Peer Review of "Reflection on 'Trial and Error (-Related Negativity)'

Stefan Gaillard, S. Devine
{"title":"Peer Review of \"Reflection on 'Trial and Error (-Related Negativity)'","authors":"Stefan Gaillard, S. Devine","doi":"10.36850/r2.pr1","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"This paper is about error on three levels. First of all, it deals with research into the ERN, error-related negativity. The ERN is a negative deflection in the EEG signal, which tends to occur within 100 milliseconds of making an error. The authors hypothesize that physical pain can be considered as a bodily signal that a type of error has been committed: there is a \"discrepancy between the actual and optimal/targeted state\", as the authors put it (p.1). This raises the question whether the ERN is also associated with pain and the avoidance of pain, and if so how. More specifically the authors want to know whether people with an elevated ERN are more prone to avoidance behaviour, which in turn can lead to chronic pain. I am an historian of psychology with philosophical interests and have no expertise in clinical neuropsychology, so I will not comment on this hypothesis. But the paper also deals with error in two other ways, which I do feel able to reflect on. The authors describe their attempts to develop an experimental paradigm for the study of the role of the ERN in pain avoidance. In these attempts they make errors which they then try to correct in a further attempt, six task versions in total. This is the second way this paper deals with errors -those of the experimenters themselves. But there is a third level too: it is crucial for the experimental task to induce the participant to make the right number of errors -not too many, not too few. The second and third aspects are obviously related: the errors of the experimenters concern, among others, the number of errors the participants make. The authors describe their challenge as an interdisciplinary one: they had to combine elements of neurophysiology (ERN) with clinical psychology (pain avoidance). Specifically: they had to somehow induce an ERN in the participants, and elicit and measure some type of avoidance behaviour at the same time. Moreover, to determine what each participant’s average ERN is, they needed at least six ERN measures per participant, and thus a minimum of six errors. The errors, finally, had to be \"inhibition errors\", not errors due to lack of knowledge or skill. It wasn’t clear to me why this was important, or what an inhibition error is in the first place, but this is no doubt due to my own lack of knowledge in this field. All in all, the specifications of the task were narrow and demanding: not any type of error would do (only inhibition errors); the errors had to produce a proper ERN; a minimum of six was needed; the participants had to be aware of their error (otherwise they would not show avoidance behaviour); and of course there had to be pain associated with the errors, but not so much pain that the ethics committee would reject the pilot study, or the subjects would refuse to participate. What followed was a kind of dance, or rather a series of dances, with the experimenters leading the participants, successively trying different choreographies in an attempt to get their partners to make the right moves and not step on their toes. In a recent article, Brenninkmeijer, Rietzschel, and I reported on a series of interviews we had conducted with researchers in psychology (Brenninkmeijer et al., 2019). We had asked them about their informal research practices, that is to say, those practices that are not made explicit in the method section of a paper but are nevertheless considered important. We saw two themes in their answers. The first is a strong concern with professionalism, expressing itself a.o.among others in an orderly lab and a smoothly running experiment, and respectful conduct towards the participants. The second theme is a focus on producing good data by managing the performance of the participant. This second theme is relevant with regard to this paper on ’Trial and Error’. Much of the work that is done in the lab, including the kind of informal, unwritten work that we explored in our interviews, is geared towards eliciting the right kind of behaviour from the participant. What happens in the lab has a theatrical quality: a particular performance is expected from the participants, and they are guided to it by the staging and scripting (or choreography) of the experiment and the conduct of the experimenter. Our interviewees mentioned","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"36 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2020-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Trial and Error","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.36850/r2.pr1","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

This paper is about error on three levels. First of all, it deals with research into the ERN, error-related negativity. The ERN is a negative deflection in the EEG signal, which tends to occur within 100 milliseconds of making an error. The authors hypothesize that physical pain can be considered as a bodily signal that a type of error has been committed: there is a "discrepancy between the actual and optimal/targeted state", as the authors put it (p.1). This raises the question whether the ERN is also associated with pain and the avoidance of pain, and if so how. More specifically the authors want to know whether people with an elevated ERN are more prone to avoidance behaviour, which in turn can lead to chronic pain. I am an historian of psychology with philosophical interests and have no expertise in clinical neuropsychology, so I will not comment on this hypothesis. But the paper also deals with error in two other ways, which I do feel able to reflect on. The authors describe their attempts to develop an experimental paradigm for the study of the role of the ERN in pain avoidance. In these attempts they make errors which they then try to correct in a further attempt, six task versions in total. This is the second way this paper deals with errors -those of the experimenters themselves. But there is a third level too: it is crucial for the experimental task to induce the participant to make the right number of errors -not too many, not too few. The second and third aspects are obviously related: the errors of the experimenters concern, among others, the number of errors the participants make. The authors describe their challenge as an interdisciplinary one: they had to combine elements of neurophysiology (ERN) with clinical psychology (pain avoidance). Specifically: they had to somehow induce an ERN in the participants, and elicit and measure some type of avoidance behaviour at the same time. Moreover, to determine what each participant’s average ERN is, they needed at least six ERN measures per participant, and thus a minimum of six errors. The errors, finally, had to be "inhibition errors", not errors due to lack of knowledge or skill. It wasn’t clear to me why this was important, or what an inhibition error is in the first place, but this is no doubt due to my own lack of knowledge in this field. All in all, the specifications of the task were narrow and demanding: not any type of error would do (only inhibition errors); the errors had to produce a proper ERN; a minimum of six was needed; the participants had to be aware of their error (otherwise they would not show avoidance behaviour); and of course there had to be pain associated with the errors, but not so much pain that the ethics committee would reject the pilot study, or the subjects would refuse to participate. What followed was a kind of dance, or rather a series of dances, with the experimenters leading the participants, successively trying different choreographies in an attempt to get their partners to make the right moves and not step on their toes. In a recent article, Brenninkmeijer, Rietzschel, and I reported on a series of interviews we had conducted with researchers in psychology (Brenninkmeijer et al., 2019). We had asked them about their informal research practices, that is to say, those practices that are not made explicit in the method section of a paper but are nevertheless considered important. We saw two themes in their answers. The first is a strong concern with professionalism, expressing itself a.o.among others in an orderly lab and a smoothly running experiment, and respectful conduct towards the participants. The second theme is a focus on producing good data by managing the performance of the participant. This second theme is relevant with regard to this paper on ’Trial and Error’. Much of the work that is done in the lab, including the kind of informal, unwritten work that we explored in our interviews, is geared towards eliciting the right kind of behaviour from the participant. What happens in the lab has a theatrical quality: a particular performance is expected from the participants, and they are guided to it by the staging and scripting (or choreography) of the experiment and the conduct of the experimenter. Our interviewees mentioned
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
同行评议“对‘试错(相关消极)’的反思”
本文从三个层面探讨误差。首先,它涉及到对ERN的研究,错误相关的消极性。ERN是脑电图信号中的负偏转,往往发生在犯错后100毫秒内。作者假设,身体上的疼痛可以被认为是一种身体信号,表明犯了某种错误:正如作者所说,“实际状态与最佳/目标状态之间存在差异”(第1页)。这就提出了一个问题,ERN是否也与疼痛和回避疼痛有关,如果是的话,又是如何关联的。更具体地说,作者想知道,ERN水平升高的人是否更容易出现回避行为,从而导致慢性疼痛。我是一个有哲学兴趣的心理学历史学家,对临床神经心理学没有专业知识,所以我不会对这个假设发表评论。但这篇论文也从另外两个方面处理了错误,我确实觉得这是可以反思的。作者描述了他们试图开发一个实验范式来研究ERN在疼痛回避中的作用。在这些尝试中,他们犯了错误,然后他们试图在进一步的尝试中纠正,总共六个任务版本。这是本文处理错误的第二种方式——实验者自己的错误。但还有第三个层面:对于实验任务来说,诱导参与者犯正确数量的错误——不要太多,也不要太少——是至关重要的。第二个和第三个方面明显相关:实验人员的错误与参与者犯错误的次数有关。作者将他们的挑战描述为跨学科的挑战:他们必须将神经生理学(ERN)与临床心理学(疼痛回避)相结合。具体来说:他们必须以某种方式在参与者中诱导ERN,同时引发并测量某种类型的回避行为。此外,为了确定每个参与者的平均ERN是多少,他们至少需要每个参与者测量6个ERN,因此至少需要6个错误。最后,这些错误必须是“抑制错误”,而不是由于缺乏知识或技能而导致的错误。我不清楚为什么这很重要,或者什么是抑制错误,但毫无疑问,这是由于我自己在这个领域缺乏知识。总而言之,任务的规格是狭窄而苛刻的:任何类型的错误都不行(只有抑制错误);误差必须产生合适的ERN;至少需要六个;参与者必须意识到他们的错误(否则他们不会表现出回避行为);当然,这些错误会带来痛苦,但不会痛苦到伦理委员会会拒绝试点研究,或者受试者会拒绝参与。接下来是一种舞蹈,或者更确切地说是一系列舞蹈,由实验者带领参与者,依次尝试不同的舞蹈编排,试图让他们的搭档做出正确的动作,而不是踩到他们的脚趾。在最近的一篇文章中,Brenninkmeijer、Rietzschel和我报道了我们对心理学研究人员进行的一系列采访(Brenninkmeijer et al., 2019)。我们询问了他们的非正式研究实践,也就是说,那些在论文的方法部分没有明确说明但仍然被认为是重要的实践。我们在他们的回答中看到了两个主题。首先是对专业精神的强烈关注,在有序的实验室和顺利进行的实验中表现出自己的优秀,并尊重参与者的行为。第二个主题是关注通过管理参与者的表现来产生良好的数据。第二个主题与这篇关于“试错”的论文有关。在实验室里完成的大部分工作,包括我们在采访中探索的那种非正式的、不成文的工作,都是为了从参与者那里引出正确的行为。在实验室里发生的事情有一种戏剧的性质:一种特定的表演是被期望从参与者那里得到的,他们被实验的舞台和脚本(或编排)以及实验者的行为所引导。我们的受访者提到
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
Serendipity in Scientific Research Three Persistent Myths about Open Science The Music Must Play On: The Music Therapy Sessions that Should not Have Stopped Medical Expert Endorsement Fails to Reduce Vaccine Hesitancy in U.K. Residents A Manifesto for Rewarding and Recognizing Team Infrastructure Roles
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1