Free Improvisation, Egalitarianism, and Knowledge

Q3 Arts and Humanities Jazz Perspectives Pub Date : 2023-09-13 DOI:10.1080/17494060.2023.2249411
Ritwik Banerji
{"title":"Free Improvisation, Egalitarianism, and Knowledge","authors":"Ritwik Banerji","doi":"10.1080/17494060.2023.2249411","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACTWhile numerous scholars and performers of free improvisation have noted that egalitarianism structures social interactions between participants of this musical practice, they have largely treated this concept as self-explanatory. Drawing on ethnographic participant observation I conducted in Berlin, Chicago, and San Francisco between 2008 and 2016, this article examines how egalitarianism generates several patterns of social interaction in habits of talk, embodied behavior, and musical sound. Conceptualizing egalitarianism as freedom from social and aesthetic hierarchy, I argue that participants of these scenes pursue egalitarianism through an array of behaviors that prevent other individuals from knowing what they prefer, intend, understand, and value in the practice of free improvisation. Preventing others from accessing these kinds of knowledge places each participant on an equal plane of unawareness which in turn allows them to experience creative freedom. More broadly, then, this article outlines how free improvisation is based in a concept of freedom in which the negation, rather than the accrual, of knowledge leads to greater experiences of personal liberty.KEYWORDS: improvisationfreedomegalitarianismknowledge Disclosure StatementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 Jason Stanyek, “Articulating Intercultural Free Improvisation: Evan Parker’s Synergetics Project,” Resonance 7, no. 2 (1999): 44–7; David W. Bernstein, “‘Listening to the Sounds of the People’: Frederic Rzewski and Musica Elettronica Viva (1966–1972),” Contemporary Music Review 29, no. 6 (2010): 535–50; Barbara Rose Lange, “Teaching the Ethics of Free Improvisation,” Critical Studies in Improvisation/Études Critiques en Improvisation 7, no. 2 (2011): 1–11; Maud Hickey, “Learning from the Experts: A Study of Free-Improvisation Pedagogues in University Settings,” Journal of Research in Music Education 62, no. 4 (2015): 425–45.2 Burkhard Beins et al., eds., Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / self-defining a scene (Hofheim: Wolke Verlag, 2011).3 David Borgo, “Synergy and Surrealestate: The Orderly Disorder of Free Improvisation,” Pacific Review of Ethnomusicology 10, no. 1 (2002): 1–24.4 See Christopher Boehm, “Egalitarian Behavior and Reverse Dominance Hierarchy,” Current Anthropology 34, no. 3 (1993): 227–54.5 Lange, “Teaching the Ethics of Free Improvisation”; Hickey, “Free-Improvisation Pedagogues.”6 Tom Arthurs, “Improvised Music in Berlin 2012–13: A Brief Ethnographic Portrait,” Critical Studies in Improvisation/Études Critiques en Improvisation 10, no. 2 (2015): n. 57.7 In terms of dates, I conducted fieldwork in Berlin during May of 2010, summer of 2012, and from fall of 2014 to summer 2016; Chicago from spring 2008 to summer 2010; and the San Francisco Bay Area free improvisation scene from fall 2010 to summer 2014.8 Dana Gooley, Fantasies of Improvisation: Free Playing in Nineteenth-Century Music (New York: Oxford University Press, 2018).9 Melvin James Backstrom, “The Field of Cultural Production and the Limits of Freedom in Improvisation,” Critical Studies in Improvisation/Études Critiques en Improvisation 9, no. 1 (2013).10 See Valerie Wilmer, As Serious as Your Life: The Story of the New Jazz (Westport, CT: Lawrence Hill, 1980); George E. Lewis, “Improvised Music After 1950: Afrological and Eurological Perspectives,” Black Music Research Journal 16, no. 1 (1996): 114–15.11 Performers active in this practice often choose objects that have neither been designed to function as musical instruments (or even to produce sound, for that matter) nor historically used as such. For example, Japanese noise artist Toshimaru Nakamura has developed the practice of “no-input mixing board,” in which the output of the device is routed directly back into any of its inputs. Similarly, the white Czech improviser Ivan Palacký performs on a sewing machine and white American improviser Judy Dunaway often performs using balloons.12 This kind of social practice is indeed an element of the work of the Association for the Advancement of Creative Musicians, the Black Artists Group, and the New York loft jazz scene. Nevertheless, each of these three musical communities regularly features forms of musical organization that greatly differ from what I describe in this article, to say nothing of their racial demographics, which are certainly contrast quite strongly from the scenes I analyze here.13 John Corbett, A Listener's Guide to Free Improvisation (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 2016), 1.14 Julie Dawn Smith, “Playing Like a Girl: The Queer Laughter of the Feminist Improvising Group,” in The Other Side of Nowhere: Jazz, Improvisation, and Communities in Dialogue, ed. Daniel Fischlin and Ajay Heble (Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press, 2004); Marc Hannaford, “Subjective (Re)positioning in Musical Improvisation: Analyzing the Work of Five Female Improvisers,” Music Theory Online 23, no. 2 (2017); Hannah Reardon-Smith, “The Uncanon: Radical Forgetting and Free Improvisation,” Sound Scripts 6, no. 1 (2019): 1–8.15 Ritwik Banerji, \"Whiteness as Improvisation, Nonwhiteness as Machine,\" Jazz and Culture 4, no. 2 (2021): 56–84.16 For a history of this idea, see Lewis, “Improvised Music After 1950.”17 For an elaboration on the notion of leveling, see Boehm, “Egalitarian Behavior.”18 For further elaboration on this sense of the term ritual, see Rupert Stasch, “Ritual and Oratory Revisited: The Semiotics of Effective Action,” Annual Review of Anthropology 40 (2011): 159–74.19 Pseudonyms have been used for each venue and ethnographic interlocutor.20 Andre Béteille, Inequality among Men, (Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell, 1977).21 As a creative, playful examination of this egalitarian stance, performance artist Diego Chamy organized a “competition” among improvising duos at a music festival in Berlin in 2009 for a cash prize. Like Carl’s reaction, it provoked a sense of disgust among audience members, who took offense at the very idea of trying to determine the “best” of the duos featured; see Diego Chamy, “Das Interaktion Festival: Eine kritische Verteidigung / The Interaktion Festival: A Critical Defense,” in Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / Self-defining a scene, ed. Burkhard Beins et al. (Hofheim: Wolke Verlag, 2011), 298–316.22 Even when speaking German, it was common for improvisers I worked with to use the term “session” as a loanword.23 In a German context, this person’s specific national identity may be less consequential than their race, which would likely be taken as East Asian.24 Here, I use “genre” in the linguistic anthropological sense of a particular form of social interaction rather than in the more common humanistic sense of a type of artistic work; see Richard Bauman, “Genre,” Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 9, no. 1/2 (1999): 84–7.25 For whatever reason, this was the precise phrase I heard improvisers use on numerous occasions when asking this question.26 This corresponds with what ethnographers of communication have observed in other egalitarian communities, where communicative ambiguity appears to similarly function as a leveling tactic; see Donald Brenneis, “Talk and Transformation,” Man 22, no. 3 (1987): 499–510; Brackette Williams, \"Humor, Linguistic Ambiguity, and Disputing in a Guyanese Community,\" International Journal of the Sociology of Language 1987, no. 65 (1987): 79–94.27 This is a type of high-occupancy apartment building constructed from large, mass-produced concrete slabs.28 This particular comment may seem charged with meaning and yet its speaker introduces it without clarifying what it refers to. Similarly, none of the participants of this interaction inquire as to what the comment means, thereby preserving a sense of mystery about its meaning, a feature of such conversations which is consistent with the general tendency to leave various intentional stances unknown.29 As mentioned previously, given that the three scenes I discuss here are overwhelmingly white, this also means that the potential pool of session invitees or performance collaborators is as well.30 As unsettling as this seems, improvisers often greatly value this unpredictability; see Burkhard Beins, “Entwurf und Ereignis / Scheme and Event,” in Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / Self-defining a scene, ed. Burkhard Beins et al. (Hofheim, Germany: Wolke Verlag, 2011), 166–81.31 This is a functional term to describe the intentional abstention from the production of sound. Realistically, an actual absence of sound is not possible; see John Cage, Silence: Lectures and Writings (Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press, 1961); Lorraine Plourde, “Disciplined Listening in Tokyo: Onkyō and Non-Intentional Sounds,” Ethnomusicology 52, no. 2 (2008): 270–95. Unless otherwise noted, my use of this term refers to intentional abstention from sound production.32 Inharmonicity and noise refer to sounds which lack a definite pitch or obvious bands of acoustic energy emanating from sources vibrating at a definite frequency, respectively; see Denis Smalley, “Spectromorphology: Explaining Sound-Shapes,” Organised Sound 2, no. 2 (1997): 107–26.33 As scholars of free improvisation now readily note, this aesthetic tendency constitutes the basic sonic parameters that define free improvisation as a genre; see Chris Atton, “Genre and the Cultural Politics of Territory: The Live Experience of Free Improvisation,” European Journal of Cultural Studies 15, no. 4 (2012): 427–41; Backstrom, “Limits of Freedom.” That is, rather than transcending the boundaries of musical genre (as is often claimed about this practice), free improvisation exhibits generic traits just like any other contemporary genre.34 Critics of egalitarianism refer to this as “leveling down,” or an approach to egalitarianism based in the idea of stripping privileges to place all on an equal plane; see Deborah L. Brake, “When Equality Leaves Everyone Worse Off: The Problem of Leveling Down in Equality Law,” William Mary Law Review 46 (2004): 513–618.35 In Isaiah Berlin’s terms, this particular dimension of freedom in this musical practice, in which performers are not expected to demonstrate certain kinds of conventional competencies, constitutes a “negative” (freedom from) rather than positive (freedom to); see “Two Concepts of Liberty,” in Liberty, ed. Henry Hardy (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002), 166–217.36 Strictly speaking, however, the habit of gaze aversion means that all this stoicism is primarily oriented towards onlookers since fellow players are unlikely to see these muted facial expressions anyway. All the same, this still amounts to a suspension of critical judgment as the audience does not see players visibly evaluate their fellow performers as their facial expressions change during the performance.37 Sofia Dahl et al., “Gestures in Performance,” in Musical Gestures: Sound, Movement, and Meaning, ed. Rolf Inge Godøy and Marc Leman (New York: Routledge, 2010), 36–68.38 Joel Robbins and Alan Rumsey, “Introduction: Cultural and Linguistic Anthropology and the Opacity of Other Minds,” Anthropological Quarterly 81, no. 2 (2008): 407–20. In many ways, the linguistic anthropological concept of an opacity doctrine shares much in common with other theories of opacity, especially in the work of Edóuard Glissant; see Poetics of Relation, trans. Betsy Wing (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1997). While Glissant’s discussion is a productive illustration of the sensory apparatus that undergirds political autonomy, it is distinct from (and as yet unconnected with) linguistic anthropological discussions of opacity, which focus on how particular speech communities develop ideologies about what one can or cannot know about other minds.39 Graeme B. Wilson and Raymond A.R. MacDonald, “The Sign of Silence: Negotiating Musical Identities in an Improvising Ensemble,” Psychology of Music 40, no. 5 (2012): 558–73.40 Graeme B. Wilson and Raymond A.R. MacDonald, “Musical Choices During Group Free Improvisation: A Qualitative Psychological Investigation,” Psychology of Music 44, no. 5 (2016): 1029–43.41 Rupert Stasch, “Knowing Minds is a Matter of Authority: Political Dimensions of Opacity Statements in Korowai Moral Psychology,” Anthropological Quarterly 81, no. 2 (2008): 443–53.42 Clément Canonne and Nicolas Garnier, “Individual Decisions and Perceived Form in Collective Free Improvisation,” Journal of New Music Research 44, no. 2 (2015): 145–67.43 These are small canals that divert the flow of the River Spree through the city.44 The unspecific quality of this praise may partially be due to the inherently disorienting effect of performing nearly an hour’s worth of pulseless music without a predetermined composition, using cumbersome, unpredictable extended techniques or instrumental preparations to manipulate sounds, all while attempting to listen to oneself and others as one composes and performs simultaneously. The disorienting nature of these experiences has been described by numerous players and other observers see John Corbett, Extended Play: Sounding Off from John Cage to Dr. Funkenstein (Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 1994), 201–08; Beins, “Entwurf und Ereignis.” If the stylistic features of free improvisation themselves are a product of an egalitarian commitment, these effectively induce an egalitarian stance on abstaining from peer criticism because they leave improvisers unable to really recall what took place over the course of the piece itself. Additionally, while players often make personal recordings of their sessions and performances, I rarely observed improvisers review them as a group for the purpose of recollection and self-evaluation, a behavior very much consistent with Derek Bailey’s overall skepticism of recordings as an authoritative, objective document of the events of an improvised performance; see Derek Bailey, Improvisation: Its Nature and Practice in Music (New York: Da Capo Press, 1993).45 See Timothy Rice, “Understanding and Producing the Variability of Oral Tradition: Learning from a Bulgarian Bagpiper,” The Journal of American Folklore 108, no. 429 (1995): 266–76.46 Ben Watson, Derek Bailey and the Story of Free Improvisation (New York: Verso, 2004), 78.47 Kwami Coleman, “Free Jazz and the ‘New Thing’ Aesthetics, Identity, and Texture, 1960–1966,” The Journal of Musicology 38, no. 3 (2021): 261–95.48 Corbett, Guide to Free Improvisation, 1.Additional informationFundingThis work was supported by Fulbright Germany and the Berlin Program for Advanced German and European Studies.","PeriodicalId":39826,"journal":{"name":"Jazz Perspectives","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2023-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Jazz Perspectives","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17494060.2023.2249411","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"Arts and Humanities","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

ABSTRACTWhile numerous scholars and performers of free improvisation have noted that egalitarianism structures social interactions between participants of this musical practice, they have largely treated this concept as self-explanatory. Drawing on ethnographic participant observation I conducted in Berlin, Chicago, and San Francisco between 2008 and 2016, this article examines how egalitarianism generates several patterns of social interaction in habits of talk, embodied behavior, and musical sound. Conceptualizing egalitarianism as freedom from social and aesthetic hierarchy, I argue that participants of these scenes pursue egalitarianism through an array of behaviors that prevent other individuals from knowing what they prefer, intend, understand, and value in the practice of free improvisation. Preventing others from accessing these kinds of knowledge places each participant on an equal plane of unawareness which in turn allows them to experience creative freedom. More broadly, then, this article outlines how free improvisation is based in a concept of freedom in which the negation, rather than the accrual, of knowledge leads to greater experiences of personal liberty.KEYWORDS: improvisationfreedomegalitarianismknowledge Disclosure StatementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 Jason Stanyek, “Articulating Intercultural Free Improvisation: Evan Parker’s Synergetics Project,” Resonance 7, no. 2 (1999): 44–7; David W. Bernstein, “‘Listening to the Sounds of the People’: Frederic Rzewski and Musica Elettronica Viva (1966–1972),” Contemporary Music Review 29, no. 6 (2010): 535–50; Barbara Rose Lange, “Teaching the Ethics of Free Improvisation,” Critical Studies in Improvisation/Études Critiques en Improvisation 7, no. 2 (2011): 1–11; Maud Hickey, “Learning from the Experts: A Study of Free-Improvisation Pedagogues in University Settings,” Journal of Research in Music Education 62, no. 4 (2015): 425–45.2 Burkhard Beins et al., eds., Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / self-defining a scene (Hofheim: Wolke Verlag, 2011).3 David Borgo, “Synergy and Surrealestate: The Orderly Disorder of Free Improvisation,” Pacific Review of Ethnomusicology 10, no. 1 (2002): 1–24.4 See Christopher Boehm, “Egalitarian Behavior and Reverse Dominance Hierarchy,” Current Anthropology 34, no. 3 (1993): 227–54.5 Lange, “Teaching the Ethics of Free Improvisation”; Hickey, “Free-Improvisation Pedagogues.”6 Tom Arthurs, “Improvised Music in Berlin 2012–13: A Brief Ethnographic Portrait,” Critical Studies in Improvisation/Études Critiques en Improvisation 10, no. 2 (2015): n. 57.7 In terms of dates, I conducted fieldwork in Berlin during May of 2010, summer of 2012, and from fall of 2014 to summer 2016; Chicago from spring 2008 to summer 2010; and the San Francisco Bay Area free improvisation scene from fall 2010 to summer 2014.8 Dana Gooley, Fantasies of Improvisation: Free Playing in Nineteenth-Century Music (New York: Oxford University Press, 2018).9 Melvin James Backstrom, “The Field of Cultural Production and the Limits of Freedom in Improvisation,” Critical Studies in Improvisation/Études Critiques en Improvisation 9, no. 1 (2013).10 See Valerie Wilmer, As Serious as Your Life: The Story of the New Jazz (Westport, CT: Lawrence Hill, 1980); George E. Lewis, “Improvised Music After 1950: Afrological and Eurological Perspectives,” Black Music Research Journal 16, no. 1 (1996): 114–15.11 Performers active in this practice often choose objects that have neither been designed to function as musical instruments (or even to produce sound, for that matter) nor historically used as such. For example, Japanese noise artist Toshimaru Nakamura has developed the practice of “no-input mixing board,” in which the output of the device is routed directly back into any of its inputs. Similarly, the white Czech improviser Ivan Palacký performs on a sewing machine and white American improviser Judy Dunaway often performs using balloons.12 This kind of social practice is indeed an element of the work of the Association for the Advancement of Creative Musicians, the Black Artists Group, and the New York loft jazz scene. Nevertheless, each of these three musical communities regularly features forms of musical organization that greatly differ from what I describe in this article, to say nothing of their racial demographics, which are certainly contrast quite strongly from the scenes I analyze here.13 John Corbett, A Listener's Guide to Free Improvisation (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 2016), 1.14 Julie Dawn Smith, “Playing Like a Girl: The Queer Laughter of the Feminist Improvising Group,” in The Other Side of Nowhere: Jazz, Improvisation, and Communities in Dialogue, ed. Daniel Fischlin and Ajay Heble (Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press, 2004); Marc Hannaford, “Subjective (Re)positioning in Musical Improvisation: Analyzing the Work of Five Female Improvisers,” Music Theory Online 23, no. 2 (2017); Hannah Reardon-Smith, “The Uncanon: Radical Forgetting and Free Improvisation,” Sound Scripts 6, no. 1 (2019): 1–8.15 Ritwik Banerji, "Whiteness as Improvisation, Nonwhiteness as Machine," Jazz and Culture 4, no. 2 (2021): 56–84.16 For a history of this idea, see Lewis, “Improvised Music After 1950.”17 For an elaboration on the notion of leveling, see Boehm, “Egalitarian Behavior.”18 For further elaboration on this sense of the term ritual, see Rupert Stasch, “Ritual and Oratory Revisited: The Semiotics of Effective Action,” Annual Review of Anthropology 40 (2011): 159–74.19 Pseudonyms have been used for each venue and ethnographic interlocutor.20 Andre Béteille, Inequality among Men, (Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell, 1977).21 As a creative, playful examination of this egalitarian stance, performance artist Diego Chamy organized a “competition” among improvising duos at a music festival in Berlin in 2009 for a cash prize. Like Carl’s reaction, it provoked a sense of disgust among audience members, who took offense at the very idea of trying to determine the “best” of the duos featured; see Diego Chamy, “Das Interaktion Festival: Eine kritische Verteidigung / The Interaktion Festival: A Critical Defense,” in Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / Self-defining a scene, ed. Burkhard Beins et al. (Hofheim: Wolke Verlag, 2011), 298–316.22 Even when speaking German, it was common for improvisers I worked with to use the term “session” as a loanword.23 In a German context, this person’s specific national identity may be less consequential than their race, which would likely be taken as East Asian.24 Here, I use “genre” in the linguistic anthropological sense of a particular form of social interaction rather than in the more common humanistic sense of a type of artistic work; see Richard Bauman, “Genre,” Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 9, no. 1/2 (1999): 84–7.25 For whatever reason, this was the precise phrase I heard improvisers use on numerous occasions when asking this question.26 This corresponds with what ethnographers of communication have observed in other egalitarian communities, where communicative ambiguity appears to similarly function as a leveling tactic; see Donald Brenneis, “Talk and Transformation,” Man 22, no. 3 (1987): 499–510; Brackette Williams, "Humor, Linguistic Ambiguity, and Disputing in a Guyanese Community," International Journal of the Sociology of Language 1987, no. 65 (1987): 79–94.27 This is a type of high-occupancy apartment building constructed from large, mass-produced concrete slabs.28 This particular comment may seem charged with meaning and yet its speaker introduces it without clarifying what it refers to. Similarly, none of the participants of this interaction inquire as to what the comment means, thereby preserving a sense of mystery about its meaning, a feature of such conversations which is consistent with the general tendency to leave various intentional stances unknown.29 As mentioned previously, given that the three scenes I discuss here are overwhelmingly white, this also means that the potential pool of session invitees or performance collaborators is as well.30 As unsettling as this seems, improvisers often greatly value this unpredictability; see Burkhard Beins, “Entwurf und Ereignis / Scheme and Event,” in Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / Self-defining a scene, ed. Burkhard Beins et al. (Hofheim, Germany: Wolke Verlag, 2011), 166–81.31 This is a functional term to describe the intentional abstention from the production of sound. Realistically, an actual absence of sound is not possible; see John Cage, Silence: Lectures and Writings (Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press, 1961); Lorraine Plourde, “Disciplined Listening in Tokyo: Onkyō and Non-Intentional Sounds,” Ethnomusicology 52, no. 2 (2008): 270–95. Unless otherwise noted, my use of this term refers to intentional abstention from sound production.32 Inharmonicity and noise refer to sounds which lack a definite pitch or obvious bands of acoustic energy emanating from sources vibrating at a definite frequency, respectively; see Denis Smalley, “Spectromorphology: Explaining Sound-Shapes,” Organised Sound 2, no. 2 (1997): 107–26.33 As scholars of free improvisation now readily note, this aesthetic tendency constitutes the basic sonic parameters that define free improvisation as a genre; see Chris Atton, “Genre and the Cultural Politics of Territory: The Live Experience of Free Improvisation,” European Journal of Cultural Studies 15, no. 4 (2012): 427–41; Backstrom, “Limits of Freedom.” That is, rather than transcending the boundaries of musical genre (as is often claimed about this practice), free improvisation exhibits generic traits just like any other contemporary genre.34 Critics of egalitarianism refer to this as “leveling down,” or an approach to egalitarianism based in the idea of stripping privileges to place all on an equal plane; see Deborah L. Brake, “When Equality Leaves Everyone Worse Off: The Problem of Leveling Down in Equality Law,” William Mary Law Review 46 (2004): 513–618.35 In Isaiah Berlin’s terms, this particular dimension of freedom in this musical practice, in which performers are not expected to demonstrate certain kinds of conventional competencies, constitutes a “negative” (freedom from) rather than positive (freedom to); see “Two Concepts of Liberty,” in Liberty, ed. Henry Hardy (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002), 166–217.36 Strictly speaking, however, the habit of gaze aversion means that all this stoicism is primarily oriented towards onlookers since fellow players are unlikely to see these muted facial expressions anyway. All the same, this still amounts to a suspension of critical judgment as the audience does not see players visibly evaluate their fellow performers as their facial expressions change during the performance.37 Sofia Dahl et al., “Gestures in Performance,” in Musical Gestures: Sound, Movement, and Meaning, ed. Rolf Inge Godøy and Marc Leman (New York: Routledge, 2010), 36–68.38 Joel Robbins and Alan Rumsey, “Introduction: Cultural and Linguistic Anthropology and the Opacity of Other Minds,” Anthropological Quarterly 81, no. 2 (2008): 407–20. In many ways, the linguistic anthropological concept of an opacity doctrine shares much in common with other theories of opacity, especially in the work of Edóuard Glissant; see Poetics of Relation, trans. Betsy Wing (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1997). While Glissant’s discussion is a productive illustration of the sensory apparatus that undergirds political autonomy, it is distinct from (and as yet unconnected with) linguistic anthropological discussions of opacity, which focus on how particular speech communities develop ideologies about what one can or cannot know about other minds.39 Graeme B. Wilson and Raymond A.R. MacDonald, “The Sign of Silence: Negotiating Musical Identities in an Improvising Ensemble,” Psychology of Music 40, no. 5 (2012): 558–73.40 Graeme B. Wilson and Raymond A.R. MacDonald, “Musical Choices During Group Free Improvisation: A Qualitative Psychological Investigation,” Psychology of Music 44, no. 5 (2016): 1029–43.41 Rupert Stasch, “Knowing Minds is a Matter of Authority: Political Dimensions of Opacity Statements in Korowai Moral Psychology,” Anthropological Quarterly 81, no. 2 (2008): 443–53.42 Clément Canonne and Nicolas Garnier, “Individual Decisions and Perceived Form in Collective Free Improvisation,” Journal of New Music Research 44, no. 2 (2015): 145–67.43 These are small canals that divert the flow of the River Spree through the city.44 The unspecific quality of this praise may partially be due to the inherently disorienting effect of performing nearly an hour’s worth of pulseless music without a predetermined composition, using cumbersome, unpredictable extended techniques or instrumental preparations to manipulate sounds, all while attempting to listen to oneself and others as one composes and performs simultaneously. The disorienting nature of these experiences has been described by numerous players and other observers see John Corbett, Extended Play: Sounding Off from John Cage to Dr. Funkenstein (Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 1994), 201–08; Beins, “Entwurf und Ereignis.” If the stylistic features of free improvisation themselves are a product of an egalitarian commitment, these effectively induce an egalitarian stance on abstaining from peer criticism because they leave improvisers unable to really recall what took place over the course of the piece itself. Additionally, while players often make personal recordings of their sessions and performances, I rarely observed improvisers review them as a group for the purpose of recollection and self-evaluation, a behavior very much consistent with Derek Bailey’s overall skepticism of recordings as an authoritative, objective document of the events of an improvised performance; see Derek Bailey, Improvisation: Its Nature and Practice in Music (New York: Da Capo Press, 1993).45 See Timothy Rice, “Understanding and Producing the Variability of Oral Tradition: Learning from a Bulgarian Bagpiper,” The Journal of American Folklore 108, no. 429 (1995): 266–76.46 Ben Watson, Derek Bailey and the Story of Free Improvisation (New York: Verso, 2004), 78.47 Kwami Coleman, “Free Jazz and the ‘New Thing’ Aesthetics, Identity, and Texture, 1960–1966,” The Journal of Musicology 38, no. 3 (2021): 261–95.48 Corbett, Guide to Free Improvisation, 1.Additional informationFundingThis work was supported by Fulbright Germany and the Berlin Program for Advanced German and European Studies.
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
自由即兴,平等主义和知识
Daniel Fischlin和Ajay Heble (Middletown, CT:卫斯理大学出版社,2004);Marc Hannaford,“音乐即兴中的主观(再)定位:五位女性即兴者的作品分析”,《乐理在线》第23期。2 (2017);汉娜·里尔登-史密斯,《非正典:彻底的遗忘与自由即兴》,《声音剧本》第6期,第2号。Ritwik Banerji,“白人作为即兴创作,非白人作为机器”,《爵士与文化》第4期,第1 - 8.15期。2(2021): 56-84.16关于这一想法的历史,参见Lewis,“1950年后的即兴音乐”。17关于平衡概念的详细说明,见Boehm,“平等主义行为”。18关于“仪式”一词的这种意义的进一步阐述,见Rupert Stasch,“重新审视仪式和演讲:有效行动的符号学”,《人类学年度评论》40 (2011):159-74.1921 . Andre bacimetille,《男性的不平等》(Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell, 1977)作为对这种平等主义立场的一种创造性的、有趣的审视,行为艺术家迭戈·查米(Diego Chamy)在2009年柏林的一个音乐节上组织了一场即兴表演二人组的“比赛”,以获得现金奖。就像卡尔的反应一样,这引起了观众的反感,他们对试图确定“最好”的二人组的想法感到反感;见Diego Chamy,“Das Interaktion Festival: Eine kritische Verteidigung / The Interaktion Festival: A Critical Defense”,见Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / Self-defining A scene,编者,Burkhard Beins等人(Hofheim: Wolke Verlag, 2011), 298-316.22。即使在说德语时,与我一起工作的即兴表演者也经常使用“session”这个词作为外用词在德国的语境中,这个人的特定的民族身份可能不如他们的种族重要,后者很可能被视为东亚人。24在这里,我使用“流派”是在语言人类学意义上的一种特殊的社会互动形式,而不是在更普遍的人文主义意义上的一种艺术作品;参见Richard Bauman,“体裁”,《语言人类学杂志》第9期。[1/2][1999]: 84-7.25]不管出于什么原因,这正是我听到即兴表演者在许多场合问到这个问题时所用的短语这与研究交流的民族志学家在其他平等主义社区中观察到的情况相一致,在这些社区中,交流的模糊性似乎也起到了类似的平衡策略的作用;看唐纳德·布伦尼斯的《谈话与转变》,22岁的男人,不。3 (1987): 499-510;“幽默、语言歧义与圭亚那社区的争论”,《国际语言社会学杂志》1987年第1期。65(1987): 79-94.27这是一种由大量生产的混凝土板建造而成的高占用公寓楼这个特别的评论似乎充满了意义,但它的说话者在介绍它时没有澄清它指的是什么。同样,这种互动的参与者中没有人询问评论的意思,从而保留了对其含义的神秘感,这是这种对话的一个特征,与不知道各种故意立场的总体趋势是一致的如前所述,考虑到我在这里讨论的三个场景绝大多数是白人,这也意味着会议受邀者或表演合作者的潜在群体也是白人尽管这看起来令人不安,但即兴表演者往往非常重视这种不可预测性;见Burkhard Beins,“Entwurf und Ereignis / Scheme and Event”,见Echtzeitmusik berlin: selbstbestimmung einer szene / Self-defining a scene, ed. Burkhard Beins et al. (Hofheim, Germany: Wolke Verlag, 2011), 116 - 81.31。这是一个功能性术语,用来描述故意放弃声音的产生。实际上,没有声音是不可能的;参见约翰·凯奇,沉默:讲座和著作(Middletown, CT:卫斯理大学出版社,1961);Lorraine Plourde,“在东京有纪律的聆听:onkyki - and nonintentional Sounds”,民族音乐学52,no。2(2008): 270-95。除非另有说明,我使用这个词是指有意不做声音制作不谐音和噪声,分别是指从一定频率振动的声源发出的声音没有一定的音高或没有明显的声能带;参见Denis Smalley,“光谱形态学:解释声音形状”,《有组织的声音》2,第2期。2(1997): 107-26.33正如自由即兴演奏的学者们现在很容易注意到的,这种审美倾向构成了将自由即兴演奏定义为一种流派的基本声音参数;参见Chris Atton,“体裁与地域的文化政治:自由即兴创作的现场经验”,《欧洲文化研究杂志》第15期。4 (2012): 427-41;巴克斯卓,《自由的极限》 也就是说,自由即兴演奏并没有超越音乐类型的界限(就像人们经常声称的那样),而是像任何其他当代音乐类型一样,表现出普遍的特征平等主义的批评者将其称为“平等化”,或者是一种基于剥夺特权、将所有人置于平等地位的平等主义方法;参见Deborah L. Brake,“当平等让每个人都变得更糟:平等法中的平等化问题”,William Mary Law Review 46(2004): 513-618.35。以赛亚·伯林(Isaiah Berlin)的术语来说,在这种音乐实践中,表演者不被期望展示某些传统能力,这种特殊的自由维度构成了“消极”(从)而不是积极(到)的自由;参见Henry Hardy编辑的《自由的两个概念》(牛津:牛津大学出版社,2002年),第166-217.36页。然而,严格地说,厌恶凝视的习惯意味着所有这些坚忍主要是针对旁观者的,因为其他玩家无论如何都不可能看到这些沉默的面部表情。尽管如此,这仍然相当于暂停了批判性判断,因为观众看不到表演者在表演过程中面部表情的变化而明显地评价他们的同伴索非亚·达尔等人,“表演中的手势”,在音乐手势:声音,运动和意义,编辑。罗尔夫·英格·格德伊和马克·莱曼(纽约:劳特利奇,2010),36-68.38乔尔·罗宾斯和艾伦·拉姆齐,“介绍:文化和语言人类学和其他思想的不透明,”人类学季刊81,no。2(2008): 407-20。在许多方面,不透明学说的语言人类学概念与其他不透明理论有很多共同之处,特别是在Edóuard Glissant的工作中;参见《关系诗学》译。贝特西·温(安娜堡:密歇根大学出版社,1997)。虽然Glissant的讨论是对支撑政治自治的感觉器官的富有成效的说明,但它与语言人类学对不透明性的讨论截然不同(迄今为止还没有联系),后者关注的是特定的语言社区如何发展出关于一个人能或不能了解他人思想的意识形态格雷姆·b·威尔逊和雷蒙德·a·r·麦克唐纳,“沉默的标志:在即兴合奏中协商音乐身份”,《音乐心理学》第40期。张晓明,“自由即兴演奏的音乐选择:一种定性的心理调查”,《音乐心理学》第44期,第5期。鲁珀特·斯塔施,“意识是一个权威问题:科罗威人道德心理学中不透明陈述的政治维度”,《人类学季刊》,第81期。classment Canonne和Nicolas Garnier,“集体自由即兴演奏中的个人决定和感知形式”,《新音乐研究杂志》第44期。[2](2015): 145-67.43这些是分流施普雷河流经城市的小运河这种赞美的不具体品质可能部分是由于演奏近一个小时的无节奏音乐而没有预先确定的作曲,使用繁琐的,不可预测的扩展技术或器乐准备来操纵声音,同时试图听自己和他人同时作曲和演奏的固有迷失效果。许多玩家和其他观察者描述了这些体验的迷失本质,如John Corbett,《延伸游戏:从John Cage到Dr. Funkenstein的谈话》(Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 1994), 201-08;“Entwurf und Ereignis”。如果自由即兴创作本身的风格特征是平等主义承诺的产物,那么这些特征有效地诱导了一种平等主义的立场,即放弃同伴批评,因为它们使即兴演奏者无法真正回忆起在作品本身的过程中发生了什么。此外,尽管演奏者经常对他们的演奏和表演进行个人录音,但我很少观察到即兴演奏者为了回忆和自我评价而将他们作为一个群体进行回顾,这种行为与Derek Bailey对录音作为即兴表演事件的权威、客观文件的总体怀疑是一致的;参见德里克·贝利:《即兴创作:音乐的本质与实践》(纽约:达·卡波出版社,1993),第45页参见Timothy Rice,“理解和产生口述传统的可变性:从保加利亚风笛手那里学习”,《美国民俗杂志》108,第108期。429(1995): 266-76.46本·沃森,德里克·贝利和自由即兴创作的故事(纽约:Verso, 2004), 78.47 Kwami Coleman,“自由爵士和‘新事物’美学,身份和质地,1960-1966,”《音乐学杂志》38,第78期。柯贝特,《自由即兴演奏指南》,1。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Jazz Perspectives
Jazz Perspectives Arts and Humanities-Music
CiteScore
0.60
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
The Shitposting of Jazz to Come: Virtual Communities, Internetworks, and the Dank Jazz Meme Free Improvisation, Egalitarianism, and Knowledge Fusion – Locating Artistic Hybridity in Miles Davis’ “Spanish Key” The Gathering Ground: Composing Collaboration in Nyilipidgi, a Dynamic Meeting of manikay and jazz Uncovering the Origin Story of Juan Tizol’s Caravan: A Predecessor
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1