关于“英国智商与肥胖之间的关系”以及对体型较大的人的不合理伤害的担忧

Q3 Medicine Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.) Pub Date : 2021-02-26 DOI:10.1002/lim2.28
Sarah A. Redsell, Kiran Bains, Sarah Le Brocq, Romola Bucks, Lucie Byrne-Davis, Lesley Gray, Sarah Hotham, Marita Hennessy, Theodore K. Kyle, Amy McPherson, Fiona Quigley, Michelle Vicari, Sarah Zinn
{"title":"关于“英国智商与肥胖之间的关系”以及对体型较大的人的不合理伤害的担忧","authors":"Sarah A. Redsell,&nbsp;Kiran Bains,&nbsp;Sarah Le Brocq,&nbsp;Romola Bucks,&nbsp;Lucie Byrne-Davis,&nbsp;Lesley Gray,&nbsp;Sarah Hotham,&nbsp;Marita Hennessy,&nbsp;Theodore K. Kyle,&nbsp;Amy McPherson,&nbsp;Fiona Quigley,&nbsp;Michelle Vicari,&nbsp;Sarah Zinn","doi":"10.1002/lim2.28","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>November 26, 2020</p><p>Dr. Fraser Birrell</p><p>Editor-in-Chief, <i>Lifestyle Medicine</i></p><p>We write to express our concern about a paper you recently published in your newly established journal. The paper examines the association of a non-modifiable measure, IQ, and its relationship to adult body mass index (BMI). We are academics, health professionals, health psychologists and lay experts in weight stigma and discrimination, public health, patient advocacy and risk communication. We believe the contents of this paper are likely to cause unjustifiable harm to people in bigger bodies, some of whom may not be in a position to raise their concerns with the authors or yourselves. We further assert that there are numerous ethical and methodological issues that should be brought to your attention, which limit the applicability of the results.</p><p>This paper goes against the stated aims and the scope of your journal. First, your journal states that you “advocate the principles of sound science publishing” and that “if the science is reliable and sound, you will publish.” Yet this paper suffers a number of methodological flaws and, in particular, breaches two ethical principles, namely, beneficence and justice that significantly detract from the soundness of the science. As we demonstrate below, on this occasion your journal has not upheld good scientific principles. Second, you state that your journal “examines clinical and scientific aspects of lifestyle medicine and its incorporation into clinical practice.” This suggests that you are interested in research that identifies potentially modifiable risk factors that might be addressed in clinical practice in a way that is beneficial to people. IQ is neither a \"lifestyle\" choice nor a modifiable variable (as noted by the authors themselves). IQ is a highly heritable trait,<span><sup>1</sup></span> which can be influenced by environmental factors,<span><sup>2</sup></span> most of which are unmodifiable from an individual perspective. We outline our remaining concerns below, along with the scientific evidence that supports them.</p><p>The paper is openly available for anyone to read online, including practitioners, researchers, decision-makers, the general public, and media outlets. Indeed, such articles are often misinterpreted in the media, adding to inaccurate portrayals, and the stigmatisation and discrimination of people with bigger bodies.<span><sup>3</sup></span> The media frequently incorrectly attributes personal responsibility<span><sup>4</sup></span> to people with bigger bodies and we believe that this article feeds into an unhelpful narrative that associates weight and measures of intelligence<span><sup>5</sup></span> and policy decisions like barring children's admission to top schools because of their parents’ weight.<span><sup>6</sup></span></p><p>Publishing this study fuels negative stereotypes that people in bigger bodies lack intelligence—a dehumanizing stereotype that serves to deeply entrench discriminatory practices. There is a growing body of evidence supporting the fact that weight-based discrimination and prejudice are highly damaging<span><sup>7</sup></span> and that weight-based discrimination carries both physiological and psychological health risks.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Weight stigma has been associated with numerous adverse psychological consequences including depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, and self-isolation.<span><sup>9</sup></span> Weight stigma, rather than living in a bigger body, can lead to unhealthy diets and sedentary activity<span><sup>9</sup></span> and may also lead to chronic social stress, which has been demonstrated to lead to immunosuppression and increased risk of cardiovascular disease.<span><sup>10, 11</sup></span> It has also been associated with inequalities in access to education, healthcare settings, employment, and society.<span><sup>4, 9</sup></span> Given that weight stigma can drive poor health, it must be eradicated<span><sup>12</sup></span> and therefore the perpetuation of weight stigma in this paper and its conclusions are unacceptable. Indeed, weight stigma is such a substantial concern that an international committee has issued a joint statement calling for its eradication.<span><sup>9</sup></span> In addition, the World Health Organisation (WHO) has recognized the profound consequences of weight stigma and has responded by detailing how the European Region can address weight bias and obesity stigma.<span><sup>13</sup></span> The overwhelming evidence of the damage caused by weight discrimination and stigma and the concerns of the international community appear to have been ignored by the authors of this paper, and as a result the true magnitude of the risk to the target population have not been considered.</p><p>We note the data for this study came from the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS), which was undertaken by the University of Leicester, UK on behalf of the National Centre (NatCen) for Social Research in 2007. The survey was commissioned by NHS Digital with funding from the Department for Health and Social Care. The authors state that ethics committee approval was obtained from the Royal Free Hospital and Medical School Research Ethics Committee. We presume this is for the original 2007 survey but this is not stated. In the interests of transparency, the date and reference number of the original ethics committee approval should be provided in the paper. We have contacted NatCen to ascertain if further permissions to use the data for secondary analysis are needed. Their response indicated that permissions are not needed for the 2007 dataset, but they rely on institutional reviews of research proposals using their data. Given the serious concerns about the paper, it would be useful to determine what, if any, scrutiny by the authors’ institutions took place prior to this research taking place.</p><p>In addition to the ethical concerns outlined above, there are also several methodological issues that we wish to draw to your attention. These issues highlight the critical necessity to carefully consider and address existing stereotypical and scientific assumptions that may negatively impact research directions, methods, and conclusions. Our methodological concerns are divided into three main categories, which we review below: (1) BMI and IQ measurement, (2) the model and the conclusions drawn from it, and (3) Patient and Public Involvement (PPI).</p><p>The paper sets out to explore the risk factors for obesity and states its ultimate purpose is to determine effective prevention strategies. The findings suggest that people with lower IQs could be regularly assessed for obesity, which, as suggested above, would be a highly discriminatory practice and—combined with the fact that most people's weights and BMIs are already frequently screened throughout their lives—a highly ineffective and circuitous practice. There are no suggestions as to how this might be explored in future research and/or implemented into clinical services, although good practice guidelines are available.<span><sup>27</sup></span> The authors state that “dietitians, physiotherapists, and general practitioners” can undertake preventative screening work with people with low IQs with no acknowledgement of the complexity of this proposed activity. It is well established that healthcare professionals find it challenging to raise weight management with people,<span><sup>28</sup></span> yet their views about the relevance of this research question and the implications for their practice have not been ascertained. The authors also suggest that “IQ may be regularly assessed in specific situations such as the follow up of children with development difficulties or the follow up of adults with psychiatry disorders” without considering the impact of this statement on people in these distinct groups.</p><p>Given our numerous, evidence-based concerns, we do not believe the paper meets the journal's criteria for publication. We seek its retraction or, failing that, the publication of this letter alongside it to address the balance.</p><p>Yours faithfully</p><p>Dr. Sarah A. Redsell, Honorary Associate Professor, University of Nottingham, UK</p><p>Dr. Kiran K. Bains, Long Term Conditions Lead in IAPT, Honorary Research Fellow, City, University of London, UK</p><p>Sarah Le Brocq, Director of Obesity UK</p><p>Romola S. Bucks, FAPS, Professor in Psychology, University of Western Australia, Australia</p><p>Lucie Byrne-Davis, Professor of Health Psychology, University of Manchester, UK</p><p>Lesley Gray FFPH, Senior Lecturer, University of Otago, New Zealand</p><p>Sarah Hotham, Senior Research Fellow, Centre for Health Services Studies, University of Kent, UK</p><p>Marita Hennessy, PhD, Postdoctoral Researcher, College of Medicine and Health, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland</p><p>Theodore K. Kyle, RPh, MBA, Founder ConscienHealth, USA</p><p>Dr. Amy McPherson, Senior Scientist, Bloorview Research Institute, Toronto, Canada</p><p>Fiona Quigley, PhD Candidate, Ulster University, Belfast, Northern Ireland</p><p>Michelle Vicari, National Board Chair, Obesity Action Coalition, USA</p><p>Sarah Renea Zinn, PhD Candidate, University of Chicago, USA</p><p>I declare that I have no competing relationships or conflicts of interest. Under ICMJE I led the response, drafted the original letter, revised it according to the feedback I received and submitted it to the journal. I approve of the final version and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the letter.</p>","PeriodicalId":74076,"journal":{"name":"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2021-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1002/lim2.28","citationCount":"1","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Concerns regarding “Association between intelligence quotient and obesity in England” and unjustifiable harm to people in bigger bodies\",\"authors\":\"Sarah A. Redsell,&nbsp;Kiran Bains,&nbsp;Sarah Le Brocq,&nbsp;Romola Bucks,&nbsp;Lucie Byrne-Davis,&nbsp;Lesley Gray,&nbsp;Sarah Hotham,&nbsp;Marita Hennessy,&nbsp;Theodore K. Kyle,&nbsp;Amy McPherson,&nbsp;Fiona Quigley,&nbsp;Michelle Vicari,&nbsp;Sarah Zinn\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/lim2.28\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>November 26, 2020</p><p>Dr. Fraser Birrell</p><p>Editor-in-Chief, <i>Lifestyle Medicine</i></p><p>We write to express our concern about a paper you recently published in your newly established journal. The paper examines the association of a non-modifiable measure, IQ, and its relationship to adult body mass index (BMI). We are academics, health professionals, health psychologists and lay experts in weight stigma and discrimination, public health, patient advocacy and risk communication. We believe the contents of this paper are likely to cause unjustifiable harm to people in bigger bodies, some of whom may not be in a position to raise their concerns with the authors or yourselves. We further assert that there are numerous ethical and methodological issues that should be brought to your attention, which limit the applicability of the results.</p><p>This paper goes against the stated aims and the scope of your journal. First, your journal states that you “advocate the principles of sound science publishing” and that “if the science is reliable and sound, you will publish.” Yet this paper suffers a number of methodological flaws and, in particular, breaches two ethical principles, namely, beneficence and justice that significantly detract from the soundness of the science. As we demonstrate below, on this occasion your journal has not upheld good scientific principles. Second, you state that your journal “examines clinical and scientific aspects of lifestyle medicine and its incorporation into clinical practice.” This suggests that you are interested in research that identifies potentially modifiable risk factors that might be addressed in clinical practice in a way that is beneficial to people. IQ is neither a \\\"lifestyle\\\" choice nor a modifiable variable (as noted by the authors themselves). IQ is a highly heritable trait,<span><sup>1</sup></span> which can be influenced by environmental factors,<span><sup>2</sup></span> most of which are unmodifiable from an individual perspective. We outline our remaining concerns below, along with the scientific evidence that supports them.</p><p>The paper is openly available for anyone to read online, including practitioners, researchers, decision-makers, the general public, and media outlets. Indeed, such articles are often misinterpreted in the media, adding to inaccurate portrayals, and the stigmatisation and discrimination of people with bigger bodies.<span><sup>3</sup></span> The media frequently incorrectly attributes personal responsibility<span><sup>4</sup></span> to people with bigger bodies and we believe that this article feeds into an unhelpful narrative that associates weight and measures of intelligence<span><sup>5</sup></span> and policy decisions like barring children's admission to top schools because of their parents’ weight.<span><sup>6</sup></span></p><p>Publishing this study fuels negative stereotypes that people in bigger bodies lack intelligence—a dehumanizing stereotype that serves to deeply entrench discriminatory practices. There is a growing body of evidence supporting the fact that weight-based discrimination and prejudice are highly damaging<span><sup>7</sup></span> and that weight-based discrimination carries both physiological and psychological health risks.<span><sup>8</sup></span> Weight stigma has been associated with numerous adverse psychological consequences including depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, and self-isolation.<span><sup>9</sup></span> Weight stigma, rather than living in a bigger body, can lead to unhealthy diets and sedentary activity<span><sup>9</sup></span> and may also lead to chronic social stress, which has been demonstrated to lead to immunosuppression and increased risk of cardiovascular disease.<span><sup>10, 11</sup></span> It has also been associated with inequalities in access to education, healthcare settings, employment, and society.<span><sup>4, 9</sup></span> Given that weight stigma can drive poor health, it must be eradicated<span><sup>12</sup></span> and therefore the perpetuation of weight stigma in this paper and its conclusions are unacceptable. Indeed, weight stigma is such a substantial concern that an international committee has issued a joint statement calling for its eradication.<span><sup>9</sup></span> In addition, the World Health Organisation (WHO) has recognized the profound consequences of weight stigma and has responded by detailing how the European Region can address weight bias and obesity stigma.<span><sup>13</sup></span> The overwhelming evidence of the damage caused by weight discrimination and stigma and the concerns of the international community appear to have been ignored by the authors of this paper, and as a result the true magnitude of the risk to the target population have not been considered.</p><p>We note the data for this study came from the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS), which was undertaken by the University of Leicester, UK on behalf of the National Centre (NatCen) for Social Research in 2007. The survey was commissioned by NHS Digital with funding from the Department for Health and Social Care. The authors state that ethics committee approval was obtained from the Royal Free Hospital and Medical School Research Ethics Committee. We presume this is for the original 2007 survey but this is not stated. In the interests of transparency, the date and reference number of the original ethics committee approval should be provided in the paper. We have contacted NatCen to ascertain if further permissions to use the data for secondary analysis are needed. Their response indicated that permissions are not needed for the 2007 dataset, but they rely on institutional reviews of research proposals using their data. Given the serious concerns about the paper, it would be useful to determine what, if any, scrutiny by the authors’ institutions took place prior to this research taking place.</p><p>In addition to the ethical concerns outlined above, there are also several methodological issues that we wish to draw to your attention. These issues highlight the critical necessity to carefully consider and address existing stereotypical and scientific assumptions that may negatively impact research directions, methods, and conclusions. Our methodological concerns are divided into three main categories, which we review below: (1) BMI and IQ measurement, (2) the model and the conclusions drawn from it, and (3) Patient and Public Involvement (PPI).</p><p>The paper sets out to explore the risk factors for obesity and states its ultimate purpose is to determine effective prevention strategies. The findings suggest that people with lower IQs could be regularly assessed for obesity, which, as suggested above, would be a highly discriminatory practice and—combined with the fact that most people's weights and BMIs are already frequently screened throughout their lives—a highly ineffective and circuitous practice. There are no suggestions as to how this might be explored in future research and/or implemented into clinical services, although good practice guidelines are available.<span><sup>27</sup></span> The authors state that “dietitians, physiotherapists, and general practitioners” can undertake preventative screening work with people with low IQs with no acknowledgement of the complexity of this proposed activity. It is well established that healthcare professionals find it challenging to raise weight management with people,<span><sup>28</sup></span> yet their views about the relevance of this research question and the implications for their practice have not been ascertained. The authors also suggest that “IQ may be regularly assessed in specific situations such as the follow up of children with development difficulties or the follow up of adults with psychiatry disorders” without considering the impact of this statement on people in these distinct groups.</p><p>Given our numerous, evidence-based concerns, we do not believe the paper meets the journal's criteria for publication. We seek its retraction or, failing that, the publication of this letter alongside it to address the balance.</p><p>Yours faithfully</p><p>Dr. Sarah A. Redsell, Honorary Associate Professor, University of Nottingham, UK</p><p>Dr. Kiran K. Bains, Long Term Conditions Lead in IAPT, Honorary Research Fellow, City, University of London, UK</p><p>Sarah Le Brocq, Director of Obesity UK</p><p>Romola S. Bucks, FAPS, Professor in Psychology, University of Western Australia, Australia</p><p>Lucie Byrne-Davis, Professor of Health Psychology, University of Manchester, UK</p><p>Lesley Gray FFPH, Senior Lecturer, University of Otago, New Zealand</p><p>Sarah Hotham, Senior Research Fellow, Centre for Health Services Studies, University of Kent, UK</p><p>Marita Hennessy, PhD, Postdoctoral Researcher, College of Medicine and Health, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland</p><p>Theodore K. Kyle, RPh, MBA, Founder ConscienHealth, USA</p><p>Dr. Amy McPherson, Senior Scientist, Bloorview Research Institute, Toronto, Canada</p><p>Fiona Quigley, PhD Candidate, Ulster University, Belfast, Northern Ireland</p><p>Michelle Vicari, National Board Chair, Obesity Action Coalition, USA</p><p>Sarah Renea Zinn, PhD Candidate, University of Chicago, USA</p><p>I declare that I have no competing relationships or conflicts of interest. Under ICMJE I led the response, drafted the original letter, revised it according to the feedback I received and submitted it to the journal. I approve of the final version and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the letter.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":74076,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2021-02-26\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1002/lim2.28\",\"citationCount\":\"1\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/lim2.28\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"Medicine\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/lim2.28","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"Medicine","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

摘要

2020年11月26日我们写信是为了表达我们对您最近在新创办的期刊上发表的一篇论文的关注。本文研究了一个不可改变的测量,智商,以及它与成人体重指数(BMI)的关系。我们是体重耻辱和歧视、公共卫生、患者宣传和风险沟通方面的学者、卫生专业人员、健康心理学家和非专业专家。我们相信这篇论文的内容可能会对体型较大的人造成不合理的伤害,其中一些人可能无法向作者或你自己提出他们的担忧。我们进一步断言,应该提请您注意许多伦理和方法问题,这些问题限制了结果的适用性。这篇论文违背了你的期刊的既定目标和范围。首先,你的期刊声明你“提倡合理的科学出版原则”,并且“如果科学是可靠和合理的,你就会发表。”然而,这篇论文在方法论上存在许多缺陷,特别是违反了两个伦理原则,即慈善和正义,这大大损害了科学的合理性。正如我们下面所展示的,在这种情况下,你们的期刊没有坚持良好的科学原则。第二,你说你的期刊“研究生活方式医学的临床和科学方面,并将其纳入临床实践。”这表明你对识别潜在可改变的风险因素的研究感兴趣,这些因素可能在临床实践中以一种对人们有益的方式加以解决。智商既不是一种“生活方式”的选择,也不是一个可修改的变量(正如作者自己所指出的)。智商是一种高度可遗传的特征,它会受到环境因素的影响,而从个人角度来看,大多数环境因素是无法改变的。我们在下面列出了我们剩下的担忧,以及支持这些担忧的科学证据。任何人都可以在网上公开阅读,包括从业人员、研究人员、决策者、公众和媒体。事实上,这样的文章经常被媒体误解,增加了不准确的描述,以及对身体较大的人的侮辱和歧视媒体经常错误地把个人责任归咎于体型较大的人。我们认为,这篇文章助长了一种无益的叙述,把体重、智力的衡量标准与一些政策决定联系在一起,比如因为父母的体重而不让孩子进入顶尖学校。发表这一研究助长了人们认为体型较大的人缺乏智力的负面刻板印象——这种非人性化的刻板印象使歧视行为根深蒂固。越来越多的证据支持这样一个事实:基于体重的歧视和偏见极具破坏性,并且基于体重的歧视会给生理和心理健康带来风险体重耻辱感与许多不良心理后果有关,包括抑郁、焦虑、自卑和自我孤立体重的耻辱,而不是生活在一个更大的身体里,会导致不健康的饮食和久坐不动的活动,还可能导致长期的社会压力,这已被证明会导致免疫抑制和心血管疾病的风险增加。10,11它还与获得教育、保健、就业和社会机会方面的不平等有关。考虑到对体重的歧视会导致健康状况不佳,必须消除这种歧视。因此,这篇论文及其结论中对体重歧视的持续存在是不可接受的。事实上,体重的耻辱是如此令人担忧,以至于一个国际委员会发表了一项联合声明,呼吁消除这种耻辱此外,世界卫生组织(世卫组织)已经认识到体重歧视的深刻后果,并对此作出回应,详细说明了欧洲区域如何能够解决体重偏见和肥胖歧视问题这篇论文的作者似乎忽视了体重歧视和耻辱造成损害的压倒性证据,以及国际社会的担忧,因此没有考虑到对目标人群风险的真实程度。我们注意到这项研究的数据来自成人精神病发病率调查(APMS),该调查是由英国莱斯特大学代表国家社会研究中心(NatCen)于2007年进行的。这项调查是由NHS Digital委托进行的,由卫生和社会保障部提供资金。提交人指出,伦理委员会已获得皇家自由医院和医学院研究伦理委员会的批准。我们假定这是针对2007年的原始调查,但没有说明。 为了透明起见,应在论文中提供原始伦理委员会批准的日期和参考编号。我们已经联系了NatCen,以确定是否需要进一步的许可才能使用这些数据进行二次分析。他们的回应表明,2007年的数据集不需要许可,但他们依赖于机构对使用他们数据的研究提案的审查。考虑到人们对这篇论文的严重担忧,确定作者所在机构在进行这项研究之前进行了哪些审查(如果有的话)将是有用的。除了上述伦理问题外,我们还希望提请您注意几个方法问题。这些问题强调了仔细考虑和解决可能对研究方向、方法和结论产生负面影响的现有刻板印象和科学假设的关键必要性。我们关注的方法分为三个主要类别,我们回顾如下:(1)BMI和IQ测量,(2)模型和从中得出的结论,以及(3)患者和公众参与(PPI)。这篇论文旨在探讨肥胖的危险因素,并指出其最终目的是确定有效的预防策略。研究结果表明,可以定期对智商较低的人进行肥胖评估,正如上面所建议的那样,这将是一种高度歧视性的做法,而且大多数人的体重和bmi已经在他们的一生中经常进行筛查,这是一种非常无效和迂回的做法。虽然有良好的实践指南,但对于如何在未来的研究中探索和/或在临床服务中实施这一点,尚无建议作者指出,“营养师、物理治疗师和全科医生”可以对低智商人群进行预防性筛查工作,而不需要意识到这项拟议活动的复杂性。众所周知,医疗保健专业人员发现向人们提出体重管理是一项挑战,但他们对这一研究问题的相关性及其对他们实践的影响的看法尚未得到确定。作者还建议,“可以在特定情况下定期评估智商,比如对有发育困难的儿童进行随访,或对有精神障碍的成年人进行随访”,而不考虑这一说法对这些不同群体的人的影响。考虑到我们众多的、基于证据的担忧,我们认为这篇论文不符合该杂志的出版标准。我们要求撤回这封信,如果做不到这一点,我们将同时发表这封信,以解决平衡问题。你的faithfullyDr。Sarah A. Redsell,英国诺丁汉大学名誉副教授。Kiran K. Bains, IAPT长期条件负责人,英国伦敦大学城市名誉研究员sarah Le Brocq,肥胖症研究主任romola S. Bucks, FAPS,澳大利亚西澳大利亚大学心理学教授lucie Byrne-Davis,英国曼彻斯特大学健康心理学教授lesley Gray FFPH,新西兰奥塔哥大学高级讲师sarah Hotham,英国肯特大学健康服务研究中心高级研究员marita Hennessy博士博士后研究员,医学与健康学院,科克大学,科克,爱尔兰西奥多K.凯尔,注册哲学博士,工商管理硕士,创始人良心健康,科克,美国。Amy McPherson,加拿大多伦多Bloorview研究所资深科学家;fiona Quigley,北爱尔兰贝尔法斯特阿尔斯特大学博士候选人;michelle Vicari,美国肥胖行动联盟国家委员会主席;sarah Renea Zinn,美国芝加哥大学博士候选人声明我没有竞争关系或利益冲突。在ICMJE的领导下,我领导了回复,起草了原始信件,根据收到的反馈对其进行了修改,并将其提交给了期刊。我同意最终版本,并同意对这封信的各个方面负责。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Concerns regarding “Association between intelligence quotient and obesity in England” and unjustifiable harm to people in bigger bodies

November 26, 2020

Dr. Fraser Birrell

Editor-in-Chief, Lifestyle Medicine

We write to express our concern about a paper you recently published in your newly established journal. The paper examines the association of a non-modifiable measure, IQ, and its relationship to adult body mass index (BMI). We are academics, health professionals, health psychologists and lay experts in weight stigma and discrimination, public health, patient advocacy and risk communication. We believe the contents of this paper are likely to cause unjustifiable harm to people in bigger bodies, some of whom may not be in a position to raise their concerns with the authors or yourselves. We further assert that there are numerous ethical and methodological issues that should be brought to your attention, which limit the applicability of the results.

This paper goes against the stated aims and the scope of your journal. First, your journal states that you “advocate the principles of sound science publishing” and that “if the science is reliable and sound, you will publish.” Yet this paper suffers a number of methodological flaws and, in particular, breaches two ethical principles, namely, beneficence and justice that significantly detract from the soundness of the science. As we demonstrate below, on this occasion your journal has not upheld good scientific principles. Second, you state that your journal “examines clinical and scientific aspects of lifestyle medicine and its incorporation into clinical practice.” This suggests that you are interested in research that identifies potentially modifiable risk factors that might be addressed in clinical practice in a way that is beneficial to people. IQ is neither a "lifestyle" choice nor a modifiable variable (as noted by the authors themselves). IQ is a highly heritable trait,1 which can be influenced by environmental factors,2 most of which are unmodifiable from an individual perspective. We outline our remaining concerns below, along with the scientific evidence that supports them.

The paper is openly available for anyone to read online, including practitioners, researchers, decision-makers, the general public, and media outlets. Indeed, such articles are often misinterpreted in the media, adding to inaccurate portrayals, and the stigmatisation and discrimination of people with bigger bodies.3 The media frequently incorrectly attributes personal responsibility4 to people with bigger bodies and we believe that this article feeds into an unhelpful narrative that associates weight and measures of intelligence5 and policy decisions like barring children's admission to top schools because of their parents’ weight.6

Publishing this study fuels negative stereotypes that people in bigger bodies lack intelligence—a dehumanizing stereotype that serves to deeply entrench discriminatory practices. There is a growing body of evidence supporting the fact that weight-based discrimination and prejudice are highly damaging7 and that weight-based discrimination carries both physiological and psychological health risks.8 Weight stigma has been associated with numerous adverse psychological consequences including depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, and self-isolation.9 Weight stigma, rather than living in a bigger body, can lead to unhealthy diets and sedentary activity9 and may also lead to chronic social stress, which has been demonstrated to lead to immunosuppression and increased risk of cardiovascular disease.10, 11 It has also been associated with inequalities in access to education, healthcare settings, employment, and society.4, 9 Given that weight stigma can drive poor health, it must be eradicated12 and therefore the perpetuation of weight stigma in this paper and its conclusions are unacceptable. Indeed, weight stigma is such a substantial concern that an international committee has issued a joint statement calling for its eradication.9 In addition, the World Health Organisation (WHO) has recognized the profound consequences of weight stigma and has responded by detailing how the European Region can address weight bias and obesity stigma.13 The overwhelming evidence of the damage caused by weight discrimination and stigma and the concerns of the international community appear to have been ignored by the authors of this paper, and as a result the true magnitude of the risk to the target population have not been considered.

We note the data for this study came from the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS), which was undertaken by the University of Leicester, UK on behalf of the National Centre (NatCen) for Social Research in 2007. The survey was commissioned by NHS Digital with funding from the Department for Health and Social Care. The authors state that ethics committee approval was obtained from the Royal Free Hospital and Medical School Research Ethics Committee. We presume this is for the original 2007 survey but this is not stated. In the interests of transparency, the date and reference number of the original ethics committee approval should be provided in the paper. We have contacted NatCen to ascertain if further permissions to use the data for secondary analysis are needed. Their response indicated that permissions are not needed for the 2007 dataset, but they rely on institutional reviews of research proposals using their data. Given the serious concerns about the paper, it would be useful to determine what, if any, scrutiny by the authors’ institutions took place prior to this research taking place.

In addition to the ethical concerns outlined above, there are also several methodological issues that we wish to draw to your attention. These issues highlight the critical necessity to carefully consider and address existing stereotypical and scientific assumptions that may negatively impact research directions, methods, and conclusions. Our methodological concerns are divided into three main categories, which we review below: (1) BMI and IQ measurement, (2) the model and the conclusions drawn from it, and (3) Patient and Public Involvement (PPI).

The paper sets out to explore the risk factors for obesity and states its ultimate purpose is to determine effective prevention strategies. The findings suggest that people with lower IQs could be regularly assessed for obesity, which, as suggested above, would be a highly discriminatory practice and—combined with the fact that most people's weights and BMIs are already frequently screened throughout their lives—a highly ineffective and circuitous practice. There are no suggestions as to how this might be explored in future research and/or implemented into clinical services, although good practice guidelines are available.27 The authors state that “dietitians, physiotherapists, and general practitioners” can undertake preventative screening work with people with low IQs with no acknowledgement of the complexity of this proposed activity. It is well established that healthcare professionals find it challenging to raise weight management with people,28 yet their views about the relevance of this research question and the implications for their practice have not been ascertained. The authors also suggest that “IQ may be regularly assessed in specific situations such as the follow up of children with development difficulties or the follow up of adults with psychiatry disorders” without considering the impact of this statement on people in these distinct groups.

Given our numerous, evidence-based concerns, we do not believe the paper meets the journal's criteria for publication. We seek its retraction or, failing that, the publication of this letter alongside it to address the balance.

Yours faithfully

Dr. Sarah A. Redsell, Honorary Associate Professor, University of Nottingham, UK

Dr. Kiran K. Bains, Long Term Conditions Lead in IAPT, Honorary Research Fellow, City, University of London, UK

Sarah Le Brocq, Director of Obesity UK

Romola S. Bucks, FAPS, Professor in Psychology, University of Western Australia, Australia

Lucie Byrne-Davis, Professor of Health Psychology, University of Manchester, UK

Lesley Gray FFPH, Senior Lecturer, University of Otago, New Zealand

Sarah Hotham, Senior Research Fellow, Centre for Health Services Studies, University of Kent, UK

Marita Hennessy, PhD, Postdoctoral Researcher, College of Medicine and Health, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland

Theodore K. Kyle, RPh, MBA, Founder ConscienHealth, USA

Dr. Amy McPherson, Senior Scientist, Bloorview Research Institute, Toronto, Canada

Fiona Quigley, PhD Candidate, Ulster University, Belfast, Northern Ireland

Michelle Vicari, National Board Chair, Obesity Action Coalition, USA

Sarah Renea Zinn, PhD Candidate, University of Chicago, USA

I declare that I have no competing relationships or conflicts of interest. Under ICMJE I led the response, drafted the original letter, revised it according to the feedback I received and submitted it to the journal. I approve of the final version and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the letter.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
2.10
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
审稿时长
7 weeks
期刊最新文献
Issue Information Meeting Abstract Issue Information Analysis of Canadian Physician Obituaries Between 2000 and 2023 to Investigate Trends in Death Between Specialties: A Retrospective Cross-Sectional Study Defining the Traditional Mediterranean Lifestyle: Joint International Consensus Statement
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1