Does a niche construction strategy adaptation really exist in brown-headed cowbirds?

IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Ethology Pub Date : 2023-10-03 DOI:10.1111/eth.13412
Manuel Soler
{"title":"Does a niche construction strategy adaptation really exist in brown-headed cowbirds?","authors":"Manuel Soler","doi":"10.1111/eth.13412","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The subject of brood parasitism has become the focus of widespread attention for the numerous finely tuned adaptations that have been discovered in coevolving specialist brood parasite–host systems (Davies, <span>2000</span>; Krüger, <span>2007</span>; Soler, <span>2017a</span>). A recent study by Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) claims to have found one of these fascinating adaptations, posing the exciting conclusion that brown-headed cowbird (<i>Molothrus ater</i>) chicks use a niche construction strategy (alteration of its own environment for its own fitness benefit) in prothonotary warbler (<i>Protonotaria citrea</i>) host nests. In this experimental study, the authors have found that the selective brood reduction strategy driven by cowbird nestlings is reducing, but not eliminating host broods. According to their interpretation, this appears to represent an adaptive niche construction strategy given the brood reduction results and that survival of cowbird nestlings is higher in broods of two warbler nestlings than when alone or in broods of four warbler nestlings. This is an intriguing possibility but does a niche construction strategy allowing the survival of two nestmates really exist in brown-headed cowbirds?</p><p>Niche construction is a mechanism whereby individuals actively manipulate their environment to obtain more appropriate conditions in which their possibilities of survival and reproductive success increase (Aaby &amp; Ramsey, <span>2022</span>; Odling-Smee et al., <span>2013</span>; Trappes et al., <span>2022</span>). In birds, nest building is a clear example of niche construction (Trappes et al., <span>2022</span>). The strategy of the common cuckoo (<i>Cuculus canorus</i>) nestling, which soon after hatching evicts all host nestmates from the nest, allowing it to monopolize the feeding effort of its foster parents, can be considered another clear case of niche construction. Other brood parasites (non-evictors) share the nest with host nestlings, but usually, the parasitic nestling(s) outcompetes host nestlings. This also implies an active manipulation of their environment (the nest), and therefore, this strategy could also be considered niche construction. However, Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) suggest that in the brown-headed cowbird—protonotary warbler system, the brood parasite selectively manipulates brood reduction favouring the presence of two warbler nestlings in the nest. This suggestion is based on comparisons of data on nestling warbler mortality in four experimental groups, with two parasitized and two unparasitized treatments. However, the crucial prediction of the niche construction hypothesis—that is, that the cowbird nestling causes selective host brood reduction, allowing the survival of just two host nestlings—was not demonstrated.</p><p>In addition, this study selectively cites publications and raises several other key questions. Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) base their study on the ‘begging assistance hypothesis’ (Kilner et al., <span>2004</span>), which states that brown-headed cowbird nestlings sharing the nest with host nestlings grow faster than cowbird nestlings raised alone because parasitic nestlings take advantage of the begging calls produced by host nestlings to procure resources. That is, begging displays of host nestlings compel their parents to increase the rate of food delivery, and this extra food would be consumed by the cowbird nestling due to its higher competitive ability (Kilner et al., <span>2004</span>). In support of the hypothesis that parasitic nestlings need the assistance of host nestlings to secure a sufficient quantity of food, the authors cite all publications that have provided any kind of support for it, both before (Lichtenstein &amp; Sealy, <span>1998</span>) and after the hypothesis was proposed (Gloag et al., <span>2012</span>; Hoover &amp; Reetz, <span>2006</span>; Kilner et al., <span>2004</span>; Li &amp; Hauber, <span>2021</span>). One of these publications, Li and Hauber (<span>2021</span>), clearly stated that this hypothesis works only with midsized host species. Further, Gloag et al. (<span>2012</span>) reported that the begging assistance hypothesis was only supported in a small host species, but not in a larger one. Unfortunately, Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) did not mention any of the experimental studies that did not support the predictions of the begging assistance hypothesis in an evictor brood parasite (Grim et al., <span>2009</span>; Hauber &amp; Moskát, <span>2008</span>; Martín-Gálvez et al., <span>2005</span>). Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) also did not seem to be aware of other studies (e.g. Bolopo et al., <span>2015</span>; Rivers et al., <span>2010</span>; Soler &amp; de Neve, <span>2013</span>) and a review (Soler, <span>2017b</span>) concluding that in several nest-sharing brood parasite species larger nestlings successfully compete for extra food, regardless of whether they are hosts or parasites. In their paper, Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) did not discuss how this crucial factor (i.e. the size of the parasitic nestling relative to the size of its host nestmates) affects which nestling gets the extra food because of the intense begging emanating from nests shared by the brood parasite and host nestlings. On the contrary, in several places of the manuscript, the authors generalized the begging assistance hypothesis to all nest-sharing brood parasites. They even cited a study by Gloag et al. (<span>2012</span>) that tested this hypothesis in two species of different sizes that are used as hosts by the shiny cowbird (<i>Molothrus bonariensis</i>). In the small-sized host (the house wren, <i>Troglodytes aedon</i>), predictions of the begging assistance hypothesis were supported, whereas in the large-sized host (the chalk-browed mockingbird, <i>Mimus saturninus</i>) the parasite nestling registered higher food intake, mass growth and survival when reared alone than when sharing the nest with host nestlings, contradicting the begging assistance hypothesis. Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) did not mention these results with the large host, nor did they discuss the conclusion of Gloag et al. (<span>2012</span>) stating that cowbird nestlings are negatively affected by the presence of host nestlings in nests of mockingbirds. In short, this bias in citing studies weakens the conclusions that can be drawn from this study (Bouter et al., <span>2016</span>; Gøtzsche, <span>2022</span>).</p><p>In addition, Antonson et al.'s (<span>2022</span>) study raises several other major questions. First, the niche construction mechanism is presented by the authors as a finely timed and balanced adaptation. The vital question here is how such a complex and finely tuned adaptation could have evolved in the brown-headed cowbird, given that it is an extremely generalist brood parasite belonging to a recent clade (Sorenson &amp; Payne, <span>2002</span>). While interactions in other brood parasite–host systems are highly coevolved, with brood parasites having specific adaptations to brood parasitism, parasitic cowbirds lack most of those specialized adaptive features (Mermoz &amp; Ornelas, <span>2004</span>). In addition, finely tuned adaptations to brood parasitism are expected to evolve mainly in specialist brood parasite species in which selection pressures are stronger and adaptations and counteradaptations are more likely to evolve (Davies, <span>2000</span>). This raises the question of how a finely tuned adaptation such as the suggested niche construction strategy in the cowbird—protonotary warbler system, which implies selective brood reduction favouring the presence in the nest of just two warbler nestlings, could have evolved in the brown-headed cowbird, a brood parasite that frequently parasitizes seven or more species at a single site with the same female usually parasitizing multiple host species (Krüger, <span>2007</span>). This fundamental question is not discussed by the authors.</p><p>Second, how could cowbirds cause brood reduction that leads to an optimal niche? Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) address this question by speculation using only their own results. However, how can it be assumed that cowbird nestlings are able to force the starvation and/or survival of the appropriate number of host nestlings when several studies (e.g. Ryser et al., <span>2016</span>; Smith et al., <span>2017</span>) have highlighted that nestlings have little or no control over food allocation by parents, including in a brood parasite—host system (Soler et al., <span>2017</span>)? Likely, the death of protonotary warbler nestlings is the consequence of starvation because they are outcompeted by the cowbird nestling, which is larger in size (adult cowbirds 35–45 g, adult warblers 14–16 g; Antonson et al., <span>2022</span>), thereby providing it with a clear advantage, given that parents in most species selectively feed larger nestlings, even when smaller nestlings beg more intensively (Caro et al., <span>2016</span>; Soler et al., <span>2022</span>). To satisfactorily address this problem of the effect of difference in size between host and brood parasite nestlings, an additional experimental group would be needed, with five warbler nestlings in which one of the warblers is twice the size of its nest mates.</p><p>The selective host brood reduction of just two nestlings may be a consequence of the size of the prothonotary warbler, which is a mid-sized host species whose provisioning capacity could be just sufficient to raise one cowbird and two warbler nestlings.</p><p>Third, Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) report that the parasitism rate in their study area was 60%, but they do not discuss the rate of multiparasitism (two or more parasite eggs per nest). This information is indispensable to determine the strength of selection pressures favouring the evolution of an optimal niche strategy, given that brown-headed cowbirds are four times larger than protonotary warblers. According to Hoover (<span>2003</span>), who worked in the same prothonotary warbler population, more than half of the parasitized nests (51.1%; extracted from table 1 in Hoover [<span>2003</span>]) have two or more cowbird nestlings, signifying that the major competitors of a cowbird nestling are not small-sized host nestlings but other large-sized cowbird nestlings. According to this information, it is difficult to imagine the selection of an optimal niche strategy based on the existence of only host nestlings as nestmates of just one cowbird nestling. This relevant point was neither mentioned nor discussed by the authors.</p><p>In summary, before proposing the attractive possibility that a niche construction strategy could have evolved in brown-headed cowbird nestlings, the authors must firstly demonstrate that parasitic nestlings are responsible for selective host brood reduction to get the optimal brood size for the parasitic nestling of just two host nestlings in the nest. Second, citation bias should be avoided, reconciling or explaining the evidence of other studies that contradict the claim of niche selection; and thirdly, address the three important questions that have not been mentioned or discussed in their article. Until these problems are resolved, the reader can consider that the results reported by Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>), which they claimed to support the niche construction hypothesis, only support the begging assistance hypothesis and may be merely a consequence of the provisioning capacity of the mid-sized protonotary warbler hosts, which could be sufficient to raise one cowbird and two warbler nestlings.</p><p><b>Manuel Soler:</b> Conceptualization; writing – original draft; investigation; writing – review and editing; visualization; funding acquisition.</p><p>There is no conflict of interest to disclose.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3000,"publicationDate":"2023-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13412","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Ethology","FirstCategoryId":"99","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13412","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

The subject of brood parasitism has become the focus of widespread attention for the numerous finely tuned adaptations that have been discovered in coevolving specialist brood parasite–host systems (Davies, 2000; Krüger, 2007; Soler, 2017a). A recent study by Antonson et al. (2022) claims to have found one of these fascinating adaptations, posing the exciting conclusion that brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater) chicks use a niche construction strategy (alteration of its own environment for its own fitness benefit) in prothonotary warbler (Protonotaria citrea) host nests. In this experimental study, the authors have found that the selective brood reduction strategy driven by cowbird nestlings is reducing, but not eliminating host broods. According to their interpretation, this appears to represent an adaptive niche construction strategy given the brood reduction results and that survival of cowbird nestlings is higher in broods of two warbler nestlings than when alone or in broods of four warbler nestlings. This is an intriguing possibility but does a niche construction strategy allowing the survival of two nestmates really exist in brown-headed cowbirds?

Niche construction is a mechanism whereby individuals actively manipulate their environment to obtain more appropriate conditions in which their possibilities of survival and reproductive success increase (Aaby & Ramsey, 2022; Odling-Smee et al., 2013; Trappes et al., 2022). In birds, nest building is a clear example of niche construction (Trappes et al., 2022). The strategy of the common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) nestling, which soon after hatching evicts all host nestmates from the nest, allowing it to monopolize the feeding effort of its foster parents, can be considered another clear case of niche construction. Other brood parasites (non-evictors) share the nest with host nestlings, but usually, the parasitic nestling(s) outcompetes host nestlings. This also implies an active manipulation of their environment (the nest), and therefore, this strategy could also be considered niche construction. However, Antonson et al. (2022) suggest that in the brown-headed cowbird—protonotary warbler system, the brood parasite selectively manipulates brood reduction favouring the presence of two warbler nestlings in the nest. This suggestion is based on comparisons of data on nestling warbler mortality in four experimental groups, with two parasitized and two unparasitized treatments. However, the crucial prediction of the niche construction hypothesis—that is, that the cowbird nestling causes selective host brood reduction, allowing the survival of just two host nestlings—was not demonstrated.

In addition, this study selectively cites publications and raises several other key questions. Antonson et al. (2022) base their study on the ‘begging assistance hypothesis’ (Kilner et al., 2004), which states that brown-headed cowbird nestlings sharing the nest with host nestlings grow faster than cowbird nestlings raised alone because parasitic nestlings take advantage of the begging calls produced by host nestlings to procure resources. That is, begging displays of host nestlings compel their parents to increase the rate of food delivery, and this extra food would be consumed by the cowbird nestling due to its higher competitive ability (Kilner et al., 2004). In support of the hypothesis that parasitic nestlings need the assistance of host nestlings to secure a sufficient quantity of food, the authors cite all publications that have provided any kind of support for it, both before (Lichtenstein & Sealy, 1998) and after the hypothesis was proposed (Gloag et al., 2012; Hoover & Reetz, 2006; Kilner et al., 2004; Li & Hauber, 2021). One of these publications, Li and Hauber (2021), clearly stated that this hypothesis works only with midsized host species. Further, Gloag et al. (2012) reported that the begging assistance hypothesis was only supported in a small host species, but not in a larger one. Unfortunately, Antonson et al. (2022) did not mention any of the experimental studies that did not support the predictions of the begging assistance hypothesis in an evictor brood parasite (Grim et al., 2009; Hauber & Moskát, 2008; Martín-Gálvez et al., 2005). Antonson et al. (2022) also did not seem to be aware of other studies (e.g. Bolopo et al., 2015; Rivers et al., 2010; Soler & de Neve, 2013) and a review (Soler, 2017b) concluding that in several nest-sharing brood parasite species larger nestlings successfully compete for extra food, regardless of whether they are hosts or parasites. In their paper, Antonson et al. (2022) did not discuss how this crucial factor (i.e. the size of the parasitic nestling relative to the size of its host nestmates) affects which nestling gets the extra food because of the intense begging emanating from nests shared by the brood parasite and host nestlings. On the contrary, in several places of the manuscript, the authors generalized the begging assistance hypothesis to all nest-sharing brood parasites. They even cited a study by Gloag et al. (2012) that tested this hypothesis in two species of different sizes that are used as hosts by the shiny cowbird (Molothrus bonariensis). In the small-sized host (the house wren, Troglodytes aedon), predictions of the begging assistance hypothesis were supported, whereas in the large-sized host (the chalk-browed mockingbird, Mimus saturninus) the parasite nestling registered higher food intake, mass growth and survival when reared alone than when sharing the nest with host nestlings, contradicting the begging assistance hypothesis. Antonson et al. (2022) did not mention these results with the large host, nor did they discuss the conclusion of Gloag et al. (2012) stating that cowbird nestlings are negatively affected by the presence of host nestlings in nests of mockingbirds. In short, this bias in citing studies weakens the conclusions that can be drawn from this study (Bouter et al., 2016; Gøtzsche, 2022).

In addition, Antonson et al.'s (2022) study raises several other major questions. First, the niche construction mechanism is presented by the authors as a finely timed and balanced adaptation. The vital question here is how such a complex and finely tuned adaptation could have evolved in the brown-headed cowbird, given that it is an extremely generalist brood parasite belonging to a recent clade (Sorenson & Payne, 2002). While interactions in other brood parasite–host systems are highly coevolved, with brood parasites having specific adaptations to brood parasitism, parasitic cowbirds lack most of those specialized adaptive features (Mermoz & Ornelas, 2004). In addition, finely tuned adaptations to brood parasitism are expected to evolve mainly in specialist brood parasite species in which selection pressures are stronger and adaptations and counteradaptations are more likely to evolve (Davies, 2000). This raises the question of how a finely tuned adaptation such as the suggested niche construction strategy in the cowbird—protonotary warbler system, which implies selective brood reduction favouring the presence in the nest of just two warbler nestlings, could have evolved in the brown-headed cowbird, a brood parasite that frequently parasitizes seven or more species at a single site with the same female usually parasitizing multiple host species (Krüger, 2007). This fundamental question is not discussed by the authors.

Second, how could cowbirds cause brood reduction that leads to an optimal niche? Antonson et al. (2022) address this question by speculation using only their own results. However, how can it be assumed that cowbird nestlings are able to force the starvation and/or survival of the appropriate number of host nestlings when several studies (e.g. Ryser et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2017) have highlighted that nestlings have little or no control over food allocation by parents, including in a brood parasite—host system (Soler et al., 2017)? Likely, the death of protonotary warbler nestlings is the consequence of starvation because they are outcompeted by the cowbird nestling, which is larger in size (adult cowbirds 35–45 g, adult warblers 14–16 g; Antonson et al., 2022), thereby providing it with a clear advantage, given that parents in most species selectively feed larger nestlings, even when smaller nestlings beg more intensively (Caro et al., 2016; Soler et al., 2022). To satisfactorily address this problem of the effect of difference in size between host and brood parasite nestlings, an additional experimental group would be needed, with five warbler nestlings in which one of the warblers is twice the size of its nest mates.

The selective host brood reduction of just two nestlings may be a consequence of the size of the prothonotary warbler, which is a mid-sized host species whose provisioning capacity could be just sufficient to raise one cowbird and two warbler nestlings.

Third, Antonson et al. (2022) report that the parasitism rate in their study area was 60%, but they do not discuss the rate of multiparasitism (two or more parasite eggs per nest). This information is indispensable to determine the strength of selection pressures favouring the evolution of an optimal niche strategy, given that brown-headed cowbirds are four times larger than protonotary warblers. According to Hoover (2003), who worked in the same prothonotary warbler population, more than half of the parasitized nests (51.1%; extracted from table 1 in Hoover [2003]) have two or more cowbird nestlings, signifying that the major competitors of a cowbird nestling are not small-sized host nestlings but other large-sized cowbird nestlings. According to this information, it is difficult to imagine the selection of an optimal niche strategy based on the existence of only host nestlings as nestmates of just one cowbird nestling. This relevant point was neither mentioned nor discussed by the authors.

In summary, before proposing the attractive possibility that a niche construction strategy could have evolved in brown-headed cowbird nestlings, the authors must firstly demonstrate that parasitic nestlings are responsible for selective host brood reduction to get the optimal brood size for the parasitic nestling of just two host nestlings in the nest. Second, citation bias should be avoided, reconciling or explaining the evidence of other studies that contradict the claim of niche selection; and thirdly, address the three important questions that have not been mentioned or discussed in their article. Until these problems are resolved, the reader can consider that the results reported by Antonson et al. (2022), which they claimed to support the niche construction hypothesis, only support the begging assistance hypothesis and may be merely a consequence of the provisioning capacity of the mid-sized protonotary warbler hosts, which could be sufficient to raise one cowbird and two warbler nestlings.

Manuel Soler: Conceptualization; writing – original draft; investigation; writing – review and editing; visualization; funding acquisition.

There is no conflict of interest to disclose.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
棕头牛鸝真的存在利基构建战略适应吗?
由于在共同进化的特殊寄主-寄主系统中发现了许多精细调节的适应,育雏寄生的主题已成为广泛关注的焦点(Davies, 2000;克鲁格,2007;太阳系,2017)。Antonson等人(2022)最近的一项研究声称发现了其中一种令人着迷的适应性,并提出了令人兴奋的结论,即褐头牛鹂(Molothrus ater)雏鸟在原冠莺(Protonotaria citrea)宿主巢穴中使用生态位构建策略(改变自身环境以获得自身健康利益)。在本实验研究中,作者发现,由牛椋鸟雏鸟驱动的选择性减育策略是减少而不是消除宿主窝。根据他们的解释,考虑到雏鸟数量减少的结果,这似乎代表了一种适应性生态位构建策略,而且两只莺巢的雏鸟存活率高于单独或四只莺巢的雏鸟存活率。这是一种有趣的可能性,但是在棕头牛鹂中真的存在一种允许两个同伴生存的生态位构建策略吗?生态位构建是一种机制,通过这种机制,个体主动操纵其环境,以获得更合适的条件,从而增加其生存和繁殖成功的可能性(aby & Ramsey, 2022;Odling-Smee et al., 2013;Trappes et al., 2022)。在鸟类中,筑巢是生态位建设的一个明显例子(Trappes et al., 2022)。普通杜鹃(Cuculus canorus)的筑巢策略,在孵化后不久就把所有的寄主同伴赶出巢,从而垄断了养父母的喂养努力,可以被认为是生态位构建的另一个明显案例。其他幼虫寄生虫(非驱逐者)与寄主雏鸟共享巢穴,但通常情况下,寄主雏鸟比寄主雏鸟竞争更激烈。这也意味着它们对环境(巢穴)的主动操纵,因此,这种策略也可以被认为是生态位构建。然而,Antonson等人(2022)认为,在褐头牛头鸟-原尾莺系统中,幼虫寄生虫选择性地操纵幼虫减少,有利于巢中有两只莺的雏鸟。这一建议是基于四个实验组的雏鸟死亡率数据的比较,两个寄生和两个未寄生处理。然而,生态位构建假说的关键预测——即,牛鹂的雏鸟导致选择性的寄主窝减少,只允许两个寄主窝存活——并没有得到证明。此外,本研究选择性地引用了出版物,并提出了其他几个关键问题。Antonson等人(2022)的研究基于“乞讨协助假说”(Kilner等人,2004),该假说认为,与寄主雏鸟共用巢穴的棕头牛鹂雏鸟比单独饲养的牛鹂雏鸟生长得更快,因为寄生雏鸟利用寄主雏鸟发出的乞讨叫声来获取资源。也就是说,寄主雏鸟的乞讨表现迫使它们的父母增加食物传递的速度,由于其更高的竞争能力,这些额外的食物将被雏鸟消耗掉(Kilner et al., 2004)。为了支持寄生雏鸟需要宿主雏鸟的帮助才能获得足够数量的食物这一假设,作者引用了所有支持这一假设的出版物,包括在该假设提出之前(Lichtenstein & Sealy, 1998)和之后(Gloag et al., 2012;Hoover & Reetz, 2006;Kilner et al., 2004;Li & Hauber, 2021)。其中一篇论文Li和Hauber(2021)明确指出,这一假设仅适用于中型宿主物种。此外,Gloag等人(2012)报告称,乞讨援助假说仅在一个小宿主物种中得到支持,而在一个较大的宿主物种中没有得到支持。不幸的是,Antonson et al.(2022)没有提到任何不支持驱逐幼虫寄生虫乞讨援助假说预测的实验研究(Grim et al., 2009;Hauber & Moskát, 2008;Martín-Gálvez et al., 2005)。Antonson et al.(2022)似乎也没有意识到其他研究(例如Bolopo et al., 2015;Rivers et al., 2010;Soler & de Neve, 2013)和一篇综述(Soler, 2017b)得出的结论是,在几个共用巢穴的寄生虫物种中,较大的雏鸟成功地竞争额外的食物,无论它们是宿主还是寄生虫。Antonson et al.(2022)在他们的论文中没有讨论这一关键因素(即寄生雏鸟相对于宿主雏鸟的大小)是如何影响哪只雏鸟获得额外食物的,因为寄生雏鸟和宿主雏鸟共享的巢穴会产生强烈的乞讨行为。 由于在共同进化的特殊寄主-寄主系统中发现了许多精细调节的适应,育雏寄生的主题已成为广泛关注的焦点(Davies, 2000;克鲁格,2007;太阳系,2017)。Antonson等人(2022)最近的一项研究声称发现了其中一种令人着迷的适应性,并提出了令人兴奋的结论,即褐头牛鹂(Molothrus ater)雏鸟在原冠莺(Protonotaria citrea)宿主巢穴中使用生态位构建策略(改变自身环境以获得自身健康利益)。在本实验研究中,作者发现,由牛椋鸟雏鸟驱动的选择性减育策略是减少而不是消除宿主窝。根据他们的解释,考虑到雏鸟数量减少的结果,这似乎代表了一种适应性生态位构建策略,而且两只莺巢的雏鸟存活率高于单独或四只莺巢的雏鸟存活率。这是一种有趣的可能性,但是在棕头牛鹂中真的存在一种允许两个同伴生存的生态位构建策略吗?生态位构建是个体主动操纵环境以获得更合适的生存条件和繁殖成功率增加的一种机制(aby &拉姆齐,2022;Odling-Smee et al., 2013;Trappes et al., 2022)。在鸟类中,筑巢是生态位建设的一个明显例子(Trappes et al., 2022)。普通杜鹃(Cuculus canorus)的筑巢策略,在孵化后不久就把所有的寄主同伴赶出巢,从而垄断了养父母的喂养努力,可以被认为是生态位构建的另一个明显案例。其他幼虫寄生虫(非驱逐者)与寄主雏鸟共享巢穴,但通常情况下,寄主雏鸟比寄主雏鸟竞争更激烈。这也意味着它们对环境(巢穴)的主动操纵,因此,这种策略也可以被认为是生态位构建。然而,Antonson等人(2022)认为,在褐头牛头鸟-原尾莺系统中,幼虫寄生虫选择性地操纵幼虫减少,有利于巢中有两只莺的雏鸟。这一建议是基于四个实验组的雏鸟死亡率数据的比较,两个寄生和两个未寄生处理。然而,生态位构建假说的关键预测——即,牛鹂的雏鸟导致选择性的寄主窝减少,只允许两个寄主窝存活——并没有得到证明。此外,本研究选择性地引用了出版物,并提出了其他几个关键问题。Antonson等人(2022)的研究基于“乞讨协助假说”(Kilner等人,2004),该假说认为,与寄主雏鸟共用巢穴的棕头牛鹂雏鸟比单独饲养的牛鹂雏鸟生长得更快,因为寄生雏鸟利用寄主雏鸟发出的乞讨叫声来获取资源。也就是说,寄主雏鸟的乞讨表现迫使它们的父母增加食物传递的速度,由于其更高的竞争能力,这些额外的食物将被雏鸟消耗掉(Kilner et al., 2004)。为了支持寄生雏鸟需要宿主雏鸟的帮助来获得足够数量的食物这一假设,作者引用了之前所有支持这一假设的出版物(Lichtenstein &Sealy, 1998)和假说提出后(Gloag et al., 2012;胡佛,Reetz, 2006;Kilner et al., 2004;李,霍伯,2021)。其中一篇论文Li和Hauber(2021)明确指出,这一假设仅适用于中型宿主物种。此外,Gloag等人(2012)报告称,乞讨援助假说仅在一个小宿主物种中得到支持,而在一个较大的宿主物种中没有得到支持。不幸的是,Antonson et al.(2022)没有提到任何不支持驱逐幼虫寄生虫乞讨援助假说预测的实验研究(Grim et al., 2009;霍伯,翻译,2008;Martín-Gálvez et al., 2005)。Antonson et al.(2022)似乎也没有意识到其他研究(例如Bolopo et al., 2015;Rivers et al., 2010;太阳系,de Neve, 2013)和一篇综述(Soler, 2017b)得出的结论是,在几个共享巢穴的寄生虫物种中,较大的雏鸟成功地竞争额外的食物,无论它们是宿主还是寄生虫。Antonson et al.(2022)在他们的论文中没有讨论这一关键因素(即寄生雏鸟相对于宿主雏鸟的大小)是如何影响哪只雏鸟获得额外食物的,因为寄生雏鸟和宿主雏鸟共享的巢穴会产生强烈的乞讨行为。 相反,在手稿的几个地方,作者将乞讨帮助假说推广到所有共享巢穴的育雏寄生虫。他们甚至引用了Gloag等人(2012)的一项研究,该研究在发光牛鹂(Molothrus bonariensis)作为宿主的两种不同大小的物种中验证了这一假设。在小型寄主(鹪鹩,Troglodytes aedon)中,乞讨帮助假说的预测得到了支持,而在大型寄主(白眉模仿鸟,Mimus saturninus)中,单独饲养的寄生虫雏鸟比与寄主雏鸟共享巢穴时的食物摄取量、质量生长和存活率更高,这与乞讨帮助假说相矛盾。Antonson et al.(2022)没有提到这些大型宿主的结果,也没有讨论Gloag et al.(2012)的结论,即在反舌鸟的巢穴中存在宿主雏鸟会对牛鹂的雏鸟产生负面影响。简而言之,这种引用研究的偏见削弱了本研究可以得出的结论(Bouter et al., 2016;Gøtzsche, 2022)。此外,Antonson等人(2022)的研究提出了其他几个主要问题。首先,作者提出生态位构建机制是一种精细的定时平衡适应。这里的关键问题是,考虑到棕头牛鹂是一种极其多面手的寄生虫,属于一个新近的进化支,这种复杂而精细的适应是如何在棕头牛鹂身上进化出来的(Sorenson &佩恩,2002)。虽然其他寄主-寄主系统中的相互作用是高度共同进化的,寄主对寄主寄生有特定的适应,但寄生牛鹂缺乏大多数专门的适应特征(Mermoz &Ornelas, 2004)。此外,对幼虫寄生的精细适应预计主要在特殊的幼虫寄生物种中进化,在这些物种中,选择压力更大,适应和反适应更有可能进化(Davies, 2000)。这就提出了一个问题,即像牛头莺-原尾莺系统中所建议的生态位构建策略这样的微调适应是如何在棕头牛头鸟中进化出来的,这意味着选择性地减少幼鸟数量,有利于只在巢中出现两只莺的雏鸟,棕头牛头鸟是一种经常在一个地点寄生7个或更多物种的幼虫寄生虫,同一只雌性通常寄生于多个宿主物种(kr<e:1>格,2007)。作者没有讨论这个基本问题。第二,牛郎鸟如何减少幼鸟数量,从而达到最佳生态位?Antonson等人(2022)仅使用他们自己的结果通过推测来解决这个问题。然而,当几项研究(例如Ryser et al., 2016;Smith等人,2017)强调,雏鸟对父母的食物分配几乎没有控制,包括在幼虫寄生虫-宿主系统中(Soler等人,2017)。很可能,原莺幼鸟的死亡是饥饿的结果,因为它们在竞争中被更大的雏鸟(成年莺35-45克,成年莺14-16克;Antonson等人,2022),因此它具有明显的优势,因为大多数物种的父母会选择性地喂养较大的雏鸟,即使较小的雏鸟更频繁地乞讨(Caro等人,2016;Soler et al., 2022)。为了令人满意地解决寄主和寄主寄生雏鸟大小差异的影响问题,还需要一个额外的实验组,其中有5只莺雏鸟,其中一只莺的体型是其巢同伴的两倍。选择性寄主减少两只雏鸟的数量可能是原翅莺体型的结果,这是一种中等大小的寄主物种,其供应能力足以抚养一只牛郎鸟和两只莺的雏鸟。第三,Antonson等人(2022)报告说,他们研究区域的寄生率为60%,但他们没有讨论多重寄生率(每个巢穴两个或更多寄生卵)。考虑到褐头牛鹂比原莺大四倍,这些信息对于确定有利于最佳生态位策略进化的选择压力的强度是必不可少的。根据Hoover(2003)的研究,在相同的原翅莺种群中,超过一半的被寄生的巢穴(51.1%;(摘自Hoover[2003]的表1)有两个或两个以上的牛鹂雏鸟,这表明牛鹂雏鸟的主要竞争对手不是小型寄主雏鸟,而是其他大型牛鹂雏鸟。根据这些信息,很难想象在只存在一个寄主雏鸟作为巢伴的情况下,选择最优的生态位策略。 作者既没有提到也没有讨论这个相关的问题。综上所述,在提出棕头牛鹂巢中可能进化出生态位构建策略的有吸引力的可能性之前,作者必须首先证明寄生雏鸟负责选择性地减少寄主的产卵量,以获得巢中只有两个寄主的寄生雏鸟的最佳产卵量。其次,应避免引文偏倚,调和或解释与生态位选择主张相矛盾的其他研究的证据;第三,解决他们的文章中没有提到或讨论的三个重要问题。在这些问题得到解决之前,读者可以认为Antonson等人(2022)报告的结果,他们声称支持生态位构建假说,只支持乞讨援助假说,可能仅仅是中等大小的原莺宿主供应能力的结果,这可能足以抚养一只牛鹂和两只莺的雏鸟。曼纽尔·索勒:概念化;写作——原稿;调查;写作——审阅和编辑;可视化;融资收购。没有需要披露的利益冲突。 作者既没有提到也没有讨论这个相关的问题。综上所述,在提出棕头牛鹂巢中可能进化出生态位构建策略的有吸引力的可能性之前,作者必须首先证明寄生雏鸟负责选择性地减少寄主的产卵量,以获得巢中只有两个寄主的寄生雏鸟的最佳产卵量。其次,应避免引文偏倚,调和或解释与生态位选择主张相矛盾的其他研究的证据;第三,解决他们的文章中没有提到或讨论的三个重要问题。在这些问题得到解决之前,读者可以认为Antonson等人(2022)报告的结果,他们声称支持生态位构建假说,只支持乞讨援助假说,可能仅仅是中等大小的原莺宿主供应能力的结果,这可能足以抚养一只牛鹂和两只莺的雏鸟。曼纽尔·索勒:概念化;写作——原稿;调查;写作——审阅和编辑;可视化;融资收购。我要感谢j.j. Soler, M. Martín-Vivaldi, J. D. Ibáñez-Álamo, M. Šulc, N. Antonson,两位匿名审稿人,尤其是《动物行为学》主编Wolfgang Goymann,他们对手稿的宝贵意见。我还要感谢David Nesbitt和Jose A. Soler Ortiz提高了英语水平。在本文的编写过程中,我得到了法国科学部长部Innovación/欧洲研究基金会(FEDER)的资助(研究项目PID2020-115950GB100)。没有需要披露的利益冲突。未使用任何数据。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Ethology
Ethology 生物-动物学
CiteScore
3.40
自引率
5.90%
发文量
89
审稿时长
4-8 weeks
期刊介绍: International in scope, Ethology publishes original research on behaviour including physiological mechanisms, function, and evolution. The Journal addresses behaviour in all species, from slime moulds to humans. Experimental research is preferred, both from the field and the lab, which is grounded in a theoretical framework. The section ''Perspectives and Current Debates'' provides an overview of the field and may include theoretical investigations and essays on controversial topics.
期刊最新文献
Cover Image Issue Information A Butterfly's Flash Coloration Distracts Predators—Read Future Textbook Knowledge in Ethology Call for Your Life: Acoustic Structure and Age-Sex Differences in Distress Calls of Red-Necked Nightjars The Relevance of Flash Coloration Against Avian Predation in a Morpho Butterfly: A Field Experiment in a Tropical Rainforest
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1