五十年来关于职业倦怠的争论

IF 6.7 2区 医学 Q1 MEDICINE, GENERAL & INTERNAL Medical Journal of Australia Pub Date : 2024-11-03 DOI:10.5694/mja2.52512
Renzo Bianchi, James F Sowden
{"title":"五十年来关于职业倦怠的争论","authors":"Renzo Bianchi,&nbsp;James F Sowden","doi":"10.5694/mja2.52512","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>First described in the mid-1970s, “burnout” has elicited continued interest among occupational health specialists.<span><sup>1, 2</sup></span> The World Health Organization<span><sup>3</sup></span> defines burnout as a triadic syndrome that comprises: (i) feelings of energy depletion or exhaustion; (ii) increased mental distance from one's job, or feelings of negativism or cynicism towards one's job; and (iii) a sense of ineffectiveness and lack of accomplishment. This definition closely aligns with the conceptualisation of burnout in the Maslach Burnout Inventory, the most prominent measure of the entity.<span><sup>2, 4</sup></span> Although burnout has become a popular indicator of job-related distress, persistent controversies surround the construct. As burnout reaches its half-century of existence, this article offers an overview of key research developments that have prompted investigators to revamp their views of the syndrome.</p><p>The conventional wisdom among researchers is that burnout arises from unresolvable job stress.<span><sup>2, 5</sup></span> The “job-relatedness” of burnout has been considered a signature feature of the syndrome.<span><sup>2</sup></span> These views have been incorporated by the World Health Organization, which characterises burnout as a syndrome “resulting from chronic workplace stress that has not been successfully managed”.<span><sup>3</sup></span> Although widely shared, the idea that work-related stress is the force driving the development of burnout has proven difficult to support. Substantial associations between work-related stress and burnout have been documented in a wealth of cross-sectional studies. However, longitudinal studies have showed a more subtle pattern of results. In a meta-analysis of 74 follow-up studies, Lesener and colleagues found that job demands and job resources predicted burnout only modestly.<span><sup>6</sup></span> Similar results were obtained by Guthier and colleagues in a meta-analysis of 48 follow-up studies focusing on job stressors and burnout.<span><sup>7</sup></span> Guthier and colleagues found that the association between job stressors and burnout was not only small but also likely overestimated.<span><sup>7</sup></span> In both meta-analyses, burnout was predictive of, rather than predicted by, work-related stress. In summary, there is no clear evidence that burnout is primarily caused by work, or that work contributes more to burnout than it does to other stress-related conditions — such as anxiety and depression. Recently, an increased focus on non-work factors (eg, negative life events, lifestyle factors), personality traits (eg, neuroticism), and physical disorders (eg, sleep–wake disorders and thyroid disorders) has been encouraged.<span><sup>7, 8</sup></span> Studies capitalising on intensive longitudinal methods, objective (health) measures, and long term follow-ups may be helpful in this endeavour. Without a deeper understanding of the determinants of burnout, designing effective interventions is likely to remain challenging.</p><p>Claims of an ongoing burnout epidemic have proliferated in recent decades. Sky-high prevalence estimates have been circulating in both the scientific and general press. Yet, the validity and plausibility of these figures have been a cause for concern. Investigators have underscored that burnout prevalence cannot be estimated because burnout cannot be accurately diagnosed.<span><sup>8, 9</sup></span> In practice, burnout prevalence has been gauged using criteria that are not only clinically and theoretically arbitrary but also loose and heterogeneous.<span><sup>9</sup></span> The use of such criteria has faced severe criticism, with calls to stop conducting (and publishing) prevalence studies until sound diagnostic criteria are established.<span><sup>8, 9</sup></span> If developing diagnostic criteria for burnout, researchers should be mindful of diagnosis creep<span><sup>10</sup></span> to avoid pathologising ordinary variations in stress, fatigue or motivation.</p><p>Although many researchers have approached burnout and depression as two different animals,<span><sup>2</sup></span> identifying tangible differences between the entities has been challenging.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Examining the clinical picture ascribed to burnout, it is difficult not to notice that the symptomatology of burnout borrows heavily from that of depressive conditions. Maslach and colleagues<span><sup>2</sup></span> themselves indicated that burnout is characterised by “a predominance of dysphoric symptoms”. A particularly puzzling finding is that burnout symptoms correlate less strongly with each other than with depressive symptoms.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Based on such results, investigators have suggested that burnout symptoms could be regarded as fragments of depressive symptomatology rather than the components of a standalone syndrome.<span><sup>11</sup></span> Aetiologically, unresolvable stress appears as a common denominator.<span><sup>8, 12</sup></span> Sen noted that burnout and depression are predicted by essentially the same factors.<span><sup>13</sup></span> Studies of cognitive functioning, focusing on how people handle tasks and process stimuli in their environment, found that burnout involves alterations typical of depression.<span><sup>8</sup></span> On the neurobiological front, research on burnout has been inconclusive.<span><sup>14</sup></span> For instance, multiple cortisolic profiles have been found from study to study, from hypocortisolism to normal cortisolemia to hypercortisolism. On the therapeutic side, many clinicians have warned against drawing a demarcation line between burnout and depression. Depressive disorders require close medical attention and are a prime risk factor for suicidal behaviors.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Separating burnout from depression may deprive people categorised as “burned out” of access to potentially life-saving treatments. Promoting the burnout–depression distinction without compelling evidence that a distinction is warranted may have sombre consequences. Investigators seeking to contextualise depressive symptoms within the work domain can rely on instruments such as the Occupational Depression Inventory.<span><sup>8, 11</sup></span></p><p>Perhaps because it did not emerge from psychiatry research, burnout has often been viewed as a benign label permitting safer communication on job-related distress.<span><sup>15</sup></span> Recent research has challenged this belief. For example, Sterkens and colleagues found that individuals with a history of burnout were less likely to be promoted.<span><sup>16</sup></span> Interestingly, having a history of burnout mattered more when being considered for a promotion than current performance. Formerly burned-out employees were perceived more negatively in multiple domains — for example, leadership capacities, motivation, autonomy, stress tolerance, current health, chances of future sick leave, and chances of finding another job. In a randomised online trial study, Smith and colleagues found that the burnout label was as stigmatising as the depression label.<span><sup>17</sup></span> The authors underlined that “providing a burnout diagnosis to explain mild depressive symptoms in workplace/occupational contexts may not be more favourable in terms of alleviating stigma and increasing help-seeking”. In conclusion, the burnout label may not be as socially accepted as previously thought and may require cautious use in organisations.</p><p>For decades, researchers have regarded exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy as the defining features and building blocks of the burnout syndrome.<span><sup>2-4</sup></span> As an illustration, Maslach and Leiter<span><sup>5</sup></span> indicated that “[t]he burnout syndrome occurs when people experience combined crises on all three of these dimensions”. Given its pivotal status, the burnout definition could be expected to rest on a solid foundation. Historical analysis reveals a more complicated picture.<span><sup>18-21</sup></span> The development of the burnout construct did not originate from robust empirical investigations or in-depth theorising. Burnout surfaced in the literature through anecdotal reports and rudimentary studies in which the construct appeared largely predefined.<span><sup>18</sup></span> These studies barely met any scientific standard (eg, in terms of measurement, data analysis or replicability) and were highly susceptible to observer bias (eg, confirmation bias).<span><sup>2, 18-21</sup></span> Such studies were thus ill-equipped to identify a syndrome. The Maslach Burnout Inventory, whose publication in 1981 formalised burnout's three-component definition, was derived from this slippery research path.<span><sup>2</sup></span> Interestingly, there is little evidence that exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy can be subsumed under a general or higher-order burnout factor.<span><sup>22</sup></span> Put differently, exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy do not show the unity expected of the components of a syndrome. In such a context, it is unsurprising that the characterisation of burnout remains widely debated.</p><p>The controversies surrounding the burnout construct have led some researchers to engage in redefinition initiatives. To our knowledge, the most recent redefinition attempt was undertaken by Tavella and colleagues.<span><sup>23</sup></span> These authors asked individuals “who self-identified as experiencing burnout” to complete a questionnaire covering multiple candidate burnout symptoms. The authors then used the symptoms reported to reshape the burnout construct. Although commendable in its clarifying intent, this redefinition attempt exhibits major limitations. Perhaps the most serious flaw is the reliance on participants self-identifying as burned out. According to Maslach and Leiter, the method of asking people whether they feel burned out is “the worst” because it mistakenly assumes that everybody has the same definition of burnout.<span><sup>24</sup></span> The modus operandi used by Tavella and colleagues<span><sup>23</sup></span> contravenes a basic survey requirement, namely, the use of univocal terms.<span><sup>25</sup></span> The term “burnout” is employed in everyday conversations with various meanings to describe various experiences. Assuming that non-specialists have a common understanding of the term when even researchers and practitioners disagree on its definition is a nonstarter. The tendency to fetishise words — forgetting that words have no inherent meaning — constitutes an epistemological fallacy frequently encountered in burnout research.</p><p>Fifty years of research on burnout have allowed investigators to refine their views of the syndrome (Box). Many of the narratives that accompanied the introduction of the burnout construct have been debunked. Some misconceptions have reached the status of urban legends, continuing to haunt the field despite their lack of validity. Somewhat disconcertingly, the most pressing issue for burnout researchers may be to agree on the basic nature of their entity of interest. The question of whether burnout reflects a “genuine phenomenon”, irreducible to classical manifestations of distress (ie, anxiety and depressive symptoms) may require special attention as researchers further elucidate the burnout enigma.</p><p>No relevant disclosures.</p><p>Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.</p>","PeriodicalId":18214,"journal":{"name":"Medical Journal of Australia","volume":"221 11","pages":"573-575"},"PeriodicalIF":6.7000,"publicationDate":"2024-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11625531/pdf/","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Five decades of debate on burnout\",\"authors\":\"Renzo Bianchi,&nbsp;James F Sowden\",\"doi\":\"10.5694/mja2.52512\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>First described in the mid-1970s, “burnout” has elicited continued interest among occupational health specialists.<span><sup>1, 2</sup></span> The World Health Organization<span><sup>3</sup></span> defines burnout as a triadic syndrome that comprises: (i) feelings of energy depletion or exhaustion; (ii) increased mental distance from one's job, or feelings of negativism or cynicism towards one's job; and (iii) a sense of ineffectiveness and lack of accomplishment. This definition closely aligns with the conceptualisation of burnout in the Maslach Burnout Inventory, the most prominent measure of the entity.<span><sup>2, 4</sup></span> Although burnout has become a popular indicator of job-related distress, persistent controversies surround the construct. As burnout reaches its half-century of existence, this article offers an overview of key research developments that have prompted investigators to revamp their views of the syndrome.</p><p>The conventional wisdom among researchers is that burnout arises from unresolvable job stress.<span><sup>2, 5</sup></span> The “job-relatedness” of burnout has been considered a signature feature of the syndrome.<span><sup>2</sup></span> These views have been incorporated by the World Health Organization, which characterises burnout as a syndrome “resulting from chronic workplace stress that has not been successfully managed”.<span><sup>3</sup></span> Although widely shared, the idea that work-related stress is the force driving the development of burnout has proven difficult to support. Substantial associations between work-related stress and burnout have been documented in a wealth of cross-sectional studies. However, longitudinal studies have showed a more subtle pattern of results. In a meta-analysis of 74 follow-up studies, Lesener and colleagues found that job demands and job resources predicted burnout only modestly.<span><sup>6</sup></span> Similar results were obtained by Guthier and colleagues in a meta-analysis of 48 follow-up studies focusing on job stressors and burnout.<span><sup>7</sup></span> Guthier and colleagues found that the association between job stressors and burnout was not only small but also likely overestimated.<span><sup>7</sup></span> In both meta-analyses, burnout was predictive of, rather than predicted by, work-related stress. In summary, there is no clear evidence that burnout is primarily caused by work, or that work contributes more to burnout than it does to other stress-related conditions — such as anxiety and depression. Recently, an increased focus on non-work factors (eg, negative life events, lifestyle factors), personality traits (eg, neuroticism), and physical disorders (eg, sleep–wake disorders and thyroid disorders) has been encouraged.<span><sup>7, 8</sup></span> Studies capitalising on intensive longitudinal methods, objective (health) measures, and long term follow-ups may be helpful in this endeavour. Without a deeper understanding of the determinants of burnout, designing effective interventions is likely to remain challenging.</p><p>Claims of an ongoing burnout epidemic have proliferated in recent decades. Sky-high prevalence estimates have been circulating in both the scientific and general press. Yet, the validity and plausibility of these figures have been a cause for concern. Investigators have underscored that burnout prevalence cannot be estimated because burnout cannot be accurately diagnosed.<span><sup>8, 9</sup></span> In practice, burnout prevalence has been gauged using criteria that are not only clinically and theoretically arbitrary but also loose and heterogeneous.<span><sup>9</sup></span> The use of such criteria has faced severe criticism, with calls to stop conducting (and publishing) prevalence studies until sound diagnostic criteria are established.<span><sup>8, 9</sup></span> If developing diagnostic criteria for burnout, researchers should be mindful of diagnosis creep<span><sup>10</sup></span> to avoid pathologising ordinary variations in stress, fatigue or motivation.</p><p>Although many researchers have approached burnout and depression as two different animals,<span><sup>2</sup></span> identifying tangible differences between the entities has been challenging.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Examining the clinical picture ascribed to burnout, it is difficult not to notice that the symptomatology of burnout borrows heavily from that of depressive conditions. Maslach and colleagues<span><sup>2</sup></span> themselves indicated that burnout is characterised by “a predominance of dysphoric symptoms”. A particularly puzzling finding is that burnout symptoms correlate less strongly with each other than with depressive symptoms.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Based on such results, investigators have suggested that burnout symptoms could be regarded as fragments of depressive symptomatology rather than the components of a standalone syndrome.<span><sup>11</sup></span> Aetiologically, unresolvable stress appears as a common denominator.<span><sup>8, 12</sup></span> Sen noted that burnout and depression are predicted by essentially the same factors.<span><sup>13</sup></span> Studies of cognitive functioning, focusing on how people handle tasks and process stimuli in their environment, found that burnout involves alterations typical of depression.<span><sup>8</sup></span> On the neurobiological front, research on burnout has been inconclusive.<span><sup>14</sup></span> For instance, multiple cortisolic profiles have been found from study to study, from hypocortisolism to normal cortisolemia to hypercortisolism. On the therapeutic side, many clinicians have warned against drawing a demarcation line between burnout and depression. Depressive disorders require close medical attention and are a prime risk factor for suicidal behaviors.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Separating burnout from depression may deprive people categorised as “burned out” of access to potentially life-saving treatments. Promoting the burnout–depression distinction without compelling evidence that a distinction is warranted may have sombre consequences. Investigators seeking to contextualise depressive symptoms within the work domain can rely on instruments such as the Occupational Depression Inventory.<span><sup>8, 11</sup></span></p><p>Perhaps because it did not emerge from psychiatry research, burnout has often been viewed as a benign label permitting safer communication on job-related distress.<span><sup>15</sup></span> Recent research has challenged this belief. For example, Sterkens and colleagues found that individuals with a history of burnout were less likely to be promoted.<span><sup>16</sup></span> Interestingly, having a history of burnout mattered more when being considered for a promotion than current performance. Formerly burned-out employees were perceived more negatively in multiple domains — for example, leadership capacities, motivation, autonomy, stress tolerance, current health, chances of future sick leave, and chances of finding another job. In a randomised online trial study, Smith and colleagues found that the burnout label was as stigmatising as the depression label.<span><sup>17</sup></span> The authors underlined that “providing a burnout diagnosis to explain mild depressive symptoms in workplace/occupational contexts may not be more favourable in terms of alleviating stigma and increasing help-seeking”. In conclusion, the burnout label may not be as socially accepted as previously thought and may require cautious use in organisations.</p><p>For decades, researchers have regarded exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy as the defining features and building blocks of the burnout syndrome.<span><sup>2-4</sup></span> As an illustration, Maslach and Leiter<span><sup>5</sup></span> indicated that “[t]he burnout syndrome occurs when people experience combined crises on all three of these dimensions”. Given its pivotal status, the burnout definition could be expected to rest on a solid foundation. Historical analysis reveals a more complicated picture.<span><sup>18-21</sup></span> The development of the burnout construct did not originate from robust empirical investigations or in-depth theorising. Burnout surfaced in the literature through anecdotal reports and rudimentary studies in which the construct appeared largely predefined.<span><sup>18</sup></span> These studies barely met any scientific standard (eg, in terms of measurement, data analysis or replicability) and were highly susceptible to observer bias (eg, confirmation bias).<span><sup>2, 18-21</sup></span> Such studies were thus ill-equipped to identify a syndrome. The Maslach Burnout Inventory, whose publication in 1981 formalised burnout's three-component definition, was derived from this slippery research path.<span><sup>2</sup></span> Interestingly, there is little evidence that exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy can be subsumed under a general or higher-order burnout factor.<span><sup>22</sup></span> Put differently, exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy do not show the unity expected of the components of a syndrome. In such a context, it is unsurprising that the characterisation of burnout remains widely debated.</p><p>The controversies surrounding the burnout construct have led some researchers to engage in redefinition initiatives. To our knowledge, the most recent redefinition attempt was undertaken by Tavella and colleagues.<span><sup>23</sup></span> These authors asked individuals “who self-identified as experiencing burnout” to complete a questionnaire covering multiple candidate burnout symptoms. The authors then used the symptoms reported to reshape the burnout construct. Although commendable in its clarifying intent, this redefinition attempt exhibits major limitations. Perhaps the most serious flaw is the reliance on participants self-identifying as burned out. According to Maslach and Leiter, the method of asking people whether they feel burned out is “the worst” because it mistakenly assumes that everybody has the same definition of burnout.<span><sup>24</sup></span> The modus operandi used by Tavella and colleagues<span><sup>23</sup></span> contravenes a basic survey requirement, namely, the use of univocal terms.<span><sup>25</sup></span> The term “burnout” is employed in everyday conversations with various meanings to describe various experiences. Assuming that non-specialists have a common understanding of the term when even researchers and practitioners disagree on its definition is a nonstarter. The tendency to fetishise words — forgetting that words have no inherent meaning — constitutes an epistemological fallacy frequently encountered in burnout research.</p><p>Fifty years of research on burnout have allowed investigators to refine their views of the syndrome (Box). Many of the narratives that accompanied the introduction of the burnout construct have been debunked. Some misconceptions have reached the status of urban legends, continuing to haunt the field despite their lack of validity. Somewhat disconcertingly, the most pressing issue for burnout researchers may be to agree on the basic nature of their entity of interest. The question of whether burnout reflects a “genuine phenomenon”, irreducible to classical manifestations of distress (ie, anxiety and depressive symptoms) may require special attention as researchers further elucidate the burnout enigma.</p><p>No relevant disclosures.</p><p>Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":18214,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Medical Journal of Australia\",\"volume\":\"221 11\",\"pages\":\"573-575\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":6.7000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-11-03\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11625531/pdf/\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Medical Journal of Australia\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.5694/mja2.52512\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"医学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"MEDICINE, GENERAL & INTERNAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Medical Journal of Australia","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.5694/mja2.52512","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"MEDICINE, GENERAL & INTERNAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

“职业倦怠”在20世纪70年代中期首次被描述,引起了职业健康专家的持续关注。1,2世界卫生组织3将倦怠定义为一种三重综合征,包括:(i)感觉精力耗尽或疲惫;(ii)与工作的心理距离增加,或对工作产生消极或愤世嫉俗的情绪;(三)无效能感和缺乏成就感。这一定义与马斯拉克职业倦怠量表(Maslach burnout Inventory)中职业倦怠的概念密切相关,后者是衡量企业最重要的指标。尽管职业倦怠已成为一个流行的工作相关压力指标,但围绕这一概念的争议一直存在。随着职业倦怠已经存在了半个世纪,这篇文章提供了一个关键研究进展的概述,这些研究进展促使研究人员改变了他们对这种综合症的看法。研究人员的传统观点是,职业倦怠源于无法解决的工作压力。2,5职业倦怠的“工作相关性”被认为是该综合征的一个标志性特征这些观点已被世界卫生组织纳入其中,该组织将职业倦怠定性为“由于长期工作压力而未得到成功管理”的一种综合症尽管工作压力是导致职业倦怠的原因这一观点得到了广泛认同,但事实证明,这一观点很难得到支持。大量的横断面研究已经证明了工作压力和职业倦怠之间的实质性联系。然而,纵向研究显示了一种更为微妙的结果模式。在对74项后续研究的荟萃分析中,Lesener及其同事发现,工作需求和工作资源对职业倦怠的预测作用不大Guthier和他的同事对48项关注工作压力源和倦怠的后续研究进行了荟萃分析,得出了类似的结果Guthier和他的同事们发现,工作压力源和倦怠之间的联系不仅很小,而且很可能被高估了在这两项荟萃分析中,职业倦怠是工作压力的预测因素,而不是工作压力的预测因素。总而言之,没有明确的证据表明职业倦怠主要是由工作引起的,或者工作对职业倦怠的影响比对其他与压力相关的情况(如焦虑和抑郁)的影响更大。最近,越来越多的关注非工作因素(如负面生活事件、生活方式因素)、人格特征(如神经质)和身体疾病(如睡眠-觉醒障碍和甲状腺疾病)。7,8利用密集的纵向方法、客观(健康)测量和长期随访的研究可能有助于这一努力。如果对职业倦怠的决定因素没有更深入的了解,设计有效的干预措施可能仍然具有挑战性。近几十年来,职业倦怠流行病的说法越来越多。科学和一般新闻界都流传着极高的流行率估计。然而,这些数字的有效性和合理性令人担忧。研究人员强调,由于无法准确诊断,因此无法估计倦怠的患病率。在实践中,职业倦怠患病率的衡量标准不仅在临床上和理论上是任意的,而且是松散和异构的这种标准的使用受到了严厉的批评,有人呼吁在建立健全的诊断标准之前停止进行(和发表)患病率研究。在制定职业倦怠的诊断标准时,研究人员应注意诊断潜行,以避免将压力、疲劳或动机方面的普通变化病态化。尽管许多研究人员将倦怠和抑郁视为两种不同的动物,但要确定两者之间的明显差异仍是一项挑战检查倦怠的临床表现,很难不注意到倦怠的症状学在很大程度上借鉴了抑郁症的症状。马斯拉奇和他的同事们自己也指出,职业倦怠的特点是“焦虑的症状占主导地位”。一个特别令人困惑的发现是,倦怠症状之间的相关性不如抑郁症状之间的相关性强基于这些结果,研究者认为,倦怠症状可以被视为抑郁症状的一部分,而不是一个独立症状的组成部分在病因学上,无法解决的压力似乎是一个共同点。8、12森指出,倦怠和抑郁基本上是由相同的因素预测的对认知功能的研究,主要关注人们在环境中如何处理任务和处理刺激,发现倦怠与抑郁症的典型变化有关在神经生物学方面,对倦怠的研究尚无定论。 许多伴随倦怠结构引入的叙述已经被揭穿。一些误解已经达到了都市传说的地位,尽管缺乏有效性,但仍然困扰着这个领域。有些令人不安的是,对于职业倦怠研究人员来说,最紧迫的问题可能是就他们感兴趣的实体的基本性质达成一致。随着研究人员进一步阐明倦怠之谜,倦怠是否反映了一种“真实现象”,即不能归结为痛苦的经典表现(即焦虑和抑郁症状),这一问题可能需要特别关注。无相关披露。不是委托;外部同行评审。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Five decades of debate on burnout

First described in the mid-1970s, “burnout” has elicited continued interest among occupational health specialists.1, 2 The World Health Organization3 defines burnout as a triadic syndrome that comprises: (i) feelings of energy depletion or exhaustion; (ii) increased mental distance from one's job, or feelings of negativism or cynicism towards one's job; and (iii) a sense of ineffectiveness and lack of accomplishment. This definition closely aligns with the conceptualisation of burnout in the Maslach Burnout Inventory, the most prominent measure of the entity.2, 4 Although burnout has become a popular indicator of job-related distress, persistent controversies surround the construct. As burnout reaches its half-century of existence, this article offers an overview of key research developments that have prompted investigators to revamp their views of the syndrome.

The conventional wisdom among researchers is that burnout arises from unresolvable job stress.2, 5 The “job-relatedness” of burnout has been considered a signature feature of the syndrome.2 These views have been incorporated by the World Health Organization, which characterises burnout as a syndrome “resulting from chronic workplace stress that has not been successfully managed”.3 Although widely shared, the idea that work-related stress is the force driving the development of burnout has proven difficult to support. Substantial associations between work-related stress and burnout have been documented in a wealth of cross-sectional studies. However, longitudinal studies have showed a more subtle pattern of results. In a meta-analysis of 74 follow-up studies, Lesener and colleagues found that job demands and job resources predicted burnout only modestly.6 Similar results were obtained by Guthier and colleagues in a meta-analysis of 48 follow-up studies focusing on job stressors and burnout.7 Guthier and colleagues found that the association between job stressors and burnout was not only small but also likely overestimated.7 In both meta-analyses, burnout was predictive of, rather than predicted by, work-related stress. In summary, there is no clear evidence that burnout is primarily caused by work, or that work contributes more to burnout than it does to other stress-related conditions — such as anxiety and depression. Recently, an increased focus on non-work factors (eg, negative life events, lifestyle factors), personality traits (eg, neuroticism), and physical disorders (eg, sleep–wake disorders and thyroid disorders) has been encouraged.7, 8 Studies capitalising on intensive longitudinal methods, objective (health) measures, and long term follow-ups may be helpful in this endeavour. Without a deeper understanding of the determinants of burnout, designing effective interventions is likely to remain challenging.

Claims of an ongoing burnout epidemic have proliferated in recent decades. Sky-high prevalence estimates have been circulating in both the scientific and general press. Yet, the validity and plausibility of these figures have been a cause for concern. Investigators have underscored that burnout prevalence cannot be estimated because burnout cannot be accurately diagnosed.8, 9 In practice, burnout prevalence has been gauged using criteria that are not only clinically and theoretically arbitrary but also loose and heterogeneous.9 The use of such criteria has faced severe criticism, with calls to stop conducting (and publishing) prevalence studies until sound diagnostic criteria are established.8, 9 If developing diagnostic criteria for burnout, researchers should be mindful of diagnosis creep10 to avoid pathologising ordinary variations in stress, fatigue or motivation.

Although many researchers have approached burnout and depression as two different animals,2 identifying tangible differences between the entities has been challenging.8 Examining the clinical picture ascribed to burnout, it is difficult not to notice that the symptomatology of burnout borrows heavily from that of depressive conditions. Maslach and colleagues2 themselves indicated that burnout is characterised by “a predominance of dysphoric symptoms”. A particularly puzzling finding is that burnout symptoms correlate less strongly with each other than with depressive symptoms.8 Based on such results, investigators have suggested that burnout symptoms could be regarded as fragments of depressive symptomatology rather than the components of a standalone syndrome.11 Aetiologically, unresolvable stress appears as a common denominator.8, 12 Sen noted that burnout and depression are predicted by essentially the same factors.13 Studies of cognitive functioning, focusing on how people handle tasks and process stimuli in their environment, found that burnout involves alterations typical of depression.8 On the neurobiological front, research on burnout has been inconclusive.14 For instance, multiple cortisolic profiles have been found from study to study, from hypocortisolism to normal cortisolemia to hypercortisolism. On the therapeutic side, many clinicians have warned against drawing a demarcation line between burnout and depression. Depressive disorders require close medical attention and are a prime risk factor for suicidal behaviors.8 Separating burnout from depression may deprive people categorised as “burned out” of access to potentially life-saving treatments. Promoting the burnout–depression distinction without compelling evidence that a distinction is warranted may have sombre consequences. Investigators seeking to contextualise depressive symptoms within the work domain can rely on instruments such as the Occupational Depression Inventory.8, 11

Perhaps because it did not emerge from psychiatry research, burnout has often been viewed as a benign label permitting safer communication on job-related distress.15 Recent research has challenged this belief. For example, Sterkens and colleagues found that individuals with a history of burnout were less likely to be promoted.16 Interestingly, having a history of burnout mattered more when being considered for a promotion than current performance. Formerly burned-out employees were perceived more negatively in multiple domains — for example, leadership capacities, motivation, autonomy, stress tolerance, current health, chances of future sick leave, and chances of finding another job. In a randomised online trial study, Smith and colleagues found that the burnout label was as stigmatising as the depression label.17 The authors underlined that “providing a burnout diagnosis to explain mild depressive symptoms in workplace/occupational contexts may not be more favourable in terms of alleviating stigma and increasing help-seeking”. In conclusion, the burnout label may not be as socially accepted as previously thought and may require cautious use in organisations.

For decades, researchers have regarded exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy as the defining features and building blocks of the burnout syndrome.2-4 As an illustration, Maslach and Leiter5 indicated that “[t]he burnout syndrome occurs when people experience combined crises on all three of these dimensions”. Given its pivotal status, the burnout definition could be expected to rest on a solid foundation. Historical analysis reveals a more complicated picture.18-21 The development of the burnout construct did not originate from robust empirical investigations or in-depth theorising. Burnout surfaced in the literature through anecdotal reports and rudimentary studies in which the construct appeared largely predefined.18 These studies barely met any scientific standard (eg, in terms of measurement, data analysis or replicability) and were highly susceptible to observer bias (eg, confirmation bias).2, 18-21 Such studies were thus ill-equipped to identify a syndrome. The Maslach Burnout Inventory, whose publication in 1981 formalised burnout's three-component definition, was derived from this slippery research path.2 Interestingly, there is little evidence that exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy can be subsumed under a general or higher-order burnout factor.22 Put differently, exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacy do not show the unity expected of the components of a syndrome. In such a context, it is unsurprising that the characterisation of burnout remains widely debated.

The controversies surrounding the burnout construct have led some researchers to engage in redefinition initiatives. To our knowledge, the most recent redefinition attempt was undertaken by Tavella and colleagues.23 These authors asked individuals “who self-identified as experiencing burnout” to complete a questionnaire covering multiple candidate burnout symptoms. The authors then used the symptoms reported to reshape the burnout construct. Although commendable in its clarifying intent, this redefinition attempt exhibits major limitations. Perhaps the most serious flaw is the reliance on participants self-identifying as burned out. According to Maslach and Leiter, the method of asking people whether they feel burned out is “the worst” because it mistakenly assumes that everybody has the same definition of burnout.24 The modus operandi used by Tavella and colleagues23 contravenes a basic survey requirement, namely, the use of univocal terms.25 The term “burnout” is employed in everyday conversations with various meanings to describe various experiences. Assuming that non-specialists have a common understanding of the term when even researchers and practitioners disagree on its definition is a nonstarter. The tendency to fetishise words — forgetting that words have no inherent meaning — constitutes an epistemological fallacy frequently encountered in burnout research.

Fifty years of research on burnout have allowed investigators to refine their views of the syndrome (Box). Many of the narratives that accompanied the introduction of the burnout construct have been debunked. Some misconceptions have reached the status of urban legends, continuing to haunt the field despite their lack of validity. Somewhat disconcertingly, the most pressing issue for burnout researchers may be to agree on the basic nature of their entity of interest. The question of whether burnout reflects a “genuine phenomenon”, irreducible to classical manifestations of distress (ie, anxiety and depressive symptoms) may require special attention as researchers further elucidate the burnout enigma.

No relevant disclosures.

Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Medical Journal of Australia
Medical Journal of Australia 医学-医学:内科
CiteScore
9.40
自引率
5.30%
发文量
410
审稿时长
3-8 weeks
期刊介绍: The Medical Journal of Australia (MJA) stands as Australia's foremost general medical journal, leading the dissemination of high-quality research and commentary to shape health policy and influence medical practices within the country. Under the leadership of Professor Virginia Barbour, the expert editorial team at MJA is dedicated to providing authors with a constructive and collaborative peer-review and publication process. Established in 1914, the MJA has evolved into a modern journal that upholds its founding values, maintaining a commitment to supporting the medical profession by delivering high-quality and pertinent information essential to medical practice.
期刊最新文献
Consensus recommendations on multiple sclerosis management in Australia and New Zealand: part 1. Consensus recommendations on multiple sclerosis management in Australia and New Zealand: part 2. Potentially preventable medication-related hospitalisations with cardiovascular disease of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander people, Queensland, 2013-2017: a retrospective cohort study. Use of ChatGPT to obtain health information in Australia, 2024: insights from a nationally representative survey. Issue Information
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1