首页 > 最新文献

Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
A Butterfly's Flash Coloration Distracts Predators—Read Future Textbook Knowledge in Ethology 蝴蝶的闪光色彩能分散捕食者的注意力--阅读未来的《选育学》教科书知识
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13520
Wolfgang Goymann
<p>A highlight of visiting a tropical rainforest is watching iridescent butterflies passing by, flashing their colours in the sunlight that makes it to the forest floor. It can be quite tricky to keep track of such a butterfly because typically only the upper side of the wings is iridescent, so that the colour only flashes when this side of a wing can be seen. People think that this kind of moving flash coloration makes it difficult for visual predators to follow the flight path of the butterfly. Hence, iridescent colours may help butterflies to distract predators. Computer simulations and experiments with humans as ‘predators’ suggest that this may be true, but as of now the flash colouration hypothesis had not been tested for real.</p><p>In this issue of Ethology, Vieira-Silva et al. (<span>2024</span>) conducted a series of clever experiments to finally test the flash colouration hypothesis in <i>Morpho helena</i> butterflies. This species displays an iridescent blue colour on the upper side of its wings (as can be seen on this issues' cover image) that flashes when they move through the forest.</p><p>In a first experiment, the authors painted the cryptic underside of the wings with a colour mimicking the iridescent blue of the upper side of the wing. Thereby, the butterflies become more constantly visible during flight, because now the blue colour is exposed all of the time. The underside of the wings of a control group got painted with a brown colour similar to the original cryptic colour, thereby controlling for the effect of catching and painting the butterflies. In a capture–recapture analysis, Vieira-Silva et al. (<span>2024</span>) found that blue-coloured butterflies were less likely to be seen again compared to the brown-coloured control individuals. Because <i>Morpho helena</i> butterflies do not move around far, the most parsimonious explanation for the lower resighting rates of the blue-coloured butterflies is that a higher proportion of them got eaten by predators.</p><p>In a second experiment, Vieira-Silva et al. (<span>2024</span>) tested if an overall cryptic colour as such would have reduced predation. To test this, they coloured the upper side of the wing—which normally has the blue flashing colouration—with brown colour, so that the butterflies become completely cryptic during flight. This treatment, however, did not affect recapture rates, suggesting that completely cryptic butterflies did not have an advantage over individuals that flashed their blue colour during flight. Hence, the distraction effect of a flashing blue colour likely has a similar effect than complete crypsis.</p><p>To show that the higher predation of butterflies with an underside coloured in blue was really due to moving butterflies and not because such butterflies became generally more visible to predators, the authors conducted a third experiment. To test if blue colouration affected predation in non-moving butterflies, they compared how likely dead mo
游览热带雨林的一大亮点是观赏彩蝶飞过,在阳光照射下闪烁着五彩斑斓的色彩。要追踪这样的蝴蝶可能相当棘手,因为通常只有翅膀的上侧才有彩虹色,所以只有当能看到翅膀的这一侧时,颜色才会闪烁。人们认为,这种移动闪烁的色彩使视觉捕食者难以跟踪蝴蝶的飞行路线。因此,彩虹色可能有助于蝴蝶分散捕食者的注意力。本期《动物学报》上,维埃拉-席尔瓦等人(2024 年)进行了一系列巧妙的实验,最终验证了闪色假说。在第一项实验中,作者在蝴蝶翅膀的隐蔽处涂上了与翅膀上侧的彩虹蓝相似的颜色。这样,蝴蝶在飞行过程中就更容易被看到了,因为现在蓝色一直暴露在外面。对照组的翅膀下侧涂上了与原始隐色相似的棕色,从而控制了捕捉和涂色对蝴蝶的影响。在一项捕获-再捕获分析中,Vieira-Silva 等人(2024 年)发现,与棕色对照组相比,蓝色蝴蝶再次出现的可能性较低。由于莫尔菲蝶(Morpho helena)的活动范围不大,因此蓝彩蝶再次被发现的几率较低的最合理解释是,它们中被捕食者吃掉的比例较高。在第二个实验中,Vieira-Silva 等人(2024 年)测试了整体隐色是否会减少捕食。为了测试这一点,他们在蝴蝶翅膀的上侧--通常是蓝色闪光色--涂上了棕色,这样蝴蝶在飞行过程中就完全隐蔽了。然而,这种处理方法并不影响捕获率,这表明完全隐蔽的蝴蝶与飞行时闪烁蓝色的个体相比并不具有优势。为了证明底色为蓝色的蝴蝶被捕食率更高的原因确实是蝴蝶在移动,而不是因为这些蝴蝶在捕食者面前更显眼,作者进行了第三个实验。为了测试蓝色是否会影响不动蝴蝶的捕食行为,他们比较了当蝴蝶翅膀底部涂上隐秘的棕色(模拟自然环境的对照组)或蓝色(与实验 1 类似)时,装死的蝴蝶受到捕食者攻击的可能性。对照组和蓝色蝴蝶坐骑受到的攻击没有区别,这表明不移动的蓝色蝴蝶并不比不移动的隐色蝴蝶吸引更多的捕食者。通过第三个实验,维埃拉-席尔瓦等人(2024 年)可以证明,实验 1 中的运动因素确实导致了更高的捕食率,而不仅仅是更显眼的颜色本身。本刊发表的一些研究成果已成为教科书知识或引文经典。我相信维埃拉-席尔瓦等人(2024 年)的这篇论文有可能成为关于捕食者分心的闪光颜色假说的经典引文。这项研究的实验设计既简单又巧妙。在统计分析日趋复杂的今天,这项研究仍然采用了非常简单的卡方检验统计方法。祝贺作者,并向他们致敬!
{"title":"A Butterfly's Flash Coloration Distracts Predators—Read Future Textbook Knowledge in Ethology","authors":"Wolfgang Goymann","doi":"10.1111/eth.13520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13520","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;A highlight of visiting a tropical rainforest is watching iridescent butterflies passing by, flashing their colours in the sunlight that makes it to the forest floor. It can be quite tricky to keep track of such a butterfly because typically only the upper side of the wings is iridescent, so that the colour only flashes when this side of a wing can be seen. People think that this kind of moving flash coloration makes it difficult for visual predators to follow the flight path of the butterfly. Hence, iridescent colours may help butterflies to distract predators. Computer simulations and experiments with humans as ‘predators’ suggest that this may be true, but as of now the flash colouration hypothesis had not been tested for real.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In this issue of Ethology, Vieira-Silva et al. (&lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;) conducted a series of clever experiments to finally test the flash colouration hypothesis in &lt;i&gt;Morpho helena&lt;/i&gt; butterflies. This species displays an iridescent blue colour on the upper side of its wings (as can be seen on this issues' cover image) that flashes when they move through the forest.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In a first experiment, the authors painted the cryptic underside of the wings with a colour mimicking the iridescent blue of the upper side of the wing. Thereby, the butterflies become more constantly visible during flight, because now the blue colour is exposed all of the time. The underside of the wings of a control group got painted with a brown colour similar to the original cryptic colour, thereby controlling for the effect of catching and painting the butterflies. In a capture–recapture analysis, Vieira-Silva et al. (&lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;) found that blue-coloured butterflies were less likely to be seen again compared to the brown-coloured control individuals. Because &lt;i&gt;Morpho helena&lt;/i&gt; butterflies do not move around far, the most parsimonious explanation for the lower resighting rates of the blue-coloured butterflies is that a higher proportion of them got eaten by predators.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In a second experiment, Vieira-Silva et al. (&lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;) tested if an overall cryptic colour as such would have reduced predation. To test this, they coloured the upper side of the wing—which normally has the blue flashing colouration—with brown colour, so that the butterflies become completely cryptic during flight. This treatment, however, did not affect recapture rates, suggesting that completely cryptic butterflies did not have an advantage over individuals that flashed their blue colour during flight. Hence, the distraction effect of a flashing blue colour likely has a similar effect than complete crypsis.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;To show that the higher predation of butterflies with an underside coloured in blue was really due to moving butterflies and not because such butterflies became generally more visible to predators, the authors conducted a third experiment. To test if blue colouration affected predation in non-moving butterflies, they compared how likely dead mo","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13520","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142641184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Call for Your Life: Acoustic Structure and Age-Sex Differences in Distress Calls of Red-Necked Nightjars 呼唤生命:红颈夜鸦窘迫叫声的声学结构和年龄性别差异
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-25 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13513
Javier Sierro, Diego Gil, Pedro Sáez-Gómez, Paula Hidalgo-Rodríguez, Julio Rabadán-González, Carlos Camacho

Predatory interactions result in strong selection pressures acting on multiple aspects of animal behaviour. Anti-predator strategies are therefore common in most animals, typically signalling at various stages of a predation event. Many species of caprimulgids perform conspicuous anti-predator displays, including stereotyped behaviours and vocal signals. Here, we described distress calls of red-necked nightjars (Caprimulgus ruficollis), produced when birds are trapped and unable to escape during a predatory interaction. Distress calls are harsh, low-frequency guttural vocalisations with irregular amplitude modulations. The age and sex of birds partially explained the acoustic variation observed, whereas size-related morphological features were poor predictors of the acoustic structure. Age-sex differences in distress calls may respond to physiological constraints associated with sexual dimorphism and/or developmental variation. Alternatively, directional selection associated with differential predation risk on each age-sex class may have resulted in the observed differences in distress calls. The extremely deep voice and the harsh quality of distress calls fit the structure of aggressive signals and may resemble those produced by a larger animal. We propose that these calls serve as a last resort strategy to reduce post-encounter risk of predation, either as a startling effect to facilitate escape or to attract other predators that could intimidate the captor.

捕食互动会对动物行为的多个方面产生强大的选择压力。因此,反捕食策略在大多数动物中都很常见,通常是在捕食事件的不同阶段发出信号。许多种类的毛冠鱼都有明显的反捕食表现,包括刻板行为和声音信号。在这里,我们描述了红颈夜鸦(Caprimulgus ruficollis)的求救鸣叫,这种鸣叫是在捕食过程中鸟类被困无法逃脱时发出的。求救鸣叫是一种刺耳的低频肠鸣音,具有不规则的振幅调制。鸟类的年龄和性别可以部分解释所观察到的声学变化,而与体型相关的形态特征则不能很好地预测声学结构。求救鸣声的年龄-性别差异可能与性二型和/或发育变异相关的生理限制有关。另外,与各年龄-性别类别不同的捕食风险相关的定向选择也可能导致了所观察到的求救信号的差异。求救信号的声音极度低沉,音质刺耳,符合攻击性信号的结构,可能与大型动物发出的求救信号相似。我们认为,这些求救信号是降低捕食后风险的最后手段,可以起到惊吓作用,帮助捕食者逃脱,也可以吸引其他捕食者来恐吓捕食者。
{"title":"Call for Your Life: Acoustic Structure and Age-Sex Differences in Distress Calls of Red-Necked Nightjars","authors":"Javier Sierro,&nbsp;Diego Gil,&nbsp;Pedro Sáez-Gómez,&nbsp;Paula Hidalgo-Rodríguez,&nbsp;Julio Rabadán-González,&nbsp;Carlos Camacho","doi":"10.1111/eth.13513","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13513","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Predatory interactions result in strong selection pressures acting on multiple aspects of animal behaviour. Anti-predator strategies are therefore common in most animals, typically signalling at various stages of a predation event. Many species of caprimulgids perform conspicuous anti-predator displays, including stereotyped behaviours and vocal signals. Here, we described distress calls of red-necked nightjars (<i>Caprimulgus ruficollis</i>), produced when birds are trapped and unable to escape during a predatory interaction. Distress calls are harsh, low-frequency guttural vocalisations with irregular amplitude modulations. The age and sex of birds partially explained the acoustic variation observed, whereas size-related morphological features were poor predictors of the acoustic structure. Age-sex differences in distress calls may respond to physiological constraints associated with sexual dimorphism and/or developmental variation. Alternatively, directional selection associated with differential predation risk on each age-sex class may have resulted in the observed differences in distress calls. The extremely deep voice and the harsh quality of distress calls fit the structure of aggressive signals and may resemble those produced by a larger animal. We propose that these calls serve as a last resort strategy to reduce post-encounter risk of predation, either as a startling effect to facilitate escape or to attract other predators that could intimidate the captor.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13513","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642365","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relevance of Flash Coloration Against Avian Predation in a Morpho Butterfly: A Field Experiment in a Tropical Rainforest 斑蝶的闪光色彩与鸟类捕食的相关性:热带雨林中的现场实验
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13517
Aline Vieira-Silva, Gabriel B. Evora, André V. L. Freitas, Paulo S. Oliveira

The flash coloration hypothesis postulates that otherwise cryptically colored animals suddenly displaying conspicuous colors during movement confuse predators, reducing capture. Morpho helenor butterflies have contrasting colors on dorsal (iridescent blue) and ventral (brown) wing surfaces, resulting in sequential blue “flashes” during flight. We tested whether this flashing pattern reduces avian predation on M. helenor in Atlantic rainforest by changing the flashing effect in three experiments. In Experiment 1, we added a blue band to the ventral wing. In Experiment 2, we covered the dorsal wing's blue band with a brown band. Control groups in each experiment were painted such that wing color patterns remained unaltered. Survivorship was evaluated through mark-recapture censuses and beak marks on the wings. Results show that survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 1 decrease markedly compared to unaltered control individuals, while survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 2 did not differ compared to control individuals. In Experiment 3, we detected scant predation on treated (blue band added to ventral wing) and control butterflies (brown band added to ventral wing) on the forest floor (wings closed), corroborating that flash coloration is an important protective mechanism during flight. Our field experiments provide the first evidence, to our knowledge, that flash coloration in bright blue Morpho butterflies is an effective defense mechanism against avian predators in a tropical rainforest.

闪光着色假说认为,原本颜色隐秘的动物在运动过程中突然显示出明显的颜色,会迷惑捕食者,从而减少捕获量。Morpho helenor 蝴蝶的背翅(彩蓝)和腹翅(棕色)表面颜色对比鲜明,因此在飞行过程中会连续 "闪烁 "蓝色。我们在三个实验中通过改变闪烁效果来测试这种闪烁模式是否会减少大西洋雨林中鸟类对 M. helenor 的捕食。在实验 1 中,我们在腹翅上添加了一条蓝带。在实验 2 中,我们在背翅的蓝色条带上覆盖了一条棕色条带。每个实验的对照组都涂上了颜色,使翅膀的颜色图案保持不变。存活率通过标记重捕普查和翅膀上的喙痕进行评估。结果表明,实验 1 中经过处理的蝴蝶的存活率与未经改变的对照组相比明显下降,而实验 2 中经过处理的蝴蝶的存活率与对照组相比没有差异。在实验 3 中,我们在森林地面(翅膀闭合)上发现,处理过的蝴蝶(腹翅上有蓝色条带)和对照组蝴蝶(腹翅上有棕色条带)很少被捕食,这证实了闪光着色是蝴蝶在飞行过程中的一种重要保护机制。据我们所知,我们的野外实验首次证明了亮蓝莫弗蝶的闪光着色是热带雨林中一种有效的抵御鸟类捕食者的防御机制。
{"title":"The Relevance of Flash Coloration Against Avian Predation in a Morpho Butterfly: A Field Experiment in a Tropical Rainforest","authors":"Aline Vieira-Silva,&nbsp;Gabriel B. Evora,&nbsp;André V. L. Freitas,&nbsp;Paulo S. Oliveira","doi":"10.1111/eth.13517","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13517","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The flash coloration hypothesis postulates that otherwise cryptically colored animals suddenly displaying conspicuous colors during movement confuse predators, reducing capture. <i>Morpho helenor</i> butterflies have contrasting colors on dorsal (iridescent blue) and ventral (brown) wing surfaces, resulting in sequential blue “flashes” during flight. We tested whether this flashing pattern reduces avian predation on <i>M</i>. <i>helenor</i> in Atlantic rainforest by changing the flashing effect in three experiments. In Experiment 1, we added a blue band to the ventral wing. In Experiment 2, we covered the dorsal wing's blue band with a brown band. Control groups in each experiment were painted such that wing color patterns remained unaltered. Survivorship was evaluated through mark-recapture censuses and beak marks on the wings. Results show that survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 1 decrease markedly compared to unaltered control individuals, while survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 2 did not differ compared to control individuals. In Experiment 3, we detected scant predation on treated (blue band added to ventral wing) and control butterflies (brown band added to ventral wing) on the forest floor (wings closed), corroborating that flash coloration is an important protective mechanism during flight. Our field experiments provide the first evidence, to our knowledge, that flash coloration in bright blue <i>Morpho</i> butterflies is an effective defense mechanism against avian predators in a tropical rainforest.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pair-Coordinated Calling: Eurasian Magpies Respond Differently to Simulated Intruder Pairs That Overlap or Alternate Their Calls 成对协调的叫声:欧亚喜鹊对呼叫重叠或交替的模拟入侵者配对做出不同反应
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13515
Miriam Kuspiel, Sjouke A. Kingma, Heleen Vermeulen, Marc Naguib

Animal vocalisations are widely used to signal strength or motivation of a caller in competitive interactions, such as in territorial defence. Substantial understanding of signalling functions in territorial conflicts is based on singing by male songbirds. Yet, in many species, both pair members call during territorial conflicts, as well as in predator-induced situations, leading to complex signalling interactions in which calls overlap or alternate. This raises the question as to whether or not variation in how individuals in pairs time their calls is perceived as meaningful by receivers. Here, we tested with playback experiments whether Eurasian magpies (Pica pica), a species producing alarm calls (so-called chatter calls) in territorial defence, respond stronger to simulated pair-intruders who overlap their calls with each other than to those who alternate them. Magpies emitted a significantly longer first chatter calls in response to playback with overlapping calls but chattered significantly sooner and approached the loudspeakers significantly more closely in response to playbacks of alternating (and therefore longer) call sequences. These findings exemplify that the timing of calls by pair members matters, but in more complex ways than we predicted. The overlapping playback appeared to trigger a longer yet later initial chatter response and a weaker approach response, suggesting that the different ways in which magpies respond reflect different levels of arousal or defence strategies. The results may also reflect uncertainty by receivers due to a potential mismatch between signalled and perceived information: While overlapping calls may signal high arousal by both callers, a longer alternating sequence could be perceived as a more aroused longer signal. These findings expand on classical experiments on call function, suggesting that pairs can vary the message by coordinating their alarm calls in different ways, similar to how duetting pairs time their song contributions in advertisement signalling.

动物的发声被广泛用于在竞争性互动(如领地保卫战)中发出呼叫者的实力或动机信号。对领地冲突中信号功能的大量了解是基于雄性鸣禽的歌唱。然而,在许多物种中,一对成员都会在领地冲突期间以及捕食者诱发的情况下发出叫声,从而导致叫声重叠或交替的复杂信号互动。这就提出了一个问题:成对个体在鸣叫时间上的差异是否会被接收者认为是有意义的。在这里,我们通过回放实验测试了欧亚喜鹊(Pica pica)这种在领地防御中发出警报声(即所谓的喋喋不休的叫声)的物种是否会对相互重叠发出叫声的模拟成对入侵者做出比交替发出叫声者更强烈的反应。喜鹊对重放的重叠叫声的第一声鸣叫时间明显更长,但对重放的交替(因此更长)叫声序列的第一声鸣叫时间明显更短,而且靠近扬声器的距离明显更近。这些发现说明,鸟对成员的鸣叫时间很重要,但其复杂程度超出了我们的预测。重叠播放似乎引发了更长但更晚的初始鸣叫反应和更弱的接近反应,这表明喜鹊的不同反应方式反映了不同的唤醒水平或防御策略。这些结果也可能反映了接收者由于信号信息和感知信息之间的潜在不匹配而产生的不确定性:虽然重叠的鸣叫可能意味着双方都处于高度亢奋状态,但较长的交替序列可能被认为是更亢奋的较长信号。这些发现拓展了经典的鸣叫功能实验,表明成对鸣叫可以通过不同的方式协调它们的警报鸣叫来改变信息,这与广告信号中二重唱成对鸣叫的时间安排类似。
{"title":"Pair-Coordinated Calling: Eurasian Magpies Respond Differently to Simulated Intruder Pairs That Overlap or Alternate Their Calls","authors":"Miriam Kuspiel,&nbsp;Sjouke A. Kingma,&nbsp;Heleen Vermeulen,&nbsp;Marc Naguib","doi":"10.1111/eth.13515","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13515","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Animal vocalisations are widely used to signal strength or motivation of a caller in competitive interactions, such as in territorial defence. Substantial understanding of signalling functions in territorial conflicts is based on singing by male songbirds. Yet, in many species, both pair members call during territorial conflicts, as well as in predator-induced situations, leading to complex signalling interactions in which calls overlap or alternate. This raises the question as to whether or not variation in how individuals in pairs time their calls is perceived as meaningful by receivers. Here, we tested with playback experiments whether Eurasian magpies (<i>Pica pica</i>), a species producing alarm calls (so-called chatter calls) in territorial defence, respond stronger to simulated pair-intruders who overlap their calls with each other than to those who alternate them. Magpies emitted a significantly longer first chatter calls in response to playback with overlapping calls but chattered significantly sooner and approached the loudspeakers significantly more closely in response to playbacks of alternating (and therefore longer) call sequences. These findings exemplify that the timing of calls by pair members matters, but in more complex ways than we predicted. The overlapping playback appeared to trigger a longer yet later initial chatter response and a weaker approach response, suggesting that the different ways in which magpies respond reflect different levels of arousal or defence strategies. The results may also reflect uncertainty by receivers due to a potential mismatch between signalled and perceived information: While overlapping calls may signal high arousal by both callers, a longer alternating sequence could be perceived as a more aroused longer signal. These findings expand on classical experiments on call function, suggesting that pairs can vary the message by coordinating their alarm calls in different ways, similar to how duetting pairs time their song contributions in advertisement signalling.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13515","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642395","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Agoutis: A Future Model for Ecologically Relevant Neuroscience and Physiology In Natura 阿古提鱼:自然界中与生态相关的神经科学和生理学的未来模型
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13514
J. I. Sanguinetti-Scheck, D. Gálvez

The overarching goal of neurobiology is to understand how complex behaviors are generated by the nervous system. The behavior of each species, and the brain that controls it, is shaped by the historical and current state of the environment that they inhabit. This fact is juxtaposed with the reductionist approach of neuroscience that isolates animals from their natural environment. Understanding how brains evolved to orchestrate the myriads of natural behaviors an animal performs in response to its environment requires an integrative approach to neuroscience that considers ecology, ethology, and evolution. Current technological developments are leading us to an inflection point at which studying brain functions in the wild is now possible. Ecological studies on how the environment affects behavior of animals (i.e., hibernation, foraging, food hoarding, and nest building) have framed a plurality of questions to be answered mechanistically, and yet, only few studies have addressed the relationship between the environment and the brain's anatomy and physiology. Neuroscience needs new animal models that allow us to tackle such questions in the wild. Here, we propose a new animal model for wild neuroscience, the agouti (Dasyprocta spp.), a large wild rodent playing a critical seasonal role in the maintenance of the central and south American rainforest ecosystems. We focus on how a rodent model, like the agouti, will allow for the investigation of large-scale brain dynamics during seasonal behaviors of ecological importance: scatter-hoarding and retrieval. We describe agouti evolution, ecology, and physiology as well as neuro-anatomical and neurophysiological studies, which have set the foundation for future neuroscience in natura. We suggest agoutis have the potential to be a groundbreaking model for wild neuroscience.

神经生物学的首要目标是了解神经系统是如何产生复杂行为的。每个物种的行为以及控制行为的大脑,都是由它们所居住的环境的历史和现状决定的。这一事实与神经科学将动物与自然环境隔离开来的还原论方法形成了鲜明对比。要了解大脑是如何进化以协调动物为适应环境而做出的无数自然行为,就需要神经科学采用一种综合方法,将生态学、伦理学和进化论考虑在内。当前的技术发展正将我们引向一个拐点,即在野外研究大脑功能现已成为可能。关于环境如何影响动物行为(如冬眠、觅食、囤积食物和筑巢)的生态学研究提出了许多需要从机理上回答的问题,但只有少数研究涉及环境与大脑解剖学和生理学之间的关系。神经科学需要新的动物模型,让我们能够在野外解决这些问题。在这里,我们为野生神经科学提出了一种新的动物模型--阿古提(Dasyprocta spp.),这是一种大型野生啮齿类动物,在维持中美洲和南美洲雨林生态系统中扮演着重要的季节性角色。我们的研究重点是如何利用啮齿类动物模型(如阿古提)来研究具有重要生态意义的季节性行为(分散囤积和检索)期间的大规模大脑动态。我们介绍了金丝猴的进化、生态学和生理学以及神经解剖学和神经生理学研究,这些研究为未来的自然神经科学奠定了基础。我们认为无尾熊有可能成为野生神经科学的开创性模型。
{"title":"The Agoutis: A Future Model for Ecologically Relevant Neuroscience and Physiology In Natura","authors":"J. I. Sanguinetti-Scheck,&nbsp;D. Gálvez","doi":"10.1111/eth.13514","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13514","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The overarching goal of neurobiology is to understand how complex behaviors are generated by the nervous system. The behavior of each species, and the brain that controls it, is shaped by the historical and current state of the environment that they inhabit. This fact is juxtaposed with the reductionist approach of neuroscience that isolates animals from their natural environment. Understanding how brains evolved to orchestrate the myriads of natural behaviors an animal performs in response to its environment requires an integrative approach to neuroscience that considers ecology, ethology, and evolution. Current technological developments are leading us to an inflection point at which studying brain functions in the wild is now possible. Ecological studies on how the environment affects behavior of animals (i.e., hibernation, foraging, food hoarding, and nest building) have framed a plurality of questions to be answered mechanistically, and yet, only few studies have addressed the relationship between the environment and the brain's anatomy and physiology. Neuroscience needs new animal models that allow us to tackle such questions in the wild. Here, we propose a new animal model for wild neuroscience, the agouti (<i>Dasyprocta</i> spp.), a large wild rodent playing a critical seasonal role in the maintenance of the central and south American rainforest ecosystems. We focus on how a rodent model, like the agouti, will allow for the investigation of large-scale brain dynamics during seasonal behaviors of ecological importance: scatter-hoarding and retrieval. We describe agouti evolution, ecology, and physiology as well as neuro-anatomical and neurophysiological studies, which have set the foundation for future neuroscience in natura. We suggest agoutis have the potential to be a groundbreaking model for wild neuroscience.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13514","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are scientific journals delaying doctoral theses? 科学期刊是否耽误了博士论文?
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13507
Wolfgang Goymann
<p>In case you are surprised, such email messages are not uncommon. In fact, the first such email I received was quite effective in exerting moral pressure on me as an editor. The message attempted to hold me accountable for the timely submission of a thesis. Presumably, this even comes with a kind of obligation to also accept the manuscript, no matter how crappy it might be. Otherwise, I as editor would be responsible for a doctoral student's failure, thereby possibly ending a promising scientific career. But is this really so?</p><p>Editors of reputable scientific journals have an ethical obligation to readers and authors to accept and publish manuscripts on the basis of scientific quality and merit. Therefore—after an initial screening to assess the overall suitability for the journal—manuscripts must be peer-reviewed. Based on the reviewers' comments and the editor's own assessment, the editor then decides whether to accept the manuscript, whether it needs some revision, or whether it should be rejected. The possibility to reject a manuscript is essential in this process—unless you work for a predatory journal whose only interest is to generate revenue for the publisher.</p><p>In <i>Ethology</i>, roughly half of all submissions are eventually rejected, so any attempt to guarantee an author a (positive) decision and within a set period of time would be irresponsible and unethical to all other authors and readers of the journal, who trust in the journal's reputation and expect only high-quality behavioural research to be published by <i>Ethology</i>.</p><p>Presumably, most people who have written an email such as the one mentioned above are not even aware that what they do is unethical. They probably write such emails in an attempt to help their students finish their theses in time. But where does the expectation come from that a journal could make a (presumably) positive decision within a certain period of time? The problem may have to do with how many universities deal with the submission of dissertations.</p><p>When I was a doctoral student, it was still common practice at German universities to submit dissertations as monographs. My university was sort of progressive in that it allowed doctoral students to structure their dissertations into separate chapters, each of which could be published as a separate paper. However, the pressure on doctoral students to publish as early as possible and ideally before finishing their degree has strongly increased since then. As a consequence, my university also changed its policy: now theses have to be submitted as monographs or cumulatively as separate chapters, two of which have to be published or at least have to be accepted by a scientific journal before thesis submission. Most doctoral students in biology choose such a cumulative thesis, even if this comes at the cost of losing time with submitting and revising manuscripts. As a consequence, funding often runs out long before the chapters have been
如果你感到惊讶,这样的电子邮件并不少见。事实上,我收到的第一封此类邮件就对我这个编辑施加了相当有效的道德压力。该邮件试图让我对及时提交论文负责。据推测,这甚至意味着我也有义务接受稿件,不管它有多糟糕。否则,作为编辑的我就要为一个博士生的失败负责,从而可能断送他前途无量的科研事业。知名科学期刊的编辑对读者和作者负有道德义务,必须根据稿件的科学质量和价值来接受和发表稿件。因此,在经过初步筛选以评估稿件是否适合该期刊后,必须对稿件进行同行评审。编辑根据审稿人的意见和自己的评估,决定是否接受稿件,是否需要修改,或者是否应该退稿。在这一过程中,退稿的可能性至关重要--除非你供职于掠夺性期刊,其唯一的兴趣就是为出版商创收。在《文选》中,大约一半的投稿最终都会被拒,因此,任何试图保证作者在规定时间内得到(肯定的)决定的做法,都是对所有其他作者和期刊读者的不负责任和不道德,因为他们信任期刊的声誉,并期望《文选》只发表高质量的行为学研究成果。他们写这样的邮件可能是为了帮助学生及时完成论文。但是,期望期刊在一定时间内做出(可能是)积极决定的想法从何而来?这个问题可能与许多大学如何处理学位论文的提交有关。当我还是一名博士生时,德国大学的普遍做法仍然是以专著的形式提交学位论文。我所在的大学算是比较先进的,它允许博士生将论文分成独立的章节,每一章都可以作为单独的论文发表。然而,自那以后,博士生尽早发表论文(最好在完成学位之前)的压力与日俱增。因此,我所在的大学也改变了政策:现在论文必须以专著或单独章节的形式提交,其中两章必须发表,或至少在提交论文前被科学杂志接受。大多数生物学博士生都选择这种累积式论文,即使这样做的代价是在提交和修改手稿方面浪费时间。因此,往往在章节发表和论文提交之前,经费早已用完。在这个过程中,就出现了上文提到的电子邮件,试图让编辑对及时提交论文负起道德责任。但是,如果说有什么道德义务让博士生按时提交论文的话,那也是大学及其投稿规则的责任,而不是期刊或编辑的责任。最近,我的一个学生为发表他的第一章论文而苦苦挣扎。经过两轮同行评审,审稿人对所有改动都很满意,并建议发表,但随后期刊编辑又开始了四轮编辑评审,要求对稿件的内容和形式做进一步的实质性改动。我当时就在想,这个编辑是在妨碍我的学生按时完成他的论文(而且会耗尽经费)。但我不得不承认,这不是编辑的责任。作为编辑,他要对期刊发表的稿件的质量和完整性负责:毕竟,我们决定把我学生的作品投给这本期刊,是因为它有很高的声誉。我可能不同意这位编辑的微观管理方法,但可以肯定的是,我的学生及时提交论文的责任不在他......Wolfgang Goymann:构思;写作--原稿;写作--审阅和编辑。
{"title":"Are scientific journals delaying doctoral theses?","authors":"Wolfgang Goymann","doi":"10.1111/eth.13507","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13507","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;In case you are surprised, such email messages are not uncommon. In fact, the first such email I received was quite effective in exerting moral pressure on me as an editor. The message attempted to hold me accountable for the timely submission of a thesis. Presumably, this even comes with a kind of obligation to also accept the manuscript, no matter how crappy it might be. Otherwise, I as editor would be responsible for a doctoral student's failure, thereby possibly ending a promising scientific career. But is this really so?&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Editors of reputable scientific journals have an ethical obligation to readers and authors to accept and publish manuscripts on the basis of scientific quality and merit. Therefore—after an initial screening to assess the overall suitability for the journal—manuscripts must be peer-reviewed. Based on the reviewers' comments and the editor's own assessment, the editor then decides whether to accept the manuscript, whether it needs some revision, or whether it should be rejected. The possibility to reject a manuscript is essential in this process—unless you work for a predatory journal whose only interest is to generate revenue for the publisher.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In &lt;i&gt;Ethology&lt;/i&gt;, roughly half of all submissions are eventually rejected, so any attempt to guarantee an author a (positive) decision and within a set period of time would be irresponsible and unethical to all other authors and readers of the journal, who trust in the journal's reputation and expect only high-quality behavioural research to be published by &lt;i&gt;Ethology&lt;/i&gt;.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Presumably, most people who have written an email such as the one mentioned above are not even aware that what they do is unethical. They probably write such emails in an attempt to help their students finish their theses in time. But where does the expectation come from that a journal could make a (presumably) positive decision within a certain period of time? The problem may have to do with how many universities deal with the submission of dissertations.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;When I was a doctoral student, it was still common practice at German universities to submit dissertations as monographs. My university was sort of progressive in that it allowed doctoral students to structure their dissertations into separate chapters, each of which could be published as a separate paper. However, the pressure on doctoral students to publish as early as possible and ideally before finishing their degree has strongly increased since then. As a consequence, my university also changed its policy: now theses have to be submitted as monographs or cumulatively as separate chapters, two of which have to be published or at least have to be accepted by a scientific journal before thesis submission. Most doctoral students in biology choose such a cumulative thesis, even if this comes at the cost of losing time with submitting and revising manuscripts. As a consequence, funding often runs out long before the chapters have been ","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13507","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142447770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Nest-Site Availability on Male–Male Competition and the Foraging Costs of Egg Attendance in an Arachnid With Exclusive Paternal Care 巢穴的可利用性对雄性-雄性竞争的影响以及蛛形纲独享父性照料的觅卵成本
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13511
Laís A. Grossel, Rachel M. Werneck, Glauco Machado

Natural cavities are monopolizable resources used as nest sites across many species exhibiting exclusive male care. However, our understanding of how the availability of this type of reproductive site influences male–male competition and the foraging costs associated with parental care is limited and based on studies of fish. The harvestman Magnispina neptunus offers the opportunity to explore these questions in invertebrates because males defend natural cavities used as oviposition sites and provide exclusive parental care. We hypothesized that low nest availability would increase male–male competition and the foraging costs of egg attendance. To test this hypothesis, we collected observational data in the field and conducted a laboratory experiment with two groups, low and high nest availability. Field data indicate natural cavity scarcity, as only 50% of the males were nest holders. Larger males were more likely to hold nests in the field, but body size did not predict nest ownership in the laboratory. In the field, nests with openings smaller than average were equally occupied by small and large males, while nests with openings larger than average were occupied almost exclusively by large males. The nest opening in the laboratory experiment was adjusted to be equal to the average found in the field, which may have increased nest defensibility, regardless of male size. Thus, prior residency may outweigh body size in determining nest possession. Because nest owners seem to have an advantage over intruders, the costs of abandoning a nest are likely high. This may explain why the frequency of nest takeovers and foraging outside the nests did not differ between experimental groups. In conclusion, although underexplored in empirical studies, external factors, such as the opening size of cavities defended by males of many species with resource-defense mating systems, may play a key role in the defensibility of nest sites.

天然洞穴是一种可垄断的资源,在许多物种中被用作巢穴,表现出专由雄性照料的特点。然而,我们对这种生殖场所的可用性如何影响雄性之间的竞争以及与父母照料相关的觅食成本的了解是有限的,而且是基于对鱼类的研究。由于雄性动物会保护作为产卵地点的天然洞穴,并提供专有的亲代照料,因此采食者Magnispina neptunus为在无脊椎动物中探讨这些问题提供了机会。我们假设,巢的可用性低会加剧雄性之间的竞争,增加产卵的觅食成本。为了验证这一假设,我们在野外收集了观察数据,并在实验室进行了巢穴可用性低和巢穴可用性高两组实验。野外数据表明自然巢穴稀缺,因为只有50%的雄性是巢的持有者。在野外,体型较大的雄性更有可能筑巢,但在实验室中,体型大小并不能预测巢的拥有情况。在野外,巢口小于平均值的巢被大小雄性平均占有,而巢口大于平均值的巢几乎只被大雄性占有。在实验室实验中,巢的开口被调整到与野外发现的平均值相等,这可能会增加巢的可防御性,而与雄性的大小无关。因此,在决定是否拥有巢时,先前的居住地可能比体型更重要。由于巢的拥有者似乎比入侵者更有优势,因此放弃巢的成本可能很高。这也许可以解释为什么不同实验组之间在巢外占巢和觅食的频率没有差异。总之,尽管在实证研究中探索不足,但外部因素,如许多具有资源防御交配系统的物种的雄性所防御的洞穴的开口大小,可能对巢穴的可防御性起着关键作用。
{"title":"Effects of Nest-Site Availability on Male–Male Competition and the Foraging Costs of Egg Attendance in an Arachnid With Exclusive Paternal Care","authors":"Laís A. Grossel,&nbsp;Rachel M. Werneck,&nbsp;Glauco Machado","doi":"10.1111/eth.13511","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13511","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Natural cavities are monopolizable resources used as nest sites across many species exhibiting exclusive male care. However, our understanding of how the availability of this type of reproductive site influences male–male competition and the foraging costs associated with parental care is limited and based on studies of fish. The harvestman <i>Magnispina neptunus</i> offers the opportunity to explore these questions in invertebrates because males defend natural cavities used as oviposition sites and provide exclusive parental care. We hypothesized that low nest availability would increase male–male competition and the foraging costs of egg attendance. To test this hypothesis, we collected observational data in the field and conducted a laboratory experiment with two groups, low and high nest availability. Field data indicate natural cavity scarcity, as only 50% of the males were nest holders. Larger males were more likely to hold nests in the field, but body size did not predict nest ownership in the laboratory. In the field, nests with openings smaller than average were equally occupied by small and large males, while nests with openings larger than average were occupied almost exclusively by large males. The nest opening in the laboratory experiment was adjusted to be equal to the average found in the field, which may have increased nest defensibility, regardless of male size. Thus, prior residency may outweigh body size in determining nest possession. Because nest owners seem to have an advantage over intruders, the costs of abandoning a nest are likely high. This may explain why the frequency of nest takeovers and foraging outside the nests did not differ between experimental groups. In conclusion, although underexplored in empirical studies, external factors, such as the opening size of cavities defended by males of many species with resource-defense mating systems, may play a key role in the defensibility of nest sites.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142641577","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Uncoupled Evolutionary Patterns in Spectral and Temporal Components of Acoustic Signals in Anurans Associated With Streams 与溪流有关的无尾类动物声信号的频谱和时间成分的非耦合进化模式
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13510
Fernando Vargas-Salinas, Carlos A. Londoño-Guarnizo, Sebastián Duarte-Marín, Olga L. Torres-Suárez, Adolfo Amézquita

The use of high-pitched auditory signals by species communicating alongside noisy streams has been often considered an adaptation, yet studies testing this hypothesis have yielded inconclusive results. The major challenge has been to quantify the proportion of across-species signal variation that could be attributed to either common history (phylogenetic load) or adaptation. We further advance in this approach by comparing the phylogenetic load between different components of anuran calls. Because stream noise allegedly represents a stronger selective pressure for call frequency than for call temporal traits, we predicted a weaker phylogenetic signal in call frequency, particularly in the taxa that breed alongside streams. We first built a phylogenetic hypothesis using four mitochondrial genes on each of three clades: the genus Hyloscirtus and the family Centrolenidae, which call alongside streams and the subfamily Phyllomedusinae, known to call at lentic water bodies. In parallel, we compiled data on the advertisement calls of 154 species and used them to calculate Blomberg's K values as a proxy for the phylogenetic load (signal) of the call traits. The phylogenetic signal was weaker in spectral than in temporal call traits within the Hyloscirtus calls and weak or absent in both spectral and temporal components of Phyllomedusine calls. Against our expectations, the phylogenetic signal was strong in call frequency, but absent in call temporal components of the centrolenid calls. Our results support uncoupled evolution between spectral and temporal components of anuran calls. They also indicate that the selective role of abiotic noise varies among taxa and that other factors must be invoked to fully understand among-species variation in advertisement calls.

在嘈杂的溪流边进行交流的物种使用高音调听觉信号通常被认为是一种适应,但对这一假设进行检验的研究却没有得出结论。主要的挑战是量化可归因于共同历史(系统发育负荷)或适应的跨物种信号变异的比例。我们通过比较无尾类叫声不同成分之间的系统发育负荷,进一步推进了这一研究方法。由于溪流噪声据称对鸣叫频率的选择压力大于对鸣叫时间特征的选择压力,我们预测鸣叫频率的系统发育信号较弱,尤其是在溪流边繁殖的类群中。我们首先利用三个支系中每个支系的四个线粒体基因建立了一个系统发育假说,这三个支系分别是:在溪流边鸣叫的 Hyloscirtus 属和 Centrolenidae 科,以及已知在湖泊水体中鸣叫的 Phyllomedusinae 亚科。与此同时,我们还收集了 154 个物种的广告鸣叫数据,并利用这些数据计算了布隆伯格 K 值,以此来代表鸣叫特征的系统发生负荷(信号)。在Hyloscirtus的叫声中,系统发生信号在频谱上比在时间上弱,而在Phyllomedusine的叫声中,频谱和时间部分的系统发生信号都很弱或不存在。与我们的预期相反,系统发生学信号在百灵鸟的叫声频率中很强,但在叫声时间成分中却不存在。我们的研究结果支持无尾类叫声的频谱和时间成分之间的非耦合进化。这些结果还表明,非生物噪声在不同类群中的选择性作用是不同的,要全面了解广告叫声的种间变异,还必须考虑其他因素。
{"title":"Uncoupled Evolutionary Patterns in Spectral and Temporal Components of Acoustic Signals in Anurans Associated With Streams","authors":"Fernando Vargas-Salinas,&nbsp;Carlos A. Londoño-Guarnizo,&nbsp;Sebastián Duarte-Marín,&nbsp;Olga L. Torres-Suárez,&nbsp;Adolfo Amézquita","doi":"10.1111/eth.13510","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13510","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The use of high-pitched auditory signals by species communicating alongside noisy streams has been often considered an adaptation, yet studies testing this hypothesis have yielded inconclusive results. The major challenge has been to quantify the proportion of across-species signal variation that could be attributed to either common history (phylogenetic load) or adaptation. We further advance in this approach by comparing the phylogenetic load between different components of anuran calls. Because stream noise allegedly represents a stronger selective pressure for call frequency than for call temporal traits, we predicted a weaker phylogenetic signal in call frequency, particularly in the taxa that breed alongside streams. We first built a phylogenetic hypothesis using four mitochondrial genes on each of three clades: the genus <i>Hyloscirtus</i> and the family Centrolenidae, which call alongside streams and the subfamily Phyllomedusinae, known to call at lentic water bodies. In parallel, we compiled data on the advertisement calls of 154 species and used them to calculate Blomberg's <i>K</i> values as a proxy for the phylogenetic load (signal) of the call traits. The phylogenetic signal was weaker in spectral than in temporal call traits within the <i>Hyloscirtus</i> calls and weak or absent in both spectral and temporal components of Phyllomedusine calls. Against our expectations, the phylogenetic signal was strong in call frequency, but absent in call temporal components of the centrolenid calls. Our results support uncoupled evolution between spectral and temporal components of anuran calls. They also indicate that the selective role of abiotic noise varies among taxa and that other factors must be invoked to fully understand among-species variation in advertisement calls.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142447522","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality may modulate learning and memory differences in two taxa of the African striped mouse genus Rhabdomys 性格可能调节非洲条纹鼠属两个类群的学习和记忆差异
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13509
Candice N. Neves, Neville Pillay

Different environments place different cognitive demands on constituent taxa. Learning and memory involve cognitive processes with associated costs, and it is expected that different levels of learning will occur in taxa from different environments. Greater memory loads linked to increased environmental complexity require greater learning and memory capacities. We investigated the variation in learning and memory in sister taxa of striped mice (genus Rhabdomys). We studied two populations each of the mesic grassland-occurring R. d. chakae and the mostly arid-occurring R. pumilio. We conducted two sets of experiments. (1) In a novel object recognition (NOR) test, we assessed memory by recording the duration of exploration of similar and novel objects by test mice. (2) In an associative learning task, we assessed whether mice could associate specific scents with or without a food incentive or with different quantities of the food incentive in previous training phases. We measured the latency of mice to contact scents in a two-sample choice in the test phase. In the NOR test, R. pumilio spent less time investigating similar objects in a training trial than R. d. chakae but increased absolute exploration of the novel object when presented with a novel and a familiar object in the retention trial, suggesting a sensitization to the novel object by R. pumilio. In the associative learning experiments, R. pumilio approached the stimuli faster than R. d. chakae, whereas mice from both taxa preferred scents associated with a seed versus no seed and scents associated with 5 seeds versus 1 seed. The data provide evidence of taxon-level differences in learning and memory, likely related to environmentally modulated personality differences between the taxa.

不同的环境对组成类群的生物提出了不同的认知要求。学习和记忆的认知过程需要付出相应的代价,因此不同环境中的类群需要不同程度的学习。与环境复杂性增加有关的更大记忆负荷需要更强的学习和记忆能力。我们研究了条纹鼠(Rhabdomys 属)姊妹类群在学习和记忆方面的变异。我们研究了中生代草原条纹鼠(R. d. chakae)和干旱地区条纹鼠(R. pumilio)的两个种群。我们进行了两组实验。(1)在新物体识别(NOR)测试中,我们通过记录受试小鼠探索相似和新物体的持续时间来评估记忆力。(2)在联想学习任务中,我们评估了小鼠是否能将特定气味与之前训练阶段的食物诱因或不同数量的食物诱因联系起来。在测试阶段,我们测量了小鼠在双样本选择中接触气味的潜伏期。在NOR测试中,与R. d. chakae相比,R. pumilio在训练试验中花在研究相似物体上的时间较少,但在保留试验中,当出现一个新物体和一个熟悉物体时,R. pumilio对新物体的绝对研究时间增加,这表明R. pumilio对新物体敏感。在联想学习实验中,R. pumilio 比 R. d. chakae 接近刺激物的速度更快,而这两个类群的小鼠都更喜欢与种子相关的气味而不是没有种子的气味,以及与 5 粒种子相关的气味而不是 1 粒种子的气味。这些数据提供了分类群学习和记忆水平差异的证据,很可能与分类群之间受环境调节的个性差异有关。
{"title":"Personality may modulate learning and memory differences in two taxa of the African striped mouse genus Rhabdomys","authors":"Candice N. Neves,&nbsp;Neville Pillay","doi":"10.1111/eth.13509","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13509","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Different environments place different cognitive demands on constituent taxa. Learning and memory involve cognitive processes with associated costs, and it is expected that different levels of learning will occur in taxa from different environments. Greater memory loads linked to increased environmental complexity require greater learning and memory capacities. We investigated the variation in learning and memory in sister taxa of striped mice (genus <i>Rhabdomys</i>). We studied two populations each of the mesic grassland-occurring <i>R. d. chakae</i> and the mostly arid-occurring <i>R. pumilio.</i> We conducted two sets of experiments. (1) In a novel object recognition (NOR) test, we assessed memory by recording the duration of exploration of similar and novel objects by test mice. (2) In an associative learning task, we assessed whether mice could associate specific scents with or without a food incentive or with different quantities of the food incentive in previous training phases. We measured the latency of mice to contact scents in a two-sample choice in the test phase. In the NOR test, <i>R. pumilio</i> spent less time investigating similar objects in a training trial than <i>R. d. chakae</i> but increased absolute exploration of the novel object when presented with a novel and a familiar object in the retention trial, suggesting a sensitization to the novel object by <i>R. pumilio</i>. In the associative learning experiments, <i>R. pumilio</i> approached the stimuli faster than <i>R. d. chakae</i>, whereas mice from both taxa preferred scents associated with a seed versus no seed and scents associated with 5 seeds versus 1 seed. The data provide evidence of taxon-level differences in learning and memory, likely related to environmentally modulated personality differences between the taxa.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13509","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142449228","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Male and female age affects the reproductive potential of two tephritid flies 雄蝇和雌蝇的年龄影响两种表蝇的生殖潜力
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13506
Viviana Díaz, Antonella Giudice, Alfonsina Palladini, Andrea Moyano, Gisela Castillo, Diana Pérez-Staples, Solana Abraham

In insects, aging produces deterioration in physiological and cellular functioning, affecting their reproductive potential. Anastrepha fraterculus and Ceratitis capitata are two fruit fly species where overwintering adults resume their reproductive activity in spring, giving old individuals the possibility of mating with young adults. Most age studies focus on male reproductive capacity; however, we lack information on how the interaction between the ages of both sexes can determine post-mating processes. Here, we studied sex and age effects on (i) female fecundity and fertility, (ii) failure to leave viable offspring (reproductive failure), and (iii) female remating behavior. We found that young pairs of both species had higher fecundity, but young C. capitata males mated with old females had the lowest fecundity. This suggests that overwintering flies in this species will not substantially contribute to the next generation. We also found in C. capitata more prevalent reproductive failure in hetero-age combinations, which could be due to age recognition between mates, resulting perhaps in differential ejaculate allocation. Copula duration was positively associated with female age, yet it was longer for older A. fraterculus females and shorter for C. capitata females. Female remating was lower when young females mated with old males in C. capitata. This would imply that males perceive young females of “good quality” and thus invest and transfer all the ejaculate possible to ensure the delay of renewal of female receptivity. Aging does not always cause a decline in reproductive potential, which may be important in species that overwinter as adults. Complex interactions between female physiology and male ejaculate senescence can impact postcopulatory behaviors that affect reproductive success for both sexes.

在昆虫中,衰老会导致生理和细胞功能退化,影响其繁殖潜力。Anastrepha fraterculus 和 Ceratitis capitata 是两个果蝇物种,其越冬成虫在春季恢复生殖活动,使老年个体有可能与年轻成虫交配。大多数年龄研究集中于雄性的生殖能力;然而,我们缺乏有关两性年龄之间的相互作用如何决定交配后过程的信息。在这里,我们研究了性别和年龄对(i)雌性繁殖力和生育能力、(ii)未能留下可存活后代(繁殖失败)和(iii)雌性再交配行为的影响。我们发现,两种苍蝇的年轻配对繁殖力都较高,但与老龄雌蝇交配的年轻雄蝇繁殖力最低。这表明该物种的越冬蝇对下一代的贡献不大。我们还发现,在异龄组合中,C. capitata的繁殖失败率更高,这可能是由于配偶之间的年龄识别,从而导致射精分配的差异。交配持续时间与雌性年龄呈正相关,但年龄较大的A. fraterculus雌性交配持续时间较长,而C. capitata雌性交配持续时间较短。C.capitata的年轻雌性与年老雄性交配时,雌性的再交配率较低。这意味着雄性认为年轻雌性 "质量好",因此会尽可能投入和转移所有射精,以确保推迟雌性接受能力的恢复。衰老并不总是导致繁殖潜力下降,这对于以成体越冬的物种可能很重要。雌性生理机能和雄性射精衰老之间复杂的相互作用会影响交配后的行为,从而影响雌雄的繁殖成功率。
{"title":"Male and female age affects the reproductive potential of two tephritid flies","authors":"Viviana Díaz,&nbsp;Antonella Giudice,&nbsp;Alfonsina Palladini,&nbsp;Andrea Moyano,&nbsp;Gisela Castillo,&nbsp;Diana Pérez-Staples,&nbsp;Solana Abraham","doi":"10.1111/eth.13506","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13506","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In insects, aging produces deterioration in physiological and cellular functioning, affecting their reproductive potential. <i>Anastrepha fraterculus</i> and <i>Ceratitis capitata</i> are two fruit fly species where overwintering adults resume their reproductive activity in spring, giving old individuals the possibility of mating with young adults. Most age studies focus on male reproductive capacity; however, we lack information on how the interaction between the ages of both sexes can determine post-mating processes. Here, we studied sex and age effects on (i) female fecundity and fertility, (ii) failure to leave viable offspring (reproductive failure), and (iii) female remating behavior. We found that young pairs of both species had higher fecundity, but young <i>C. capitata</i> males mated with old females had the lowest fecundity. This suggests that overwintering flies in this species will not substantially contribute to the next generation. We also found in <i>C. capitata</i> more prevalent reproductive failure in hetero-age combinations, which could be due to age recognition between mates, resulting perhaps in differential ejaculate allocation. Copula duration was positively associated with female age, yet it was longer for older <i>A. fraterculus</i> females and shorter for <i>C. capitata</i> females. Female remating was lower when young females mated with old males in <i>C. capitata</i>. This would imply that males perceive young females of “good quality” and thus invest and transfer all the ejaculate possible to ensure the delay of renewal of female receptivity. Aging does not always cause a decline in reproductive potential, which may be important in species that overwinter as adults. Complex interactions between female physiology and male ejaculate senescence can impact postcopulatory behaviors that affect reproductive success for both sexes.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142184415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1