种群动力学中耦合模型的零可控性

IF 0.3 Q4 MATHEMATICS Mathematica Bohemica Pub Date : 2022-08-04 DOI:10.21136/mb.2022.0088-21
Y. Echarroudi
{"title":"种群动力学中耦合模型的零可控性","authors":"Y. Echarroudi","doi":"10.21136/mb.2022.0088-21","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"ly, a functional response can be defined as the relationship between an individual’s rate of consumption (here we talk about a consumption of predator) and food’s density (i.e., prey’s density). This amounts to saying that a functional response reflects the capture ability of the predator to prey or in other words, the functional response is introduced to describe the change in the rate of consumption of prey by predator when the density of prey varies. In the plotting point of view, each type of functional response I, II or III has a special characteristic. In fact, type I, or the linear case of the predator response, is the situation when the plot of the number of prey consumed (per unit of time) as a function of prey density shows a linear relationship between the number of prey consumed and the prey density. The Holling type II, called also concave upward response, is the case when the gradient of the curve decreases monotonically with increasing prey density, probably saturating at a constant value of prey consumption. For information, the Lotka-Volterra model involving this functional response is known as the Rosenzweig-MacArthur model. The type III response is known between the specialists of population dynamics as the sigmoid response having a concave downward part at low food density. Actually, for the Holling III, a sigmoidal behavior occurs when the gradient of the curve first increases and then decreases with increasing prey density. This behavior is due to the “learning behavior” in the predator population. Now, we address some “ecological” interpretations of the three first Holling types functional responses. The type I response is the result of simple assumption that the probability of a given predator (usually the passive one) encountering prey in a fixed time interval [0, Tt] within a fixed spatial region depends linearly on the prey density. This can be expressed under the form Y = aTsX, where Y is the amount of prey consumed by one predator, X is the prey density, Ts is the time available for searching and a is a constant of proportionality, termed as the discovery rate (which is in our case represented by the parameter b). In the absence of need to spend time handling the prey, all the time can be used for searching, i.e., Ts = Tt, and we have the type I response: assuming that the predators (having the density P ) act independently, in time Tt the total amount of prey will be reduced by quantity aTtXP . In addition, if each predator requires a handling time h for each individual prey that is consumed, the time available for searching Ts is reduced: Ts = Tt−hY . Taking into account the expression of Y in response type I, this leads to Y = aTtX − ahXY and this implies Y = aTtX/(1 + ahX) and this is exactly the type II response. Therefore, in the interval [0, Tt] the total amount of prey is reduced by the quantity aTtXP/(1 + ahX). Let us point out that the term “ah” is dimensionless and can be interpreted as the ability of a generic predator to kill and consume a generic prey and it possesses the following characteristics times: “ah” is large if the handling time h is much longer than the typical discovery time 1/a and “ah” is small in the opposite limit; in this case 8 Online first the type II response is reduced to type I. The Holling type III functional response can be viewed as a generalization of type II and takes the form aTtX /(1 + ahX) with k > 1. In literature, this response is stimulated by supposing that learning behavior occurs in the predator population with a consequent increase in the discovery rate as more encounters with prey occurs (see [25] for more details). To see to wingspan of the Lotka-Volterra models from many sides of investigations, we provide a nonexhaustive list of some works dealing with crucial questions, which are discussed widely. We begin with the system whose functional response is Holling I. One of the important problems which takes a special attention, is the study of the steady states and more accurately, in [40] a prey-predator system with nonlinear diffusion effects is considered. Such nonlinear diffusion effects have an impact on a biological species as well as their resource-biomass (i.e., the capacity of their environment). Herein, the workers assume that the dispersive force and the diffusion depend on population pressure from other species. The question of equilibrium of Lotka-Volterra systems with Holling type I functional type response takes also a broad study theoretically and numerically in [54], specially the interior one, as well as their dynamical behavior such as the cyclic-fold, saddle-fold, homoclinic saddle connection. The Holling I introduced here is from the range of the so-called Beddington-DeAngelis functional response. Remaining in the type I, the authors in [44] tried to prove the existence of an asymptotically stable pest-eradication for a prey-predator system modeled by an ordinary differential equation, when the impulsive period is some critical value less by implementing Floquet theorem and a small amplitude perturbation method. Such a solution of eradication is somehow the mixing between a synthetic strategy (insecticides or pesticides for instance) and biological control, e.g. the natural enemies “killing” the dangerous pests (the prey here) without causing a serious damages to the two population densities (see also [59] for a similar study). Even the similarity appearance between their curves, functional responses of type I and type II have two considerable differences: the first one was pointed out before and it concerns the predator time handling of prey. Contrary to the Holling type II, the time handling is missed for predators in type I, which means that the consumers have a little difficulty capturing and assimilating prey but they switch their time to other activities once their ingestion rate is great enough to satisfy their energetic needs. The second difference is in the dynamical behavior. In fact, while the Holling type II displays the local Hopf bifurcation, the Holling type I makes clear a global cyclic-fold bifurcation. These differences between Holling I and Holling II, in particular the first, lead a numerous works to take into account the predator time handling in their different models. We emphasize here that Holling II possesses a generalization, which is exactly Beddington-DeAngelis functional response cited previously. This functional takes the form ΦBD(N,P ) = cN/(e+N + h1P ), where N Online first 9 and P should be the densities of prey and predator, while e stands for the halfsaturation constant, i.e., the amount of prey at which the per capita predation rate is half of its maximum c and h1 is a positive constant (see [54] for further details about this functional response). Among the works interested in Lotka-Volterra with Holling type II we cite for instance [48], [55]. In [55], a statistical study was presented to see if one can replace Holling type II by functional response from the type of Beddington-DeAngelis, Crowly-Martin or Hassel-Valey model for a divers cases related to the predator feeding rate. Peng et al. in [48] were concerned with the question of the steady-states of some reaction-diffusion models and they established the non-existence of a non-constant equilibrium solutions of two prey-predator systems with Holling II when the interaction between the two populations is strong as they claimed and where the constant measuring the ability of generic predator to kill and consume generic prey is equal to 1. By the way, a wide classical ecological literature assumed that mathematical models with Holling type II (or in general the non-sigmoid) functional response involving a diffusion terms match thoroughly in description of the pattern formation of a phytoplankton-zooplankton system. The affirmative answers are basically related to experiments realized in laboratories on zooplankton feeding, which are carried out in small-sized containers or bottles. But if one wants to investigate zooplankton grazing control in real ecosystems (may be the oceans), it will be more relevant to introduce the Holling type III response as stated in the introduction of [46]. Actually, the main focus of [46] was to set a generic model which explains the observed alteration of type between the different functional responses of plankton systems and gives, as he presumed, an evidence that for such a system the Holling type III is more adequate than other kinds. In the vocation of well-posedness, the global existence, uniqueness and the boundedness of a strong solution of partial differential equation with a special case of Holling III was brought out in [11]. This strong solution was approximated numerically using a spectral method and a Runge-Kutta time solver. The modelling using the Holling type III does not stop here, it can play also a crucial role to model the entomophagous species (see [16] for more details). But when a functional response describes the interaction between predator and prey when the prey exhibits group defense (like buffalo) or has ability to hide itself (like chameleon), then we talk about the Holling type IV functional response or the so-called Monod-Haldane function. This function takes the form mX/(γ + b1X +X ), where X is the prey density, m > 0 is the complete saturation, whereas γ and b1 denote, respectively, the half-saturation constant and b1 the prey environmental carrying capacity. A space independent system of LotkaVolterra kind using type IV was under consideration in [45] and the principal purpose of this item is to assess the impact of the harvesting on equilibria of both prey and 10 Online first predator populations. The quandaries used here are, as the authors cited, based on the dynamical theory combined with a technique of Hopf bifurcation. A numerical analysis is provided to compare the dependence of the dynamical behavior on the harvesting effort for the prey between Holl","PeriodicalId":45392,"journal":{"name":"Mathematica Bohemica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3000,"publicationDate":"2022-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"1","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Null controllability of a coupled model in population dynamics\",\"authors\":\"Y. Echarroudi\",\"doi\":\"10.21136/mb.2022.0088-21\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"ly, a functional response can be defined as the relationship between an individual’s rate of consumption (here we talk about a consumption of predator) and food’s density (i.e., prey’s density). This amounts to saying that a functional response reflects the capture ability of the predator to prey or in other words, the functional response is introduced to describe the change in the rate of consumption of prey by predator when the density of prey varies. In the plotting point of view, each type of functional response I, II or III has a special characteristic. In fact, type I, or the linear case of the predator response, is the situation when the plot of the number of prey consumed (per unit of time) as a function of prey density shows a linear relationship between the number of prey consumed and the prey density. The Holling type II, called also concave upward response, is the case when the gradient of the curve decreases monotonically with increasing prey density, probably saturating at a constant value of prey consumption. For information, the Lotka-Volterra model involving this functional response is known as the Rosenzweig-MacArthur model. The type III response is known between the specialists of population dynamics as the sigmoid response having a concave downward part at low food density. Actually, for the Holling III, a sigmoidal behavior occurs when the gradient of the curve first increases and then decreases with increasing prey density. This behavior is due to the “learning behavior” in the predator population. Now, we address some “ecological” interpretations of the three first Holling types functional responses. The type I response is the result of simple assumption that the probability of a given predator (usually the passive one) encountering prey in a fixed time interval [0, Tt] within a fixed spatial region depends linearly on the prey density. This can be expressed under the form Y = aTsX, where Y is the amount of prey consumed by one predator, X is the prey density, Ts is the time available for searching and a is a constant of proportionality, termed as the discovery rate (which is in our case represented by the parameter b). In the absence of need to spend time handling the prey, all the time can be used for searching, i.e., Ts = Tt, and we have the type I response: assuming that the predators (having the density P ) act independently, in time Tt the total amount of prey will be reduced by quantity aTtXP . In addition, if each predator requires a handling time h for each individual prey that is consumed, the time available for searching Ts is reduced: Ts = Tt−hY . Taking into account the expression of Y in response type I, this leads to Y = aTtX − ahXY and this implies Y = aTtX/(1 + ahX) and this is exactly the type II response. Therefore, in the interval [0, Tt] the total amount of prey is reduced by the quantity aTtXP/(1 + ahX). Let us point out that the term “ah” is dimensionless and can be interpreted as the ability of a generic predator to kill and consume a generic prey and it possesses the following characteristics times: “ah” is large if the handling time h is much longer than the typical discovery time 1/a and “ah” is small in the opposite limit; in this case 8 Online first the type II response is reduced to type I. The Holling type III functional response can be viewed as a generalization of type II and takes the form aTtX /(1 + ahX) with k > 1. In literature, this response is stimulated by supposing that learning behavior occurs in the predator population with a consequent increase in the discovery rate as more encounters with prey occurs (see [25] for more details). To see to wingspan of the Lotka-Volterra models from many sides of investigations, we provide a nonexhaustive list of some works dealing with crucial questions, which are discussed widely. We begin with the system whose functional response is Holling I. One of the important problems which takes a special attention, is the study of the steady states and more accurately, in [40] a prey-predator system with nonlinear diffusion effects is considered. Such nonlinear diffusion effects have an impact on a biological species as well as their resource-biomass (i.e., the capacity of their environment). Herein, the workers assume that the dispersive force and the diffusion depend on population pressure from other species. The question of equilibrium of Lotka-Volterra systems with Holling type I functional type response takes also a broad study theoretically and numerically in [54], specially the interior one, as well as their dynamical behavior such as the cyclic-fold, saddle-fold, homoclinic saddle connection. The Holling I introduced here is from the range of the so-called Beddington-DeAngelis functional response. Remaining in the type I, the authors in [44] tried to prove the existence of an asymptotically stable pest-eradication for a prey-predator system modeled by an ordinary differential equation, when the impulsive period is some critical value less by implementing Floquet theorem and a small amplitude perturbation method. Such a solution of eradication is somehow the mixing between a synthetic strategy (insecticides or pesticides for instance) and biological control, e.g. the natural enemies “killing” the dangerous pests (the prey here) without causing a serious damages to the two population densities (see also [59] for a similar study). Even the similarity appearance between their curves, functional responses of type I and type II have two considerable differences: the first one was pointed out before and it concerns the predator time handling of prey. Contrary to the Holling type II, the time handling is missed for predators in type I, which means that the consumers have a little difficulty capturing and assimilating prey but they switch their time to other activities once their ingestion rate is great enough to satisfy their energetic needs. The second difference is in the dynamical behavior. In fact, while the Holling type II displays the local Hopf bifurcation, the Holling type I makes clear a global cyclic-fold bifurcation. These differences between Holling I and Holling II, in particular the first, lead a numerous works to take into account the predator time handling in their different models. We emphasize here that Holling II possesses a generalization, which is exactly Beddington-DeAngelis functional response cited previously. This functional takes the form ΦBD(N,P ) = cN/(e+N + h1P ), where N Online first 9 and P should be the densities of prey and predator, while e stands for the halfsaturation constant, i.e., the amount of prey at which the per capita predation rate is half of its maximum c and h1 is a positive constant (see [54] for further details about this functional response). Among the works interested in Lotka-Volterra with Holling type II we cite for instance [48], [55]. In [55], a statistical study was presented to see if one can replace Holling type II by functional response from the type of Beddington-DeAngelis, Crowly-Martin or Hassel-Valey model for a divers cases related to the predator feeding rate. Peng et al. in [48] were concerned with the question of the steady-states of some reaction-diffusion models and they established the non-existence of a non-constant equilibrium solutions of two prey-predator systems with Holling II when the interaction between the two populations is strong as they claimed and where the constant measuring the ability of generic predator to kill and consume generic prey is equal to 1. By the way, a wide classical ecological literature assumed that mathematical models with Holling type II (or in general the non-sigmoid) functional response involving a diffusion terms match thoroughly in description of the pattern formation of a phytoplankton-zooplankton system. The affirmative answers are basically related to experiments realized in laboratories on zooplankton feeding, which are carried out in small-sized containers or bottles. But if one wants to investigate zooplankton grazing control in real ecosystems (may be the oceans), it will be more relevant to introduce the Holling type III response as stated in the introduction of [46]. Actually, the main focus of [46] was to set a generic model which explains the observed alteration of type between the different functional responses of plankton systems and gives, as he presumed, an evidence that for such a system the Holling type III is more adequate than other kinds. In the vocation of well-posedness, the global existence, uniqueness and the boundedness of a strong solution of partial differential equation with a special case of Holling III was brought out in [11]. This strong solution was approximated numerically using a spectral method and a Runge-Kutta time solver. The modelling using the Holling type III does not stop here, it can play also a crucial role to model the entomophagous species (see [16] for more details). But when a functional response describes the interaction between predator and prey when the prey exhibits group defense (like buffalo) or has ability to hide itself (like chameleon), then we talk about the Holling type IV functional response or the so-called Monod-Haldane function. This function takes the form mX/(γ + b1X +X ), where X is the prey density, m > 0 is the complete saturation, whereas γ and b1 denote, respectively, the half-saturation constant and b1 the prey environmental carrying capacity. A space independent system of LotkaVolterra kind using type IV was under consideration in [45] and the principal purpose of this item is to assess the impact of the harvesting on equilibria of both prey and 10 Online first predator populations. The quandaries used here are, as the authors cited, based on the dynamical theory combined with a technique of Hopf bifurcation. A numerical analysis is provided to compare the dependence of the dynamical behavior on the harvesting effort for the prey between Holl\",\"PeriodicalId\":45392,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Mathematica Bohemica\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.3000,\"publicationDate\":\"2022-08-04\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"1\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Mathematica Bohemica\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.21136/mb.2022.0088-21\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q4\",\"JCRName\":\"MATHEMATICS\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Mathematica Bohemica","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.21136/mb.2022.0088-21","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"MATHEMATICS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

摘要

ly,功能反应可以定义为个体的消费率(这里我们谈论的是捕食者的消费)和食物密度(即猎物密度)之间的关系。这相当于说,功能反应反映了捕食者对猎物的捕获能力,或者换句话说,引入功能反应是为了描述当猎物密度变化时捕食者对猎物消耗率的变化。从绘图的角度来看,每种类型的功能反应I、II或III都有一个特殊的特征。事实上,I型,或捕食者反应的线性情况,是指消耗的猎物数量(每单位时间)与猎物密度的关系图显示消耗的猎物数目与猎物密度之间存在线性关系的情况。Holling II型,也称为凹向上响应,是曲线的梯度随着猎物密度的增加而单调下降的情况,可能在猎物消耗的恒定值下饱和。作为参考,涉及这种功能反应的Lotka-Volterra模型被称为Rosenzweig-MacArthur模型。III型反应在种群动力学专家中被称为S型反应,在低食物密度下具有向下凹陷的部分。事实上,对于霍林III,当曲线的梯度随着猎物密度的增加而先增加后减少时,就会出现S形行为。这种行为是由于捕食者群体中的“学习行为”造成的。现在,我们讨论了对前三种霍林型功能反应的一些“生态学”解释。I型反应是简单假设的结果,即给定捕食者(通常是被动捕食者)在固定空间区域内的固定时间间隔[0,Tt]内遇到猎物的概率线性取决于猎物密度。这可以用Y=aTsX的形式表示,其中Y是一个捕食者消耗的猎物数量,X是猎物密度,Ts是可用于搜索的时间,a是比例常数,称为发现率(在我们的情况下由参数b表示)。在不需要花费时间处理猎物的情况下,所有时间都可以用于搜索,即Ts=Tt,我们有i型反应:假设捕食者(密度为P)独立行动,在时间Tt内,猎物的总量将减少aTtXP。此外,如果每个捕食者对消耗的每个猎物都需要一个处理时间h,那么可用于搜索的时间Ts就会减少:Ts=Tt−hY。考虑到Y在I型响应中的表达,这导致Y=aTtX−ahXY,这意味着Y=atX/(1+ahX),这正是II型响应。因此,在区间[0,Tt]中,猎物的总量减少aTtXP/(1+ahX)。让我们指出,术语“ah”是无量纲的,可以解释为一般捕食者杀死和吃掉一般猎物的能力,它具有以下特征次数:如果处理时间h比典型的发现时间1/a长得多,则“ah”很大,而“ah”在相反的极限下很小;在这种情况下,8在线首先,II型反应被简化为I型。Holling III型功能性反应可以被视为II型的概括,其形式为aTtX/(1+ahX),k>1。在文献中,这种反应是通过假设捕食者群体中发生学习行为来刺激的,随着与猎物的更多接触,发现率随之增加(更多细节请参见[25])。为了从多个方面考察Lotka-Volterra模型的翼展,我们提供了一些涉及关键问题的工作的非排他性列表,这些工作被广泛讨论。我们从函数响应为Holling I的系统开始。需要特别注意的一个重要问题是稳态的研究,更准确地说,在[40]中,考虑了具有非线性扩散效应的捕食系统。这种非线性扩散效应对生物物种及其资源生物量(即其环境容量)有影响。在此,研究人员假设分散力和扩散取决于来自其他物种的种群压力。具有Holling I型函数型响应的Lotka-Volterra系统的平衡问题在[54]中也进行了广泛的理论和数值研究,特别是内部系统,以及它们的动力学行为,如循环折叠、鞍折叠、同宿鞍连接。我在这里介绍的Holling来自所谓的Beddington DeAngelis功能反应的范围。 正如作者所引用的,这里使用的难题是基于动力学理论和Hopf分岔技术。提供了一个数值分析来比较Holl和Holl之间的动力学行为对猎物捕获努力的依赖性
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Null controllability of a coupled model in population dynamics
ly, a functional response can be defined as the relationship between an individual’s rate of consumption (here we talk about a consumption of predator) and food’s density (i.e., prey’s density). This amounts to saying that a functional response reflects the capture ability of the predator to prey or in other words, the functional response is introduced to describe the change in the rate of consumption of prey by predator when the density of prey varies. In the plotting point of view, each type of functional response I, II or III has a special characteristic. In fact, type I, or the linear case of the predator response, is the situation when the plot of the number of prey consumed (per unit of time) as a function of prey density shows a linear relationship between the number of prey consumed and the prey density. The Holling type II, called also concave upward response, is the case when the gradient of the curve decreases monotonically with increasing prey density, probably saturating at a constant value of prey consumption. For information, the Lotka-Volterra model involving this functional response is known as the Rosenzweig-MacArthur model. The type III response is known between the specialists of population dynamics as the sigmoid response having a concave downward part at low food density. Actually, for the Holling III, a sigmoidal behavior occurs when the gradient of the curve first increases and then decreases with increasing prey density. This behavior is due to the “learning behavior” in the predator population. Now, we address some “ecological” interpretations of the three first Holling types functional responses. The type I response is the result of simple assumption that the probability of a given predator (usually the passive one) encountering prey in a fixed time interval [0, Tt] within a fixed spatial region depends linearly on the prey density. This can be expressed under the form Y = aTsX, where Y is the amount of prey consumed by one predator, X is the prey density, Ts is the time available for searching and a is a constant of proportionality, termed as the discovery rate (which is in our case represented by the parameter b). In the absence of need to spend time handling the prey, all the time can be used for searching, i.e., Ts = Tt, and we have the type I response: assuming that the predators (having the density P ) act independently, in time Tt the total amount of prey will be reduced by quantity aTtXP . In addition, if each predator requires a handling time h for each individual prey that is consumed, the time available for searching Ts is reduced: Ts = Tt−hY . Taking into account the expression of Y in response type I, this leads to Y = aTtX − ahXY and this implies Y = aTtX/(1 + ahX) and this is exactly the type II response. Therefore, in the interval [0, Tt] the total amount of prey is reduced by the quantity aTtXP/(1 + ahX). Let us point out that the term “ah” is dimensionless and can be interpreted as the ability of a generic predator to kill and consume a generic prey and it possesses the following characteristics times: “ah” is large if the handling time h is much longer than the typical discovery time 1/a and “ah” is small in the opposite limit; in this case 8 Online first the type II response is reduced to type I. The Holling type III functional response can be viewed as a generalization of type II and takes the form aTtX /(1 + ahX) with k > 1. In literature, this response is stimulated by supposing that learning behavior occurs in the predator population with a consequent increase in the discovery rate as more encounters with prey occurs (see [25] for more details). To see to wingspan of the Lotka-Volterra models from many sides of investigations, we provide a nonexhaustive list of some works dealing with crucial questions, which are discussed widely. We begin with the system whose functional response is Holling I. One of the important problems which takes a special attention, is the study of the steady states and more accurately, in [40] a prey-predator system with nonlinear diffusion effects is considered. Such nonlinear diffusion effects have an impact on a biological species as well as their resource-biomass (i.e., the capacity of their environment). Herein, the workers assume that the dispersive force and the diffusion depend on population pressure from other species. The question of equilibrium of Lotka-Volterra systems with Holling type I functional type response takes also a broad study theoretically and numerically in [54], specially the interior one, as well as their dynamical behavior such as the cyclic-fold, saddle-fold, homoclinic saddle connection. The Holling I introduced here is from the range of the so-called Beddington-DeAngelis functional response. Remaining in the type I, the authors in [44] tried to prove the existence of an asymptotically stable pest-eradication for a prey-predator system modeled by an ordinary differential equation, when the impulsive period is some critical value less by implementing Floquet theorem and a small amplitude perturbation method. Such a solution of eradication is somehow the mixing between a synthetic strategy (insecticides or pesticides for instance) and biological control, e.g. the natural enemies “killing” the dangerous pests (the prey here) without causing a serious damages to the two population densities (see also [59] for a similar study). Even the similarity appearance between their curves, functional responses of type I and type II have two considerable differences: the first one was pointed out before and it concerns the predator time handling of prey. Contrary to the Holling type II, the time handling is missed for predators in type I, which means that the consumers have a little difficulty capturing and assimilating prey but they switch their time to other activities once their ingestion rate is great enough to satisfy their energetic needs. The second difference is in the dynamical behavior. In fact, while the Holling type II displays the local Hopf bifurcation, the Holling type I makes clear a global cyclic-fold bifurcation. These differences between Holling I and Holling II, in particular the first, lead a numerous works to take into account the predator time handling in their different models. We emphasize here that Holling II possesses a generalization, which is exactly Beddington-DeAngelis functional response cited previously. This functional takes the form ΦBD(N,P ) = cN/(e+N + h1P ), where N Online first 9 and P should be the densities of prey and predator, while e stands for the halfsaturation constant, i.e., the amount of prey at which the per capita predation rate is half of its maximum c and h1 is a positive constant (see [54] for further details about this functional response). Among the works interested in Lotka-Volterra with Holling type II we cite for instance [48], [55]. In [55], a statistical study was presented to see if one can replace Holling type II by functional response from the type of Beddington-DeAngelis, Crowly-Martin or Hassel-Valey model for a divers cases related to the predator feeding rate. Peng et al. in [48] were concerned with the question of the steady-states of some reaction-diffusion models and they established the non-existence of a non-constant equilibrium solutions of two prey-predator systems with Holling II when the interaction between the two populations is strong as they claimed and where the constant measuring the ability of generic predator to kill and consume generic prey is equal to 1. By the way, a wide classical ecological literature assumed that mathematical models with Holling type II (or in general the non-sigmoid) functional response involving a diffusion terms match thoroughly in description of the pattern formation of a phytoplankton-zooplankton system. The affirmative answers are basically related to experiments realized in laboratories on zooplankton feeding, which are carried out in small-sized containers or bottles. But if one wants to investigate zooplankton grazing control in real ecosystems (may be the oceans), it will be more relevant to introduce the Holling type III response as stated in the introduction of [46]. Actually, the main focus of [46] was to set a generic model which explains the observed alteration of type between the different functional responses of plankton systems and gives, as he presumed, an evidence that for such a system the Holling type III is more adequate than other kinds. In the vocation of well-posedness, the global existence, uniqueness and the boundedness of a strong solution of partial differential equation with a special case of Holling III was brought out in [11]. This strong solution was approximated numerically using a spectral method and a Runge-Kutta time solver. The modelling using the Holling type III does not stop here, it can play also a crucial role to model the entomophagous species (see [16] for more details). But when a functional response describes the interaction between predator and prey when the prey exhibits group defense (like buffalo) or has ability to hide itself (like chameleon), then we talk about the Holling type IV functional response or the so-called Monod-Haldane function. This function takes the form mX/(γ + b1X +X ), where X is the prey density, m > 0 is the complete saturation, whereas γ and b1 denote, respectively, the half-saturation constant and b1 the prey environmental carrying capacity. A space independent system of LotkaVolterra kind using type IV was under consideration in [45] and the principal purpose of this item is to assess the impact of the harvesting on equilibria of both prey and 10 Online first predator populations. The quandaries used here are, as the authors cited, based on the dynamical theory combined with a technique of Hopf bifurcation. A numerical analysis is provided to compare the dependence of the dynamical behavior on the harvesting effort for the prey between Holl
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Mathematica Bohemica
Mathematica Bohemica MATHEMATICS-
CiteScore
1.10
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
审稿时长
52 weeks
期刊最新文献
Dynamic behavior of vector solutions of a class of 2-D neutral differential systems Global well-posedness and energy decay for a one dimensional porous-elastic system subject to a neutral delay On forbidden configuration of pseudomodular lattices Sakaguchi type functions defined by balancing polynomials Finite logarithmic order meromorphic solutions of linear difference/differential-difference equations
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1