Sentence-Level Writing Skills in Children With and Without Developmental Language Disorders

IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Topics in Language Disorders Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI:10.1097/tld.0000000000000327
{"title":"Sentence-Level Writing Skills in Children With and Without Developmental Language Disorders","authors":"","doi":"10.1097/tld.0000000000000327","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"In this issue of Topics in Language Disorders, guest issue editor Dr. Anthony Koutsoftas invited groups of authors to present research related to text generation, primarily at the sentence level, by preschoolers and school-age children, including children with language impairments. Five articles are included, representing systematic reviews of the literature, examination of syntactic and morphological complexity and accuracy, and assessment and intervention for sentence writing. In the first article, Williams and Larkin conducted a systematic review of 39 studies in which transcription (i.e., writing mechanics such as spelling) and/or translation (i.e., converting ideas into comprehensible language) processes were evaluated in children between 4 and 17 years of age. They found that children with developmental language disorders (DLD) exhibited delays in both translation and transcription processes relative to their same-age peers with typical language development. These delays were observed across different measures including writing quality, writing productivity, and spelling. The authors discuss the limitations they saw in the literature they reviewed and offer suggestions for researchers for further work. In the second article, Brimo et al. analyzed narrative writing samples produced by a group of elementary students with DLD and another group with typical language development who were on average nearly a year younger. They specifically examined morphological (e.g., past tense -ed and be verbs) and syntactic (e.g., word omission and word order) errors in both simple and complex sentences produced by the students. They found that children with DLD produced a higher percentage of regular and irregular past tense errors than children with typical language development but only in simple sentences. Children with DLD produced significantly more syntactic errors than children without language delay in simple and complex sentences. However, children with DLD did not produce significantly more errors in simple sentences than in complex sentences. They discuss implications for assessment by speech–language pathologists. Next, Ritchey et al. performed a systematic review of 16 sentence writing intervention studies. These studies included oral language instruction, written language instruction, or combined oral and written language instruction. Participants in the reviewed studies ranged from typically developing to students at risk for or identified with learning disabilities or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder to students with significant developmental disabilities such as autism. The core instructional procedures that appeared to yield positive outcomes in sentence writing included explicit instruction, self-regulation procedures, opportunities to link oral and written language, and sentence generation practice. The authors noted several significant limitations in the extant research, including the limited number of studies in this area. Then, in the study reported by Hall-Mills and Wood, the syntactic complexity of informational texts produced by fifth-grade students with and without language impairment, with and without native English-speaking proficiency, was compared. They focused on the frequency of utterances containing complex syntax and four specific clause types (conjoined, subordinate, relative, and full complement). Children with typical language development, regardless of English proficiency, wrote more words, utterances, and different word roots than their peers with language impairment. When text length in the writing samples was controlled, a significant difference between groups was found for use of relative clauses but not for other clause types. Specifically, English-proficient students with language impairment produced a greater proportion of utterances with relative clauses, which is not necessarily associated with more sophisticated sentences in informational texts. In addition, there were moderate positive correlations between variables of complex syntax and a measure of text narrativity, which was low given the informative genre of the writing samples. In the last article, Marble-Flint and Koutsoftas conducted a feasibility study of a virtual sentence writing probe task with 15 intermediate-grade children, some of whom were considered struggling writers. The probe task included visual (pictures), written (descriptive sentences for pictures plus key words, representing nouns, verbs, adverbs, and conjunctions, to be used in responses), and verbal (oral rehearsal) scaffolds. Scores examined included total number of words, a sentence accuracy score, and a word accuracy score, which were compared across three administration time points. Generally, there were no significant differences across time points on the measures for the sample of children. Moreover, scores on the sentence writing probes were correlated with standardized measures of oral language. The study supports the proof of concept that virtual assessment procedures can be used to assess sentence-level writing in late elementary-age students, an important finding, given the expansion of online education options since the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. Overall, the articles in this issue of Topics in Language Disorders indicate children who struggle with writing, especially those of whom are diagnosed with DLD, exhibit morphological and syntactic errors in their written sentences (even when such errors are resolved in their spoken language), use more relative clauses when this may not be the best mechanism for conveying information through writing, and have more difficulties with transcription and translation processes involved in text production (and, correspondingly, produce less text than their typical peers). Fortunately, research reviewed in this issue also indicates that sentence-level writing instruction is generally effective for students with disabilities and assessments to examine the sentence level of written language production can be employed even in virtual settings. —Gary A. Troia, PhD, CCC-SLP —Sarah E. Wallace, PhD, CCC-SLP Co-Editors","PeriodicalId":51604,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Language Disorders","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8000,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Topics in Language Disorders","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1097/tld.0000000000000327","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"LINGUISTICS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

In this issue of Topics in Language Disorders, guest issue editor Dr. Anthony Koutsoftas invited groups of authors to present research related to text generation, primarily at the sentence level, by preschoolers and school-age children, including children with language impairments. Five articles are included, representing systematic reviews of the literature, examination of syntactic and morphological complexity and accuracy, and assessment and intervention for sentence writing. In the first article, Williams and Larkin conducted a systematic review of 39 studies in which transcription (i.e., writing mechanics such as spelling) and/or translation (i.e., converting ideas into comprehensible language) processes were evaluated in children between 4 and 17 years of age. They found that children with developmental language disorders (DLD) exhibited delays in both translation and transcription processes relative to their same-age peers with typical language development. These delays were observed across different measures including writing quality, writing productivity, and spelling. The authors discuss the limitations they saw in the literature they reviewed and offer suggestions for researchers for further work. In the second article, Brimo et al. analyzed narrative writing samples produced by a group of elementary students with DLD and another group with typical language development who were on average nearly a year younger. They specifically examined morphological (e.g., past tense -ed and be verbs) and syntactic (e.g., word omission and word order) errors in both simple and complex sentences produced by the students. They found that children with DLD produced a higher percentage of regular and irregular past tense errors than children with typical language development but only in simple sentences. Children with DLD produced significantly more syntactic errors than children without language delay in simple and complex sentences. However, children with DLD did not produce significantly more errors in simple sentences than in complex sentences. They discuss implications for assessment by speech–language pathologists. Next, Ritchey et al. performed a systematic review of 16 sentence writing intervention studies. These studies included oral language instruction, written language instruction, or combined oral and written language instruction. Participants in the reviewed studies ranged from typically developing to students at risk for or identified with learning disabilities or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder to students with significant developmental disabilities such as autism. The core instructional procedures that appeared to yield positive outcomes in sentence writing included explicit instruction, self-regulation procedures, opportunities to link oral and written language, and sentence generation practice. The authors noted several significant limitations in the extant research, including the limited number of studies in this area. Then, in the study reported by Hall-Mills and Wood, the syntactic complexity of informational texts produced by fifth-grade students with and without language impairment, with and without native English-speaking proficiency, was compared. They focused on the frequency of utterances containing complex syntax and four specific clause types (conjoined, subordinate, relative, and full complement). Children with typical language development, regardless of English proficiency, wrote more words, utterances, and different word roots than their peers with language impairment. When text length in the writing samples was controlled, a significant difference between groups was found for use of relative clauses but not for other clause types. Specifically, English-proficient students with language impairment produced a greater proportion of utterances with relative clauses, which is not necessarily associated with more sophisticated sentences in informational texts. In addition, there were moderate positive correlations between variables of complex syntax and a measure of text narrativity, which was low given the informative genre of the writing samples. In the last article, Marble-Flint and Koutsoftas conducted a feasibility study of a virtual sentence writing probe task with 15 intermediate-grade children, some of whom were considered struggling writers. The probe task included visual (pictures), written (descriptive sentences for pictures plus key words, representing nouns, verbs, adverbs, and conjunctions, to be used in responses), and verbal (oral rehearsal) scaffolds. Scores examined included total number of words, a sentence accuracy score, and a word accuracy score, which were compared across three administration time points. Generally, there were no significant differences across time points on the measures for the sample of children. Moreover, scores on the sentence writing probes were correlated with standardized measures of oral language. The study supports the proof of concept that virtual assessment procedures can be used to assess sentence-level writing in late elementary-age students, an important finding, given the expansion of online education options since the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. Overall, the articles in this issue of Topics in Language Disorders indicate children who struggle with writing, especially those of whom are diagnosed with DLD, exhibit morphological and syntactic errors in their written sentences (even when such errors are resolved in their spoken language), use more relative clauses when this may not be the best mechanism for conveying information through writing, and have more difficulties with transcription and translation processes involved in text production (and, correspondingly, produce less text than their typical peers). Fortunately, research reviewed in this issue also indicates that sentence-level writing instruction is generally effective for students with disabilities and assessments to examine the sentence level of written language production can be employed even in virtual settings. —Gary A. Troia, PhD, CCC-SLP —Sarah E. Wallace, PhD, CCC-SLP Co-Editors
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
发展性语言障碍儿童和非发展性语言障碍儿童的句子水平写作技巧
在本期的《语言障碍话题》中,特邀编辑Anthony Koutsoftas博士邀请了几组作者来介绍有关学龄前儿童和学龄儿童(包括有语言障碍的儿童)在句子层面上的文本生成的研究。包括五篇文章,代表了文献的系统回顾,句法和形态的复杂性和准确性的检查,以及句子写作的评估和干预。在第一篇文章中,Williams和Larkin对39项研究进行了系统回顾,这些研究对4至17岁儿童的转录(即拼写等写作机制)和/或翻译(即将想法转换为可理解的语言)过程进行了评估。他们发现,患有发育性语言障碍(DLD)的儿童在翻译和转录过程中都比同龄的语言发育正常的儿童表现出延迟。这些延迟是通过不同的指标观察到的,包括写作质量、写作效率和拼写。作者讨论了他们在他们审查的文献中看到的局限性,并为研究人员提供了进一步工作的建议。在第二篇文章中,Brimo等人分析了一组DLD小学生和另一组语言发育正常的学生的叙事写作样本,他们的平均年龄比他们小近一岁。他们特别检查了学生们在简单句和复杂句中出现的形态学错误(例如,过去式-ed和be动词)和句法错误(例如,遗漏单词和词序)。他们发现,与语言发育正常的儿童相比,患有DLD的儿童出现规则和不规则过去时错误的比例更高,但仅限于简单句。在简单句和复杂句中,有语言障碍的儿童比没有语言障碍的儿童出现更多的句法错误。然而,DLD儿童在简单句上的错误并没有显著高于复杂句。他们讨论了语言病理学家评估的意义。接下来,Ritchey等人对16项句子写作干预研究进行了系统回顾。这些研究包括口头语言教学,书面语言教学,或口头和书面语言相结合的教学。被审查的研究的参与者范围从典型的发展到有学习障碍或注意力缺陷/多动障碍风险或被确定为学习障碍的学生到有明显发育障碍(如自闭症)的学生。在句子写作中产生积极效果的核心教学程序包括明确的指导、自我调节程序、将口头和书面语言联系起来的机会以及句子生成练习。作者指出了现有研究的几个重大局限性,包括该领域的研究数量有限。然后,在Hall-Mills和Wood报告的研究中,比较了有语言障碍和没有语言障碍的五年级学生所写的信息文本的句法复杂性,以及母语为英语的学生的句法复杂性。他们关注的是包含复杂句法和四种特定子句类型(连词、从属、关系和完整补语)的话语的频率。具有典型语言发育的儿童,无论英语水平如何,都比有语言障碍的同龄人写出更多的单词、话语和不同的词根。当写作样本中的文本长度受到控制时,两组之间在使用关系分句方面存在显著差异,而在其他分句类型方面则没有显著差异。具体来说,英语熟练但有语言障碍的学生使用关系从句的比例更大,这与信息文本中更复杂的句子并不一定相关。此外,复杂语法变量与文本叙事性之间存在适度的正相关,考虑到写作样本的信息类型,这种正相关程度较低。在上一篇文章中,Marble-Flint和Koutsoftas对15名初中儿童进行了虚拟句子写作探测任务的可行性研究,其中一些儿童被认为是写作困难。探究任务包括视觉(图片)、书面(图片的描述性句子加上关键词,代表名词、动词、副词和连词,用于回答)和口头(口头排练)脚手架。测试的分数包括单词总数、句子准确性分数和单词准确性分数,这些分数在三个给药时间点进行比较。一般来说,在儿童样本的测量上,在不同的时间点上没有显著的差异。此外,句子写作测试的得分与口语的标准化测试相关。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
3.50
自引率
0.00%
发文量
26
期刊介绍: Topics in Language Disorders (TLD) is a double-blind peer-reviewed topical journal that has dual purposes: (1) to serve as a scholarly resource for researchers and clinicians who share an interest in spoken and written language development and disorders across the lifespan, with a focus on interdisciplinary and international concerns; and (2) to provide relevant information to support theoretically sound, culturally sensitive, research-based clinical practices.
期刊最新文献
Implementing Strategy-Based Instruction for Struggling Writers via Telepractice Effects of Integrating Different Types of Physical Activity Into Virtual Rapid Word Learning Instruction for Children Who Are Deaf and Hard of Hearing Innovations in Language and Literacy for Children and Adolescents With Language Disorders Using Multiliteracies to Target Critical Media Literacy for Adolescents With Language Learning Disabilities Coordinating Multiple Language Levels in Writing
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1