{"title":"When is higher LMX comparison not always effective? The role of team-level LMX disparity and neuroticism","authors":"Kaili Zhang, Chiyin Chen, Ningyu Tang","doi":"10.1080/1359432x.2023.2259545","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACTTreating relative leader-member exchange (i.e., RLMX) as the objective and leader-member exchange social comparison (i.e., LMXSC) as the subjective operation of LMX comparison (i.e., LMXC) within a team, we integrate the RLMX and LMXSC literature and examine when higher LMXC is not always effective in employees’ workplace outcomes (namely, self-efficacy, task performance, and creativity). Revisiting social comparison theory, we propose that LMXC has positive relationships with team members’ task performance and creativity via the role of self-efficacy. Furthermore, team-level LMX disparity and team members’ neuroticism affect the above direct and indirect relationships. Specifically, we hypothesize that amid low LMX disparity, members are more likely to perform assimilation rather than comparison that weakens the positive impacts of LMXC. Team members with high neuroticism are prone to make upward rather than downward social comparisons, which also mitigates the positive impacts of LMXC. We test our hypotheses in a field (using RLMX as the objective LMXC measure; n = 559, N = 71) and an experimental study (using LMXSC as the subjective LMXC measure; n = 176). Generally, we find support for our hypotheses. Our study thus deepens the understanding of the effectiveness of LMXC within team contexts.KEYWORDS: RLMXLMXSCLMX disparityneuroticismsocial comparison Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author.Ethical approvalAll participants were fully informed that this study was anonymized. This study was organized in non-interventional way and the data were only used for a study focusing on leadership effectiveness. There were no risks during completing the survey.Informed Consent StatementWe have got consent from all participants during the survey. All participants were informed that they could skip any question he or she wished or quit the survey at any time. All participants were informed that this study was anonymized and the conclusions were only used for scientific studies.Notes1. Although LMXSC studies have demonstrated that LMXSC captures one focal employee’s subjective comparison of LMX with that of other team co-workers rather than average team LMX level (Vidyarthi et al., Citation2010), we suggest that the reference in LMXSC can be shifted based on the research question. For instance, Pan et al. (Citation2021) have shifted the reference of LMXSC to one particular co-worker. Hence, the reference in LMXSC can be shifted to team average LMX level, rendering LMXSC, basically, the subjective measure of LMXC.2. Although Vidyarthi et al. (Citation2010) have proposed that RLMX is the antecedent of LMXSC, they have found that LMXSC has a relatively high and positive relationship with RLMX (.79, p < .01). In this sense, we consider these two concepts compatible.3. We also applied LMX separation to test our hypothesized model. According to Harrison and Klein (Citation2007), LMX separation is calculated using the standard deviation of LMX within each team (as also seen in Tremblay et al., Citation2021). The results remained virtually the same. These results are available upon request from the first author.4. Because RLMX is calculated via the group-mean centre approach, we did not calculate the group-mean centre for it again. However, we defined it as the within-group level variable to reduce model miscalculations. Similar treatment was given to LMX, which was only defined as a within-group level variable and did not undergo group-mean centring (as RLMX is essentially calculated by the group-mean centring of LMX).5. As creativity and task performance were both obtained from supervisors, we added a common method factor with items from task performance and creativity, loading it to control for any potential common method effect. Although LMX and neuroticism were collected at the same time from the same source, we focused on the role of RLMX (LMX minus the team LMX mean); hence, we did not perform this common method test. The covariance between the common method factor and other latent factors was fixed at zero because the method effect concerns only the measures, not the constructs indicated thereby.6. In our regression analysis, we added a common method factor comprising five factors and specified this common method factor to have zero correlations with the focal variables (Podsakoff et al., Citation2003). This approach facilitated our interpretation of the findings by taking the CMV into consideration.7. We further tested the three-way interaction of RLMX, LMX disparity, and neuroticism on self-efficacy. The interaction effect among RLMX, LMX disparity and neuroticism on self-efficacy was significant (estimate = 1.19, SE = .58, p = .038). We followed Walker et al. (Citation2014) to examine the three-way interactions. Under low neuroticism, RLMX had a strong main effect, while the moderating role of LMX disparity was nonsignificant (slope difference = .05, t = 2.50, p > .05). Individuals with high neuroticism are thus more likely to make comparisons based on both RLMX and LMX disparities because of their insecure traits. Hence, the two-way interaction between RLMX and LMX disparity was significant, with a slope difference of .68 (t = 48.57, p < .001). Such findings affirm our statement suggesting that highly neurotic group members tend to engage in upward comparison.Additional informationFundingThis study was funded by the National Science Foundation of China under Grant [71902061], Grant [71902023] and Grant [72072116].","PeriodicalId":48240,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology","volume":"29 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0000,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432x.2023.2259545","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"MANAGEMENT","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
ABSTRACTTreating relative leader-member exchange (i.e., RLMX) as the objective and leader-member exchange social comparison (i.e., LMXSC) as the subjective operation of LMX comparison (i.e., LMXC) within a team, we integrate the RLMX and LMXSC literature and examine when higher LMXC is not always effective in employees’ workplace outcomes (namely, self-efficacy, task performance, and creativity). Revisiting social comparison theory, we propose that LMXC has positive relationships with team members’ task performance and creativity via the role of self-efficacy. Furthermore, team-level LMX disparity and team members’ neuroticism affect the above direct and indirect relationships. Specifically, we hypothesize that amid low LMX disparity, members are more likely to perform assimilation rather than comparison that weakens the positive impacts of LMXC. Team members with high neuroticism are prone to make upward rather than downward social comparisons, which also mitigates the positive impacts of LMXC. We test our hypotheses in a field (using RLMX as the objective LMXC measure; n = 559, N = 71) and an experimental study (using LMXSC as the subjective LMXC measure; n = 176). Generally, we find support for our hypotheses. Our study thus deepens the understanding of the effectiveness of LMXC within team contexts.KEYWORDS: RLMXLMXSCLMX disparityneuroticismsocial comparison Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author.Ethical approvalAll participants were fully informed that this study was anonymized. This study was organized in non-interventional way and the data were only used for a study focusing on leadership effectiveness. There were no risks during completing the survey.Informed Consent StatementWe have got consent from all participants during the survey. All participants were informed that they could skip any question he or she wished or quit the survey at any time. All participants were informed that this study was anonymized and the conclusions were only used for scientific studies.Notes1. Although LMXSC studies have demonstrated that LMXSC captures one focal employee’s subjective comparison of LMX with that of other team co-workers rather than average team LMX level (Vidyarthi et al., Citation2010), we suggest that the reference in LMXSC can be shifted based on the research question. For instance, Pan et al. (Citation2021) have shifted the reference of LMXSC to one particular co-worker. Hence, the reference in LMXSC can be shifted to team average LMX level, rendering LMXSC, basically, the subjective measure of LMXC.2. Although Vidyarthi et al. (Citation2010) have proposed that RLMX is the antecedent of LMXSC, they have found that LMXSC has a relatively high and positive relationship with RLMX (.79, p < .01). In this sense, we consider these two concepts compatible.3. We also applied LMX separation to test our hypothesized model. According to Harrison and Klein (Citation2007), LMX separation is calculated using the standard deviation of LMX within each team (as also seen in Tremblay et al., Citation2021). The results remained virtually the same. These results are available upon request from the first author.4. Because RLMX is calculated via the group-mean centre approach, we did not calculate the group-mean centre for it again. However, we defined it as the within-group level variable to reduce model miscalculations. Similar treatment was given to LMX, which was only defined as a within-group level variable and did not undergo group-mean centring (as RLMX is essentially calculated by the group-mean centring of LMX).5. As creativity and task performance were both obtained from supervisors, we added a common method factor with items from task performance and creativity, loading it to control for any potential common method effect. Although LMX and neuroticism were collected at the same time from the same source, we focused on the role of RLMX (LMX minus the team LMX mean); hence, we did not perform this common method test. The covariance between the common method factor and other latent factors was fixed at zero because the method effect concerns only the measures, not the constructs indicated thereby.6. In our regression analysis, we added a common method factor comprising five factors and specified this common method factor to have zero correlations with the focal variables (Podsakoff et al., Citation2003). This approach facilitated our interpretation of the findings by taking the CMV into consideration.7. We further tested the three-way interaction of RLMX, LMX disparity, and neuroticism on self-efficacy. The interaction effect among RLMX, LMX disparity and neuroticism on self-efficacy was significant (estimate = 1.19, SE = .58, p = .038). We followed Walker et al. (Citation2014) to examine the three-way interactions. Under low neuroticism, RLMX had a strong main effect, while the moderating role of LMX disparity was nonsignificant (slope difference = .05, t = 2.50, p > .05). Individuals with high neuroticism are thus more likely to make comparisons based on both RLMX and LMX disparities because of their insecure traits. Hence, the two-way interaction between RLMX and LMX disparity was significant, with a slope difference of .68 (t = 48.57, p < .001). Such findings affirm our statement suggesting that highly neurotic group members tend to engage in upward comparison.Additional informationFundingThis study was funded by the National Science Foundation of China under Grant [71902061], Grant [71902023] and Grant [72072116].
摘要本文以相对领导-成员交换(RLMX)为客观,以领导-成员交换社会比较(LMXSC)为团队内部LMX比较(LMXC)的主观操作,整合RLMX和LMXSC的文献,考察高LMXC在员工的工作场所结果(即自我效能感、任务绩效和创造力)中是否总是有效。重新审视社会比较理论,我们提出LMXC通过自我效能感的作用与团队成员的任务绩效和创造力存在正相关关系。团队层面的LMX差异和团队成员的神经质对上述关系有直接和间接的影响。具体而言,我们假设在低LMX差异下,成员更倾向于同化而不是比较,从而削弱了LMXC的积极影响。高神经质的团队成员更倾向于进行向上而非向下的社会比较,这也减弱了LMXC的正向影响。我们在一个领域中检验了我们的假设(使用RLMX作为客观LMXC测量;n = 559, n = 71)和一项实验研究(使用LMXSC作为主观LMXC测量;N = 176)。一般来说,我们会找到支持我们假设的证据。因此,我们的研究加深了对团队背景下LMXC有效性的理解。关键词:RLMXLMXSCLMX差异神经症社会比较披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。支持本研究结果的数据可向通讯作者索取。伦理批准所有参与者都被充分告知本研究是匿名的。本研究采用非介入的方式组织,数据仅用于关注领导力有效性的研究。在完成调查过程中没有风险。知情同意书我们在调查过程中已经获得了所有参与者的同意。所有参与者都被告知,他们可以跳过他或她想要的任何问题,也可以随时退出调查。所有参与者被告知本研究是匿名的,结论仅用于科学研究。虽然LMXSC研究表明,LMXSC捕获的是一个焦点员工与其他团队同事的主观LMX比较,而不是团队平均LMX水平(Vidyarthi et al., Citation2010),但我们建议LMXSC中的参考可以根据研究问题进行转移。例如,Pan等人(Citation2021)已经将LMXSC的引用转移到一个特定的同事身上。因此,LMXSC中的参考可以转移到团队平均LMX水平,使LMXSC基本上成为LMXSC的主观测量。虽然Vidyarthi等人(Citation2010)提出RLMX是LMXSC的前项,但他们发现LMXSC与RLMX有较高的正相关关系(79, p < 0.01)。在这个意义上,我们认为这两个概念是相容的。我们还应用LMX分离来检验我们的假设模型。根据Harrison和Klein (Citation2007), LMX分离是使用每个团队内LMX的标准差来计算的(Tremblay et al., Citation2021也可以看到)。结果几乎是一样的。这些结果可向第一作者索取。因为RLMX是通过群均值中心方法计算的,所以我们没有再次计算它的群均值中心。然而,我们将其定义为组内水平变量,以减少模型的错误计算。对LMX进行了类似的处理,仅将其定义为组内水平变量,不进行组均值中心化(因为RLMX本质上是通过LMX的组均值中心化来计算的)。由于创造力和任务绩效都是从主管那里获得的,我们添加了一个共同方法因素,其中包括任务绩效和创造力的项目,将其加载到控制任何潜在的共同方法效果。虽然LMX和神经质是同时从同一来源收集的,但我们关注的是RLMX的作用(LMX减去团队LMX的平均值);因此,我们没有执行这个常见的方法测试。共同方法因素和其他潜在因素之间的协方差固定为零,因为方法效应只涉及测量,而不是由此表明的结构。在我们的回归分析中,我们添加了一个由五个因素组成的共同方法因素,并指定该共同方法因素与焦点变量的相关性为零(Podsakoff et al., Citation2003)。这种方法通过考虑巨细胞病毒促进了我们对研究结果的解释。我们进一步检验了RLMX、LMX差异和神经质对自我效能的三向交互作用。RLMX、LMX差异和神经质对自我效能感的交互作用显著(估计值= 1.19,SE = 0.58, p = 0.038)。我们遵循Walker et al. (Citation2014)来检验三方相互作用。 低神经质状态下,RLMX具有较强的主效应,而LMX差异的调节作用不显著(斜率差= 0.05,t = 2.50, p < 0.05)。因此,高神经质的个体更有可能在RLMX和LMX差异的基础上进行比较,因为他们的不安全特征。因此,RLMX与LMX差异的双向交互作用显著,斜率差为0.68 (t = 48.57, p < .001)。这些发现证实了我们的说法,即高度神经质的群体成员倾向于进行向上比较。本研究由中国国家自然科学基金资助,资助项目[71902061],资助项目[71902023]和资助项目[72072116]。
期刊介绍:
The mission of the European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology is to promote and support the development of Work and Organizational Psychology by publishing high-quality scientific articles that improve our understanding of phenomena occurring in work and organizational settings. The journal publishes empirical, theoretical, methodological, and review articles that are relevant to real-world situations. The journal has a world-wide authorship, readership and editorial board. Submissions from all around the world are invited.