对亵渎的感知:种族、性别和咒骂选择如何影响感知的冒犯性

Q3 Social Sciences North American Journal of Psychology Pub Date : 2014-06-01 DOI:10.1037/e603132013-001
Lora L. Jacobi
{"title":"对亵渎的感知:种族、性别和咒骂选择如何影响感知的冒犯性","authors":"Lora L. Jacobi","doi":"10.1037/e603132013-001","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Many factors affect what is considered profane language and the relative offensiveness of different profane words. Perceived offensiveness is affected by mere exposure to profanity (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004), the profanity spoken (Jay, 2009), the gender (Selnow, 1985) and ethnicity of the speaker and perceiver (Popp, Donovan, Crawford, Marsh, & Peele, 2003), as well as the context in which profanities are spoken (Johnson & Lewis, 2010). Additionally, the use of profanity has long been found to affect individuals' perceptions of the speaker and impression formation (Cohen & Saine, 1977; Mulac, 1976). The current research examined gender and ethnicity of the speaker as well as the choice of expletive with regard to perceived offensiveness of the profanity spoken. Profane Language There has been a general increase in the use of profanities spoken in both public and private settings. Words that would have been censored by the media twenty-years ago are now commonplace. Within the broadcasting media, 9 out of 10 programs contained at least one obscenity, and most television programs have an incidence of risque language approximately once every five minutes (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004, 2009). As the media have changed their standards of acceptance of profane language, the public's exposure to words that were traditionally considered taboo has increased. There are potential negative consequences of increased public exposure to profanities through media sources. Mere exposure to excessive cursing can desensitize listeners to the use of profane words (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Additionally, based on the principles of behavior modeling, increased exposure to profanities increases their use in everyday language, particularly when reinforced (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Whether the use of obscenities is reinforced is dependent upon the purpose and context of use. Individuals curse for a variety of reasons; reasons include serving to release negative emotions in the speaker, to shock or insult the receiver, and out of sheer habit (Rassin & Van der Heiden, 2005). An estimated two-thirds of all incidents of profanity usage are for the purpose of expressing anger and frustration; in such instances, a decrease in the speaker's perceived anger or frustration would negatively reinforce the use of profanity under similar circumstances in the future (Jay, 2000). The taboo words individuals speak are typically contingent upon the situational context, thereby affecting the formality/informality of speech (Jay 2009; Johnson & Lewis, 2010). As context varies (e.g., a formal dinner versus a casual conversation between friends), so does the perceived offensiveness of coarse language. Although the frequency of hearing obscenities has increased, the top 10 most frequently used taboo words have remained consistent over time and contains words such as fuck, shit, and variants of the word ass (e.g., a-hole; Jay, 2009). Various researchers have grouped taboo words by type, such as the \"seven dirty words,\" sexual words, excretory words, body parts, religious blasphemy, references to animals, social deviations, and ethnic/ethnic slurs (e.g., Foote & Woodward, 1973; Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). The seven dirty words semantically focus on aspects of the physical body and includes the words fuck and shit (Beck, 2009). Although the seven dirty words have historically been banned from broadcast, these specific curse words are heard approximately once every three hours in prime-time television, with the word fuck being heard most often (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Excretory words/body parts, such as ass and butt are also used with great frequency in the media, and are second to only the most mild of taboo words (e.g., hell, damn; Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Other taboo words that are considered to be even more offensive (e.g., nigger, cunt, cocksucker) are used less frequently in public and have remained highly offensive (Jay, 2009). …","PeriodicalId":35502,"journal":{"name":"North American Journal of Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2014-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"12","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Perceptions of Profanity: How Race, Gender, and Expletive Choice Affect Perceived Offensiveness\",\"authors\":\"Lora L. Jacobi\",\"doi\":\"10.1037/e603132013-001\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Many factors affect what is considered profane language and the relative offensiveness of different profane words. Perceived offensiveness is affected by mere exposure to profanity (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004), the profanity spoken (Jay, 2009), the gender (Selnow, 1985) and ethnicity of the speaker and perceiver (Popp, Donovan, Crawford, Marsh, & Peele, 2003), as well as the context in which profanities are spoken (Johnson & Lewis, 2010). Additionally, the use of profanity has long been found to affect individuals' perceptions of the speaker and impression formation (Cohen & Saine, 1977; Mulac, 1976). The current research examined gender and ethnicity of the speaker as well as the choice of expletive with regard to perceived offensiveness of the profanity spoken. Profane Language There has been a general increase in the use of profanities spoken in both public and private settings. Words that would have been censored by the media twenty-years ago are now commonplace. Within the broadcasting media, 9 out of 10 programs contained at least one obscenity, and most television programs have an incidence of risque language approximately once every five minutes (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004, 2009). As the media have changed their standards of acceptance of profane language, the public's exposure to words that were traditionally considered taboo has increased. There are potential negative consequences of increased public exposure to profanities through media sources. Mere exposure to excessive cursing can desensitize listeners to the use of profane words (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Additionally, based on the principles of behavior modeling, increased exposure to profanities increases their use in everyday language, particularly when reinforced (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Whether the use of obscenities is reinforced is dependent upon the purpose and context of use. Individuals curse for a variety of reasons; reasons include serving to release negative emotions in the speaker, to shock or insult the receiver, and out of sheer habit (Rassin & Van der Heiden, 2005). An estimated two-thirds of all incidents of profanity usage are for the purpose of expressing anger and frustration; in such instances, a decrease in the speaker's perceived anger or frustration would negatively reinforce the use of profanity under similar circumstances in the future (Jay, 2000). The taboo words individuals speak are typically contingent upon the situational context, thereby affecting the formality/informality of speech (Jay 2009; Johnson & Lewis, 2010). As context varies (e.g., a formal dinner versus a casual conversation between friends), so does the perceived offensiveness of coarse language. Although the frequency of hearing obscenities has increased, the top 10 most frequently used taboo words have remained consistent over time and contains words such as fuck, shit, and variants of the word ass (e.g., a-hole; Jay, 2009). Various researchers have grouped taboo words by type, such as the \\\"seven dirty words,\\\" sexual words, excretory words, body parts, religious blasphemy, references to animals, social deviations, and ethnic/ethnic slurs (e.g., Foote & Woodward, 1973; Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). The seven dirty words semantically focus on aspects of the physical body and includes the words fuck and shit (Beck, 2009). Although the seven dirty words have historically been banned from broadcast, these specific curse words are heard approximately once every three hours in prime-time television, with the word fuck being heard most often (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Excretory words/body parts, such as ass and butt are also used with great frequency in the media, and are second to only the most mild of taboo words (e.g., hell, damn; Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Other taboo words that are considered to be even more offensive (e.g., nigger, cunt, cocksucker) are used less frequently in public and have remained highly offensive (Jay, 2009). …\",\"PeriodicalId\":35502,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"North American Journal of Psychology\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2014-06-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"12\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"North American Journal of Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1037/e603132013-001\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"Social Sciences\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"North American Journal of Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/e603132013-001","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"Social Sciences","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12

摘要

许多因素影响着什么是亵渎语言以及不同亵渎词的相对冒犯性。感知到的冒犯性受到以下因素的影响:仅仅是接触到亵渎(Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004)、所说的亵渎(Jay, 2009)、性别(Selnow, 1985)、说话者和感知者的种族(Popp, Donovan, Crawford, Marsh, & Peele, 2003),以及说亵渎的语境(Johnson & Lewis, 2010)。此外,人们早就发现,使用脏话会影响个人对说话者的看法和印象的形成(Cohen & Saine, 1977;Mulac, 1976)。目前的研究调查了说话者的性别和种族,以及对脏话的选择,以及对脏话的冒犯感。在公共场合和私人场合,亵渎语言的使用普遍增加。20年前会被媒体审查的词语现在已经司空见惯了。在广播媒体中,每10个节目中就有9个包含至少一个淫秽内容,大多数电视节目大约每5分钟就会出现一次淫秽语言(Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004, 2009)。随着媒体改变了接受亵渎语言的标准,公众接触到传统上被视为禁忌的词汇的机会也增加了。公众通过媒体渠道接触到更多的亵渎行为,可能会产生负面后果。仅仅暴露在过度的咒骂中就会使听者对亵渎性词汇的使用失去敏感性(Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004)。此外,基于行为建模的原则,增加对脏话的接触会增加它们在日常语言中的使用,特别是在强化时(Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004)。淫秽的使用是否得到加强取决于使用的目的和背景。人们咒骂的原因有很多;原因包括释放说话者的负面情绪,震惊或侮辱接收者,以及纯粹的习惯(Rassin & Van der Heiden, 2005)。据估计,三分之二的脏话使用事件是为了表达愤怒和沮丧;在这种情况下,说话者感知到的愤怒或沮丧的减少会在未来类似的情况下消极地加强脏话的使用(Jay, 2000)。个人所说的禁忌语通常取决于情景语境,从而影响讲话的正式/非正式性(Jay 2009;Johnson & Lewis, 2010)。随着语境的变化(例如,正式的晚餐与朋友之间的随意交谈),粗话的冒犯程度也会有所不同。尽管听到脏话的频率有所增加,但10个最常用的禁忌词一直保持一致,其中包括诸如fuck, shit和ass的变体(例如,a-hole;杰,2009)。许多研究人员将禁忌语按类型分类,如“七个脏词”、性词汇、排泄词、身体部位、宗教亵渎、提及动物、社会偏差和种族/种族侮辱(例如,Foote & Woodward, 1973;Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004)。这七个脏词在语义上侧重于身体的各个方面,包括单词fuck和shit (Beck, 2009)。虽然这七个脏话在历史上是禁止播出的,但这些特定的脏话在黄金时段的电视节目中大约每三个小时就会听到一次,其中最常听到的是fuck这个词(Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004)。排泄词/身体部位,如ass和butt在媒体中使用频率也很高,仅次于最温和的禁忌词(如hell, damn;Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004)。其他被认为更具攻击性的禁忌词(例如,nigger, cunt, cocksucker)在公共场合使用的频率较低,并且仍然具有高度的攻击性(Jay, 2009)。...
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Perceptions of Profanity: How Race, Gender, and Expletive Choice Affect Perceived Offensiveness
Many factors affect what is considered profane language and the relative offensiveness of different profane words. Perceived offensiveness is affected by mere exposure to profanity (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004), the profanity spoken (Jay, 2009), the gender (Selnow, 1985) and ethnicity of the speaker and perceiver (Popp, Donovan, Crawford, Marsh, & Peele, 2003), as well as the context in which profanities are spoken (Johnson & Lewis, 2010). Additionally, the use of profanity has long been found to affect individuals' perceptions of the speaker and impression formation (Cohen & Saine, 1977; Mulac, 1976). The current research examined gender and ethnicity of the speaker as well as the choice of expletive with regard to perceived offensiveness of the profanity spoken. Profane Language There has been a general increase in the use of profanities spoken in both public and private settings. Words that would have been censored by the media twenty-years ago are now commonplace. Within the broadcasting media, 9 out of 10 programs contained at least one obscenity, and most television programs have an incidence of risque language approximately once every five minutes (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004, 2009). As the media have changed their standards of acceptance of profane language, the public's exposure to words that were traditionally considered taboo has increased. There are potential negative consequences of increased public exposure to profanities through media sources. Mere exposure to excessive cursing can desensitize listeners to the use of profane words (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Additionally, based on the principles of behavior modeling, increased exposure to profanities increases their use in everyday language, particularly when reinforced (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Whether the use of obscenities is reinforced is dependent upon the purpose and context of use. Individuals curse for a variety of reasons; reasons include serving to release negative emotions in the speaker, to shock or insult the receiver, and out of sheer habit (Rassin & Van der Heiden, 2005). An estimated two-thirds of all incidents of profanity usage are for the purpose of expressing anger and frustration; in such instances, a decrease in the speaker's perceived anger or frustration would negatively reinforce the use of profanity under similar circumstances in the future (Jay, 2000). The taboo words individuals speak are typically contingent upon the situational context, thereby affecting the formality/informality of speech (Jay 2009; Johnson & Lewis, 2010). As context varies (e.g., a formal dinner versus a casual conversation between friends), so does the perceived offensiveness of coarse language. Although the frequency of hearing obscenities has increased, the top 10 most frequently used taboo words have remained consistent over time and contains words such as fuck, shit, and variants of the word ass (e.g., a-hole; Jay, 2009). Various researchers have grouped taboo words by type, such as the "seven dirty words," sexual words, excretory words, body parts, religious blasphemy, references to animals, social deviations, and ethnic/ethnic slurs (e.g., Foote & Woodward, 1973; Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). The seven dirty words semantically focus on aspects of the physical body and includes the words fuck and shit (Beck, 2009). Although the seven dirty words have historically been banned from broadcast, these specific curse words are heard approximately once every three hours in prime-time television, with the word fuck being heard most often (Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Excretory words/body parts, such as ass and butt are also used with great frequency in the media, and are second to only the most mild of taboo words (e.g., hell, damn; Kaye & Sapolsky, 2004). Other taboo words that are considered to be even more offensive (e.g., nigger, cunt, cocksucker) are used less frequently in public and have remained highly offensive (Jay, 2009). …
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
North American Journal of Psychology
North American Journal of Psychology Social Sciences-Sociology and Political Science
CiteScore
0.70
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
Stressors and Resources of Oregon Psychologists: How Are Helpers Being Helped? Perceptions of Profanity: How Race, Gender, and Expletive Choice Affect Perceived Offensiveness Much or More? Experiments of Rationality and Spite with School Children The Social Projection of Belongingness Needs Contrasting Case Definitions: The ME International Consensus Criteria vs. the Fukuda et al. CFS Criteria.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1