the American host university where the data collection was conducted. My Ph.D. research was funded by the Department of Language and Communication at the University of Southern Denmark in Odense, Denmark. The writing of this article took place during my work as a postdoctoral fellow at the Postdoc Network Brandenburg, which granted me an individual grant for my research.Notes on contributorsMarie-Theres Fester-SeegerMarie-Theres Fester-Seeger is a postdoctoral fellow at the European University of Viadrina (Frankfurt (Oder), Germany). She received her PhD at the Department of Language and Communication at the University of Southern Denmark in 2021. Her research interests include distributed language/languaging, multiscalar temporal cognition, systemic views on cognition and dialogical approaches to language. She is particularly interested in how human engagement with a direct Other determines human perception, action and thinking. Interested in human lived experience and temporality, she investigates how people are able to perceive and act upon what is not directly present and how this contributes to human becoming. On the grounds of that, she developed the idea of human presencing in her PhD. She received an individual grant from the Postdoc Network Brandenburg and currently investigates human engagement with digital voice assistants in their home environments.\",\"PeriodicalId\":51614,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Social Epistemology\",\"volume\":\" 5\",\"pages\":\"0\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.4000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-11-09\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Social Epistemology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1080/02691728.2023.2266716\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"哲学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"HISTORY & PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Social Epistemology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02691728.2023.2266716","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"HISTORY & PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

一个人过去参与特定语言社区的经验使他们能够对特定的发音运动模式作出反应并采取行动。我敢肯定,我们中的许多人都偶然发现过他们过去学生时代的旧课本或文件夹,就像我前几天一样。看着我的旧笔记,我立刻想起了在课堂上与老师和同学交谈和讨论的情景。它带来了过去与他人交往的时刻,现在在我自己的写作中具体化。系统认知(Cowley and vall<s:1> - tourangeau Citation2013)强调了人作为人类认知代理(Giere Citation2004)在更广泛的系统中的作用。考虑到一个人是如何嵌入到系统结构中并产生系统结构的,这种观点呼吁人们注意一个人在系统中的行为(例如,在工作环境或运动队中)。5 .稍后我将详细介绍这一观点。“捏造”一词是在与斯蒂芬·考利就手稿初稿进行的生动讨论中出现的,他正确地看到,被调查的学生并没有展示出专业知识,而是在为自己和他人构建知识。从生物学的角度来看,Maturana使用“双方同意”来描述生物体如何相互作用,因为“它们在相同的物理环境中暴露于相似的感官刺激”(Kravchenko Citation2007, 652)。在这样做的过程中,Maturana强调了直接环境对生物体行为的作用。微尺度包括持续时间从毫秒到十分之一秒的身体动态,微尺度的身体动态追踪可以从十分之一秒到几秒测量的“说话和行为”,最后,宏观尺度使人们能够追踪可以以秒、分等测量的情境社会事件(Thibault Citation2011, 2015-216)。Järviletho (Citation1998)引入了生物-环境系统理论,以抵消将生物与环境明显分开的“两个系统”观点。当喝一杯咖啡时,杯子是属于生物还是环境,Järviletho可视化了生物和环境是如何不可分割的。因此,行为属于生物-环境系统,不能被视为两个系统之间的中介。生物-环境系统是行为的结果。在强调有机体和环境的不可分割性时,他强调“系统的所有部分都是与结果相关的活跃的”(330)——可观察的行为。在这个观点中,按下电梯按钮,例如,不依赖于对感知刺激的反应,因此不能仅仅归因于个人的思维,而是归因于一个人与他的直接环境的接触,并在更广泛的上层建筑文化约束下行动。知道什么时候按下按钮,它做什么和它的功能不仅来自于过去与他人的接触,也来自于一个人的文化嵌入。因此,心理活动不能定位于一个有机体,而必须归因于整个系统。该理论告知系统认知,因为它允许承认非本地约束(例如文化,自传事件等)如何影响人类的认知行为。这种形式的教学建立在建构主义思想的基础上,其前提是“通过解决问题来构建知识”(Blumenfeld等人)。Citation2000, 150)。重点是“现实世界的问题”,学生们会围绕一个特定的主题提出一个驱动问题。学生在小组中学习和发展新想法,并“代表围绕驱动问题的知识”(Blumenfeld等人)。Citation2000)。。这个小组由四个人组成,而宝拉、加比和文森特组成了核心小组。作为第四名成员,安娜只是不定期地参加小组会议。课程一周上两次,第二天是小组作业。这个小组只在课堂上见过面(尽管他们也尝试过在课外见面),在拍摄的7周时间里,整个小组只见过两次面。因此,“缺席”和“不规律”的主题在很大程度上决定了该小组的工作。因为每个人都想单独工作,并且有不同的兴趣,所以这四个人很难组成一个小组。Paula对烹饪感兴趣,Gaby对韩国流行音乐感兴趣,Vincent喜欢制作YouTube视频,Anna喜欢烤面包。他们面临着把这四个主题结合起来的挑战。详情见附录13。这些录像是用教室里的几台摄像机拍摄的。所有的学生都在课堂上一起讨论他们的项目。因此,所有小组讨论的声音重叠,每个小组的谈话,因此,部分难以理解。无法抄写的实例,我标记为((听不懂))。 对于考利和哈维(Citation2016)来说,措辞是“与一个人的社会文化经验相关的感知和解释”的短期事件(58)。他们强调,扬声器不能发出相同的声音,而是发出一种被视为“相同”的声音模式。在这个逻辑中,我们在声音上产生的一切都是一个临时事件。只有当我们对所听到的东西采取现象学的行动时,这些行动才能产生规范效果。请在附录中找到更详细的文字记录。Kendon (Citation2004)区分了两种摊开手的手势:摊开手仰卧(手掌朝上)和摊开手俯卧(手掌朝下)。这些手势在对话中具有语用功能。对于Kendon来说,前者(手掌向上)与否定或打断的语境有关,而后者与服务或提供的语境有关。从目前的情况可以看出,这两个方面都没有在这个协调时刻发生。相反,手势取代了一种认知功能:当学生建立一个项目并为自己创造知识时,它们允许学生将过去的事件在时空上联系在一起。这与传统的语言方法相反,传统的语言方法将手势和其他身体动态降级为副语言线索,因此对语言学来说不是必要的。在采用语言视角的过程中,这种民族志将这些副语言线索视为必不可少的,并且与分析所显示的具有相同的重要性。参与本研究的学生在这门课上获得3%的额外学分,此外,他们的参与还获得了额外的20美元。国家自然科学基金项目(批准号:31571141、31628010)与华南师范大学合作资助。该研究已被美国主办大学的研究合规人类研究计划批准为豁免,该大学进行了数据收集。我的博士研究是由丹麦欧登塞的南丹麦大学语言与传播系资助的。这篇文章的写作是在我作为博士后在勃兰登堡博士后网络工作期间进行的,该网络为我的研究提供了个人资助。作者简介marie - theres Fester-Seeger,欧洲大学(德国法兰克福)博士后研究员。她于2021年获得南丹麦大学语言与传播系博士学位。她的研究兴趣包括分布式语言/语言,多标量时间认知,认知系统观点和语言对话方法。她特别感兴趣的是人类与直接他者的接触如何决定人类的感知、行动和思考。她对人类的生活经验和时间性感兴趣,研究人们如何能够感知和行动不直接存在的东西,以及这如何有助于人类的发展。基于此,她在博士学位期间提出了人类存在的想法。她获得了勃兰登堡博士后网络(Postdoc Network Brandenburg)的个人资助,目前正在研究人类在家庭环境中与数字语音助手的互动。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Becoming a Knower: Fabricating Knowing Through Coaction
ABSTRACTThis paper takes a step back from considering expertise as a social phenomenon. One should investigate how people become knowers before assigning expertise to a person’s actions. Using a temporal-sensitive systemic ethnography, a case study shows how undergraduate students form a social system out of necessity as they fabricate knowledge around an empty wording like ‘conscious living’. Tracing the engagement with students and tutor to recursive moments of coaction, I argue that, through the subtleties of bodily movements, people incorporate the actions of others as they become knowers. Knowing for a person solidifies as they imbue concepts, terms, facts, etc. with their own understanding. While coaction refers to the interlocking of actions in a specific moment, the fabrication of knowledge resides in temporally distributed moments of coaction where students deliberately incorporate and build on past occurrences in a present moment. In so doing, people cannot be separated from their systemic embedding. Linking coaction with systemic cognition, people fabricate knowledge within wider systemic structures. Within these boundaries, knowers come to fabricate knowledge for themselves and a wider system. Thus, knowing must be seen as an active, embodied, dialogical and multiscalar activity.KEYWORDS: Languagingcoactionsystemic cognitionembodiment AcknowledgmentsThis paper benefited greatly from the comments of two anonymous reviewers. I would also like to thank Stephen Cowley for his very valuable comments and for the insightful conversations about this paper. I also would like to thank the two editors of this special issue, Sarah Bro Trasmundi and Charlie Lassiter, for their patience and kindness and for making this special issue happen. My special thanks go to Charlie Lassiter for his invaluable feedback and help on this paper. A heartfelt thanks go to the research participants who consented to this study. Without them, this paper could not have been written.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1. The written language bias emerges from Linell’s (Citation1982) observation that ‘[o]ur conception of linguistic behavior is biased by a tendency to treat processes, activities, and conditions on them in terms of object-like, static, autonomous and permanent structures, i.e. as if they shared such properties with written characters, words, texts, pictures and images’ (1). Pointing towards the bias of treating ‘natural’ language in terms of its written forms – that is, as ‘thing’-like entities – greatly excludes how human beings bring about language. Consequently, rather than acknowledging the heterogeneous character of language, linguists (and beyond) assign fixed and stable meanings to lexical items, view language as homogeneous systems, and think about language use in terms of acting with stable structures (Linell Citation2019). Hence, traditional linguistics dismiss how people, through gaze, gestures and other bodily dynamics, engage in linguistic activity together (see Cowley Citation2010; Thibault Citation2011). Thus, the written language bias as a notion reminds scholars not to view language as an autonomous system and stable entity in itself but as something that emerges, first and foremost, from human bodily engagement with other people and things in and with the world.2. As my concern broadly is with how the actions of others change personhood in the long run, I find much value in Wegner and Sparrow’s (Citation2007) notion of coaction as it grants attention to how people incorporate the actions of others in their actions. This leads to blurring the lines of authorship in one’s actions. For me, there is, therefore, a difference between ‘joint action’ and ‘coaction’. While ‘joint action’ might presuppose ‘coaction’, in my view, ‘joint action’ alludes to how people together achieve a common goal paired with we-intentions (Fiebich and Gallagher Citation2013). The notion, thus, focuses on the activity in itself and what happens between people but not on how people build on the actions of others and how it induces a change in a person.3. Emphasizing that language is embodied activity before it is symbolic, Cowley defines wordings as ‘readily repeated aspects of vocalizations that, for speakers of a community, carry historically derived information’ (Cowley Citation2011, 186). Wordings differ from ‘words’ as the notion highlights that in languaging, people phenomenologically draw on stabilized patterns of articulatory gestures rather than on abstract objects in a language system (as highlighted in the Written Language Bias) (Cowley and Harvey Citation2016). The notion of wording fits the multiscalar character of languaging: bodily dynamics occurring on fast bodily time scales mesh with the slower sociocultural scales of a community. A person’s past experience of engaging in a specific linguistic community enables them to react to and act upon specific patterns of articulatory movements.4. I am certain that many of us have stumbled upon an old textbook or folder from their past student days, as I have the other day. Looking at my old notes immediately evoked instances of talking and discussing in class with teachers and co-students. It brought about past moments of engaging with others, now reified in my own writing.5. Systemic cognition (Cowley and Vallée-Tourangeau Citation2013) highlights the role of a person – as a human cognitive agent (Giere Citation2004) – in wider systems. Giving due weight to how a person is embedded in and brings forth systemic structures, the view calls attention to a person’s actions within a system (e.g. in a work environment or a sports team). I will present the view in more detail later.6. The term ‘fabricating’ emerged during vivid discussions with Stephen Cowley on a first draft of the manuscript, who rightly saw that the students under investigation do not display expertise but engage in moments of constructing knowledge for themselves and others.7. From a biological view, Maturana uses ‘consensual’ to describe how organisms interacting with each other do so as ‘they are exposed to similar sensory stimuli in the same physical environment’ (Kravchenko Citation2007, 652). In doing so, Maturana emphasizes the role of the immediate environment on an organism’s actions.8. The pico-scale comprises bodily dynamics that last from milliseconds to tenths of seconds, the microscale of bodily dynamics traces ‘saying and doings’ that can be measured from tenths of seconds to seconds’ and, finally, the enchro-scale enables one to trace situated social events that can be measured in seconds, minutes, etc. (Thibault Citation2011, 2015–216).9. Järviletho (Citation1998) introduces the theory of organism-environment system to counteract a ‘two systems’ view that clearly separates the organism from the environment. In asking, when drinking from a cup of coffee, whether the cup belongs to the organism or the environment, Järviletho visualizes how organism and environment are inseparable. Thus, behavior belongs to the organism-environment system and cannot be treated as a mediator between two systems. An organism-environment system is the result of behavior. In emphasizing the inseparability of organism and environment, he stresses that ‘all parts of the system are active in relation to the result’ (330) – observable behavior. In this view, pressing a button of an elevator, for example, does not depend on responding to a perceived stimuli, thus cannot only be attributed to an individual mind but to a person engaging with their immediate environment and acting within wider supra-structural cultural constraints. Knowing when to press the button, what it does and its functions emerges not only from past engagement with others but also from one’s cultural embedding. Mental activity, therefore, cannot be located in an organism but must be attributed to the whole system. The theory informs systemic cognition as it allows for acknowledging how non-local constraints (e.g. culture, autobiographical events, etc.) affect human cognitive action.10. Built on constructivist ideas, this form of instruction leads with the premise ‘to construct knowledge by solving problems’ (Blumenfeld et al. Citation2000, 150). With the focus on ‘real-world problems’, students are given a driving question around a particular topic. Students learn and develop new ideas in groups and ‘represent knowledge around the driving question’ (Blumenfeld et al. Citation2000).11. The group consists of four people, while Paula, Gaby and Vincent formed the core group. Anna, as the fourth member, only irregularly joined the group meetings. The course took place twice a week, while the second day was allocated for group work. The group only managed to meet in class (although attempts were made to meet outside of class), and only twice during the 7 weeks of filming has the whole group met. Hence, the theme of ‘absence’ and ‘irregularity’ greatly determined the group’s work. The four people struggled to form a group as each person wanted to work alone and had different interests. Paula’s interest was in cooking, Gaby had an interest in Korean pop music, Vincent in creating YouTube videos and Anna in baking bread. They faced the challenge of combining all four themes.12. See detailed transcript in appendix.13. The recordings were made with several cameras in the classroom. All students were in class together to discuss their projects. Hence, sound overlaps of all the group discussions and the talk of each group is, therefore, partly unintelligible. Instances that were impossible to transcribe, I marked as ((unintelligible)).14. For Cowley and Harvey (Citation2016), wordings are nonce events ‘that are perceived and construed in relation to a person’s sociocultural experience’ (58). They stress that speakers cannot produce the same sounds, but rather they produce an acoustic pattern that is treated as ‘the same’. In this logic, everything we acoustically produce is a nonce event. Only when we phenomenological act on what we hear can these actions have a normative effect.15. Please find a more detailed transcript in the appendix.16. Kendon (Citation2004) distinguishes two gesture families of the open hand: Open Hand Supine (‘palm up’) and Open Hand Prone (palm down). These gestures take on a pragmatic function in conversations. For Kendon, the former (palm up) relates to contexts of negating or interrupting while the latter to contexts of serving or offering. As seen in the current instance, none of the two aspects are happening in this moment of coordination. Instead, the gestures take over a cognitive function: they allow the students to spatio-temporally anchor past events together as they establish a project and create knowledge for themselves.17. This is contrary to traditional linguistic approaches which relegate gestures and other bodily dynamics to paralinguistic cues and, thus, not essential to linguistics. In adopting the languaging perspective, this ethnography treats such paralinguistic cues as essential and being on the same level of importance as the analysis has shown.Additional informationFundingParticipating students in this study were given a 3% extra credit for this class and, additionally, received an extra $20 for their participation. The amount was supported by a grant from the National Natural Science Foundation of China [Grants No. 31571141 and No. 31628010] in collaboration with South China Normal University. The research has been approved as exempt by the Research Compliance Human Studies Program at the American host university where the data collection was conducted. My Ph.D. research was funded by the Department of Language and Communication at the University of Southern Denmark in Odense, Denmark. The writing of this article took place during my work as a postdoctoral fellow at the Postdoc Network Brandenburg, which granted me an individual grant for my research.Notes on contributorsMarie-Theres Fester-SeegerMarie-Theres Fester-Seeger is a postdoctoral fellow at the European University of Viadrina (Frankfurt (Oder), Germany). She received her PhD at the Department of Language and Communication at the University of Southern Denmark in 2021. Her research interests include distributed language/languaging, multiscalar temporal cognition, systemic views on cognition and dialogical approaches to language. She is particularly interested in how human engagement with a direct Other determines human perception, action and thinking. Interested in human lived experience and temporality, she investigates how people are able to perceive and act upon what is not directly present and how this contributes to human becoming. On the grounds of that, she developed the idea of human presencing in her PhD. She received an individual grant from the Postdoc Network Brandenburg and currently investigates human engagement with digital voice assistants in their home environments.
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
2.60
自引率
17.60%
发文量
60
期刊介绍: Social Epistemology provides a forum for philosophical and social scientific enquiry that incorporates the work of scholars from a variety of disciplines who share a concern with the production, assessment and validation of knowledge. The journal covers both empirical research into the origination and transmission of knowledge and normative considerations which arise as such research is implemented, serving as a guide for directing contemporary knowledge enterprises. Social Epistemology publishes "exchanges" which are the collective product of several contributors and take the form of critical syntheses, open peer commentaries interviews, applications, provocations, reviews and responses
期刊最新文献
Scientism and the Problem of Self-Referential Incoherence Testimonial Injustice from Countervailing Prejudices ‘Blackness’, the Body and Epistemological and Epistemic Traps: A Phenomenological Analysis The Contribution of Logic to Epistemic Injustice Friend or Foe? Rethinking Epistemic Trespassing
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1