Don't look any further: How a model's linguistic group membership limits exploration and discovery in preschool children

IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Infant and Child Development Pub Date : 2024-01-30 DOI:10.1002/icd.2494
Nazlı Altınok, Rebeka Anna Zsoldos, Krisztina Andrási, Ildikó Király, Marco F. H. Schmidt
{"title":"Don't look any further: How a model's linguistic group membership limits exploration and discovery in preschool children","authors":"Nazlı Altınok,&nbsp;Rebeka Anna Zsoldos,&nbsp;Krisztina Andrási,&nbsp;Ildikó Király,&nbsp;Marco F. H. Schmidt","doi":"10.1002/icd.2494","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>From an early age, children are highly motivated to learn from others how ‘we’ do things in our cultural communities, and in a variety of domains (Csibra &amp; Gergely, <span>2009</span>; Schmidt &amp; Tomasello, <span>2012</span>; Tomasello, <span>2016</span>). The human unique propensity for rapid acquisition of knowledge from others allows us to develop and maintain a vast array of belief systems, arbitrary conventions, cultural tools and complex institutions (Boyd et al., <span>2011</span>; Gergely &amp; Csibra, <span>2006</span>; Legare, <span>2019</span>; Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, <span>2019</span>; Tomasello, <span>1996</span>). However, when children observe others' behaviours, some actions they observe may be erroneous, purposefully deceptive or, in some cases, simply irrelevant. Think of human-made artefacts whose causal structure and intended function(s) are often opaque to naïve learners (Hernik &amp; Csibra, <span>2009</span>; Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, <span>2023</span>). Chopsticks might be designed for eating, but they also have other potential functions: you can scratch your back with them, you can use them to tidy up your hair, you can use them to stir food or pick up out-of-reach objects. Observing all these different functions of the very same object, how do children decide which of these is the proper and culturally right way to use the artefact?</p><p>According to current theories of socio-cultural learning, human children have a propensity to engage in shared and collective forms (‘we-mode’) of cognition that allow for acquiring and constructing cultural-normative knowledge (Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, <span>2018</span>, <span>2019</span>; Tomasello, <span>2019</span>). Children are also attuned to ostensive communicative signals (e.g., eye contact and infant-directed speech) displayed by others that indicate their pedagogical intention (Natural Pedagogy Theory, Csibra &amp; Gergely, <span>2009</span>, Csibra &amp; Gergely, <span>2011</span>). Ostensive communicative signals instantiate a presumption of relevance (Sperber &amp; Wilson, <span>1986</span>) and epistemic trust in their addressees (Gergely et al., <span>2007</span>). Hence recognising the teaching intention of others helps young children to selectively attend to and acquire knowledge from trustworthy resources in a noisy social learning environment. Computational modelling work on teaching further indicates that a naïve learner expects a teacher to purposefully generate samples to increase their belief in the hypothesis that is to be learned: there is a basic epistemic expectation in naïve learners that a teacher would sample useful data for them (Bonawitz et al., <span>2011</span>; Buchsbaum et al., <span>2011</span>; Shafto &amp; Goodman, <span>2008</span>).</p><p>Whilst to date, there are several lines of empirical evidence showing that children indeed construe teachers as selectively demonstrating evidence to promote efficient and rapid information gain that allows for inductive inferences (e.g., Butler &amp; Markman, <span>2012</span>, <span>2014</span>), previous research also uncovered that teaching contexts restrict children's exploratory behaviours (Bonawitz et al., <span>2011</span>; Gweon et al., <span>2014</span>). Yet an important question remains: Do children exercise context-sensitive epistemic vigilance against teachers taking into account their linguistic group membership and thus the social relevance of the demonstration? When it comes to the demonstration of an artefact function (i.e., a piece of normative cultural knowledge; Casler et al., <span>2009</span>; Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, <span>2018</span>), we argue that children will use explicit (linguistic) cues about the teacher's social group membership to rapidly acquire relevant cultural knowledge and thus restrict their exploratory behaviour on the artefact upon viewing a teacher from the same social group as them demonstrating the artefact function. In other words, we predict that if children observe a demonstrator speaking in their own native language showing a particular function on a novel artefact with multiple potential affordances, then they would be more likely to prioritise the demonstrated function in their own reenactment. On the other hand, when they see the same function demonstration of the artefact performed by a foreign language speaker they would be more likely to explore the artefact to realise other potential functions as they would not interpret the demonstration as indicating conventional, normative use of the object relevant to them.</p><p>Here, we focus on linguistic group membership as language-based social categories reliably mark the boundaries of different social communities, unlike race or gender (Baker, <span>2001</span>; Giles &amp; Billings, <span>2004</span>; Henrich &amp; Henrich, <span>2007</span>; Moya &amp; Henrich, <span>2016</span>; Pietraszewski &amp; Schwartz, <span>2014a</span>; Pietraszewski &amp; Schwartz, <span>2014b</span>). Even if infants show sensitivity to the dimension of race or gender from a very young age as reflected by their different visual attention to these social category distinctions (Bar-Haim et al., <span>2006</span>; Quinn et al., <span>2002</span>), we argue that linguistic group membership plays a special role in children's learning from others. Before their first birthday, infants not only pay more attention to individuals who speak their own language but also critically they pay more attention to novel stimuli when these are introduced by their own language speakers than by foreign language speakers (Marno et al., <span>2016</span>; Soley &amp; Sebastián-Gallés, <span>2015</span>). They also selectively prefer the objects or food items offered by their native speakers to those offered by foreign language speakers (Kinzler et al., <span>2007</span>; Shutts et al., <span>2009</span>). At 11 months, infants also represent an informant who speaks the same language as they do as someone who would provide them with relevant information as indicated by the greater anticipatory theta activity upon viewing a native language speaker versus a foreign language speaker (Begus et al., <span>2016</span>). In toddlerhood, they show propensity to selectively imitate sub-efficient instrumental actions from demonstrators who speak the same language as them (Altınok et al., <span>2022</span>; Buttelmann et al., <span>2013</span>; Howard et al., <span>2015</span>) and as they grow older, they keep these sub-efficient actions in their repertoire despite viewing efficient action alternatives in achieving the same goal as shown by a linguistic outgroup demonstrator (Altınok et al., <span>2020</span>).</p><p>Critically, empirical evidence also indicates that the native language survives over race: when racial group membership of the agents 5-year-olds viewed was pitted against their accent, children privileged native accent over same-race (Kinzler &amp; Dautel, <span>2012</span>, see similar findings with infants Kinzler &amp; Spelke, <span>2011</span>). All these findings corroborate the evidence showing the privileged role linguistic group membership has in guiding young children's learning from others. After all, shared-language cue signals the potential of a common cultural background between two parties—between the teacher and the naïve learner, and also the teacher's capacity to transmit culturally relevant information. Observing a teacher who speaks the same language as themselves, the naïve learners could infer that they are not only similar in the language they speak but also in several other culturally significant domains that they have yet no expertise in (see also Oláh et al., <span>2019</span>; Soley &amp; Aldan, <span>2020</span>) In this vein, shared language<sup>1</sup> can reliably guide naïve learners to the informants who could be the optimal teachers from whom they can learn culturally relevant knowledge.</p><p>This is directly demonstrated by studies on how children learn artefact functions—given that artefacts, human-made tools surrounding our daily lives, are fundamental to our material cultural knowledge. Upon being demonstrated the use of a novel artefact, children represent the demonstrated function as the target artefact's enduring function and avoid using the artefact for any other function (Casler &amp; Kelemen, <span>2005</span>). Children also appreciate that the proper function of artefacts—that is, how to use a human-made object—is not an idiosyncratic affair, but generalizable and normatively prescribed (Rakoczy &amp; Schmidt, <span>2013</span>; Schmidt &amp; Tomasello, <span>2012</span>). Upon being taught what the novel artefact is for, they protest (as a third-party observer) against an agent who uses that target artefact differently (Casler et al., <span>2009</span>). Young children also show selective protest against ingroup rather than outgroup individuals in the context of violations of conventional norms on how to act on human-made artefacts (Schmidt et al., <span>2012</span>). They can reason that artefacts ought to be used in conventionally agreed-upon ways despite appearing to afford other functions, and there is a proper way in which ‘we’ ought to use them (Chaigneau et al., <span>2016</span> on normative accounts of artefact functions; see also Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, <span>2019</span>, <span>2023</span>).</p><p>Previous research has shown that even toddlers understand that the conventional use of familiar objects is part of and linked to the broader shared knowledge. Two-year-olds expect the agent to be the source of a foreign language if they have previously seen the agent using an artefact unconventionally, but not if the agent has used it conventionally (Oláh et al., <span>2014</span>). Toddlers also expect those who speak their language to use the same artefact for the same function, suggesting that they understand the social consensus behind the normative use of novel artefacts (Pető et al., <span>2021</span>). They form representations of artefacts and their functions in inflexible ways when these are demonstrated by their native language speakers, as revealed by their persistent endorsement of tools that are presented by members of their own language group, even though the tool does not effectively achieve the intended purpose (Oláh et al., <span>2016</span>). Around the age of 5, children also evaluate a novel tool as serving only one function but only when this tool use demonstration is delivered by their linguistic group member, a person who speaks the same language as they do, leading enduring mutually exclusive tool-function mappings (Pető et al., <span>2018</span>).</p><p>Considering the accumulating evidence on the role of native language influencing the way children map the functions to novel artefacts, and how artefact use is rooted in cultural learning as the way they are used is shaped by conventional agreement, here we are using the classic paradigm of Bonawitz et al. (<span>2011</span>) to investigate how children limit their exploratory behaviour on a novel artefact following a demonstration by linguistic ingroup versus outgroup. The study has two aims. Our first aim is to conceptually replicate the original study of Bonawitz et al. (<span>2011</span>) in a different cultural setting, in [ANONYMIZED] sample of children. Secondly, we aim to extend the interpretation of the original findings by arguing that children's spontaneous restriction of their exploration of novel artefacts may be a consequence of their strong motivation to rapidly acquire relevant cultural knowledge from their linguistic ingroups.</p><p>Different lines of research suggest that young children have a tendency to over-attribute functions and norms of proper use to novel artefacts, even if there are alternative and equally feasible ways of how to use the artefact (promiscuous teleology and normativity; Casler &amp; Kelemen, Casler &amp; Kelemen, <span>2005</span>; Kelemen, Kelemen, <span>1999</span>, <span>2004</span>; Schmidt et al., Schmidt et al., <span>2011</span>, Schmidt et al., <span>2016</span>). Considering this, we propose using an unfamiliar apparatus with multiple functions as it was in the original study of Bonawitz et al. (<span>2011</span>). We would argue that such apparatus mimics everyday artefacts that have one intended function but multiple possible uses (e.g., a fork is to eat with, but it can be used to brush one's hair or to pull something out from underneath the drawer) and thus it can be a suitable tool for investigating cultural learning. Thus, as in Bonawitz and her colleagues' study, children will view a novel artefact with four different functions. In the same-language demonstration, they will observe a female adult speaking the same language as themselves demonstrating one function of the artefact (henceforth ‘linguistic ingroup’ condition). In the foreign-language demonstration, they will observe the same function demonstration by another female adult who speaks a different language than them (‘linguistic outgroup’ condition). Importantly, the demonstrations will be nonverbal but incorporate ostensive signals to reveal the demonstrators' teaching intention. When the demonstration phase is done, children will be given the artefact, and we will measure children's exploratory behaviours on it. We predict that children will be fixating more on the demonstrated function of the artefact, thus limiting their exploratory behaviour and discovery when the demonstration is delivered by their linguistic ingroup teacher. We expect children to widely explore the artefact and also discover its non-demonstrated functions when the demonstration is delivered by a linguistic outgroup teacher.</p><p>In line with our main hypothesis, we predict that children will limit their explorative behaviour on the target artefact upon viewing the function demonstration by their linguistic ingroup. We expect this to be evident by the greater proportion of time children will spend on the demonstrated function in the linguistic ingroup condition than in the linguistic outgroup condition. We will carry out an independent samples <i>t</i>-test following the previous work of Gweon et al. (<span>2014</span>) comparing the proportion of time children will spend on the demonstrated function in two conditions. Critically, we predict that children in both conditions will use the artefact realising its demonstrated function at least once during the response phase. To test this, we will compare the number of children doing so in the two conditions using Fisher's exact test.</p><p>Moreover, we expect children to discover fewer target functions after the linguistic ingroup's demonstration than after the linguistic outgroup demonstration. We also predict that children in the linguistic ingroup condition will perform fewer unique actions on the artefact than children in the linguistic outgroup condition. Both the number of target functions children will discover and the unique actions they will perform on the artefact in two conditions will be contrasted by an independent samples <i>t</i>-test.</p><p>Given Bonawitz et al. (<span>2011</span>) findings, we also predict condition differences in the overall amount of time children will spend playing. The overall time children will spend interacting with the artefact in the linguistic ingroup condition will be shorter than in the linguistic outgroup condition. This will be again contrasted with independent samples <i>t</i>-tests. However, we do not predict that children will be paying differential attention to two demonstrations (as these will be nonverbal and identical except for the identity of the demonstrator in two conditions), nor do we predict that there will be any differences between the experimental conditions in the latency they act on the artefact. We will contrast the overt attention children display to the demonstration and the latency they act on the artefact in two conditions using independent samples <i>t</i>-tests.</p><p>When it comes to the first action and first function children would realise we expect no differences between the two conditions (see Altınok et al., <span>2020</span> for a similar pattern of findings in an over-imitation paradigm in children's imitative behaviour after linguistic ingroup and outgroup models). Whilst we anticipate that children in both conditions will first act on the artefact using the button to make it squeak, as demonstrated, it is also likely that children would be performing a different action or bringing about a different function than the one demonstrated as the first action or as the first function they will realise. Depending on the number of children who do so, we might also compare the number of children who faithfully adhere to the demonstrated action and to the function as the first action and the first function they perform between the experimental conditions using Fisher's exact test.</p><p>Regarding the baseline condition, our prediction is that the children in the baseline condition will explore the artefact similarly to those children in the linguistic outgroup condition, as will be evident in the overall playtime, in the number of target functions discovered, and in the number of unique actions performed on the artefact. We plan to run one-way ANOVAs for these variables between the three conditions using a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Following the original paper, we may also compare the three conditions using planned linear contrasts by applying the weights of 2, −1 and −1, respectively for the conditions of linguistic ingroup, linguistic outgroup and baseline for the outlined pattern of predictions explained above (namely for the overall play time, for the number of target functions discovered and for the number of unique actions performed).</p><p>We predict that children will be more likely to successfully bring about the target function in both experimental conditions than in the baseline condition. The number of children who successfully bring about the function (‘making the toy squeak’) will be contrasted by Fisher's exact test with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. When it comes to the first action and the first function children would realise, we also predict that children in the two experimental conditions will be prioritising the demonstration in their reenactments whilst we do not predict this for the baseline. Again, we will compare the number of children in three conditions using Fisher's exact test with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.</p><p>Since we aim to also have a fixed time-window coding (i.e., first 30 s) to match the duration of time children interact with the artefact across conditions, we will present the analyses for this time window separately in the Supplementary Materials for the number of unique actions children perform on the apparatus and the total number of target functions children will discover in three conditions.</p><p>Children show the propensity to assign functions to novel, unfamiliar objects from a very young age and rapidly form enduring tool-function mappings (Casler &amp; Kelemen, <span>2005</span>, <span>2007</span>; Deborah et al., <span>2004</span>). They also show sensitivity to the inherently cultural nature of artefact function, interpreting artefact use as reflecting conventional agreement behind it (Casler et al., <span>2009</span>; German et al., <span>2007</span>; Siegel &amp; Callanan, <span>2007</span>; see also Tomasello, <span>1999</span>). Whilst pedagogical demonstrations limit their exploration of artefact functions, leading them to fixate more on the demonstrated function despite the artefact potentially serving other purposes (Bonawitz et al., <span>2011</span>), it remains unclear whether relevant demonstrator characteristics, such as the language the demonstrator speaks, could also influence children's exploration behaviour on novel artefacts. Given that the same-language spoken is one direct social cue signalling that the teacher is likely to sample evidence that would be indicative of culturally relevant information worth acquiring, here with this proposed study, we predict that children will rely on their linguistic ingroup members for efficient information gain about artefact functions. When the demonstrator speaks the same language as children, they will be more likely to spend more time operating the novel artefact using the demonstrated function, showing limited exploratory behaviour on the artefact. When the demonstration is delivered by a foreign language speaker, we predict that children will be more likely to explore the artefact extensively.</p><p>If the data support our predictions, then the restrictive force the linguistic ingroup teacher demonstration exerts on children's exploration would suggest that children are making strong normative cultural inferences about the functions of artefacts (and more generally about how we should act in our cultural groups). However, if the linguistic group membership brings no additional interpretation of relevance about the demonstrated function then this planned study will still be valuable in conceptually replicating the original paradigm in a different cultural setting: given the nonverbal ostensive signals used by both demonstrators, children should be perceiving both demonstrations as relevant teaching episodes tailored for them and should limit their exploration after both teacher's demonstration independently of the language the demonstrators previously spoke. In this vein, we believe our baseline comparison to the planned experimental conditions will be critical in painting the full picture of whether or how children limit their exploratory behaviour in a cultural context outside of North America (but see Shneidman et al., <span>2016</span> for a study with Yucatec Mayan children) and expand our understanding of the ways cultural factors may influence preschool-aged children's understanding of artefact functions in general.</p><p><b>Nazlı Altınok:</b> Conceptualization; data curation; formal analysis; investigation; methodology; project administration; writing – original draft. <b>Rebeka Anna Zsoldos:</b> Conceptualization; data curation; investigation; methodology; resources; writing – review and editing. <b>Krisztina Andrási:</b> Formal analysis; methodology; writing – review and editing. <b>Ildikó Király:</b> Conceptualization; funding acquisition; methodology; resources; supervision; writing – review and editing. <b>Marco F. H. Schmidt:</b> Conceptualization; funding acquisition; methodology; supervision; writing – review and editing.</p><p>The authors declare no competing interests.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8000,"publicationDate":"2024-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2494","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Infant and Child Development","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/icd.2494","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

From an early age, children are highly motivated to learn from others how ‘we’ do things in our cultural communities, and in a variety of domains (Csibra & Gergely, 2009; Schmidt & Tomasello, 2012; Tomasello, 2016). The human unique propensity for rapid acquisition of knowledge from others allows us to develop and maintain a vast array of belief systems, arbitrary conventions, cultural tools and complex institutions (Boyd et al., 2011; Gergely & Csibra, 2006; Legare, 2019; Schmidt & Rakoczy, 2019; Tomasello, 1996). However, when children observe others' behaviours, some actions they observe may be erroneous, purposefully deceptive or, in some cases, simply irrelevant. Think of human-made artefacts whose causal structure and intended function(s) are often opaque to naïve learners (Hernik & Csibra, 2009; Schmidt & Rakoczy, 2023). Chopsticks might be designed for eating, but they also have other potential functions: you can scratch your back with them, you can use them to tidy up your hair, you can use them to stir food or pick up out-of-reach objects. Observing all these different functions of the very same object, how do children decide which of these is the proper and culturally right way to use the artefact?

According to current theories of socio-cultural learning, human children have a propensity to engage in shared and collective forms (‘we-mode’) of cognition that allow for acquiring and constructing cultural-normative knowledge (Schmidt & Rakoczy, 2018, 2019; Tomasello, 2019). Children are also attuned to ostensive communicative signals (e.g., eye contact and infant-directed speech) displayed by others that indicate their pedagogical intention (Natural Pedagogy Theory, Csibra & Gergely, 2009, Csibra & Gergely, 2011). Ostensive communicative signals instantiate a presumption of relevance (Sperber & Wilson, 1986) and epistemic trust in their addressees (Gergely et al., 2007). Hence recognising the teaching intention of others helps young children to selectively attend to and acquire knowledge from trustworthy resources in a noisy social learning environment. Computational modelling work on teaching further indicates that a naïve learner expects a teacher to purposefully generate samples to increase their belief in the hypothesis that is to be learned: there is a basic epistemic expectation in naïve learners that a teacher would sample useful data for them (Bonawitz et al., 2011; Buchsbaum et al., 2011; Shafto & Goodman, 2008).

Whilst to date, there are several lines of empirical evidence showing that children indeed construe teachers as selectively demonstrating evidence to promote efficient and rapid information gain that allows for inductive inferences (e.g., Butler & Markman, 2012, 2014), previous research also uncovered that teaching contexts restrict children's exploratory behaviours (Bonawitz et al., 2011; Gweon et al., 2014). Yet an important question remains: Do children exercise context-sensitive epistemic vigilance against teachers taking into account their linguistic group membership and thus the social relevance of the demonstration? When it comes to the demonstration of an artefact function (i.e., a piece of normative cultural knowledge; Casler et al., 2009; Schmidt & Rakoczy, 2018), we argue that children will use explicit (linguistic) cues about the teacher's social group membership to rapidly acquire relevant cultural knowledge and thus restrict their exploratory behaviour on the artefact upon viewing a teacher from the same social group as them demonstrating the artefact function. In other words, we predict that if children observe a demonstrator speaking in their own native language showing a particular function on a novel artefact with multiple potential affordances, then they would be more likely to prioritise the demonstrated function in their own reenactment. On the other hand, when they see the same function demonstration of the artefact performed by a foreign language speaker they would be more likely to explore the artefact to realise other potential functions as they would not interpret the demonstration as indicating conventional, normative use of the object relevant to them.

Here, we focus on linguistic group membership as language-based social categories reliably mark the boundaries of different social communities, unlike race or gender (Baker, 2001; Giles & Billings, 2004; Henrich & Henrich, 2007; Moya & Henrich, 2016; Pietraszewski & Schwartz, 2014a; Pietraszewski & Schwartz, 2014b). Even if infants show sensitivity to the dimension of race or gender from a very young age as reflected by their different visual attention to these social category distinctions (Bar-Haim et al., 2006; Quinn et al., 2002), we argue that linguistic group membership plays a special role in children's learning from others. Before their first birthday, infants not only pay more attention to individuals who speak their own language but also critically they pay more attention to novel stimuli when these are introduced by their own language speakers than by foreign language speakers (Marno et al., 2016; Soley & Sebastián-Gallés, 2015). They also selectively prefer the objects or food items offered by their native speakers to those offered by foreign language speakers (Kinzler et al., 2007; Shutts et al., 2009). At 11 months, infants also represent an informant who speaks the same language as they do as someone who would provide them with relevant information as indicated by the greater anticipatory theta activity upon viewing a native language speaker versus a foreign language speaker (Begus et al., 2016). In toddlerhood, they show propensity to selectively imitate sub-efficient instrumental actions from demonstrators who speak the same language as them (Altınok et al., 2022; Buttelmann et al., 2013; Howard et al., 2015) and as they grow older, they keep these sub-efficient actions in their repertoire despite viewing efficient action alternatives in achieving the same goal as shown by a linguistic outgroup demonstrator (Altınok et al., 2020).

Critically, empirical evidence also indicates that the native language survives over race: when racial group membership of the agents 5-year-olds viewed was pitted against their accent, children privileged native accent over same-race (Kinzler & Dautel, 2012, see similar findings with infants Kinzler & Spelke, 2011). All these findings corroborate the evidence showing the privileged role linguistic group membership has in guiding young children's learning from others. After all, shared-language cue signals the potential of a common cultural background between two parties—between the teacher and the naïve learner, and also the teacher's capacity to transmit culturally relevant information. Observing a teacher who speaks the same language as themselves, the naïve learners could infer that they are not only similar in the language they speak but also in several other culturally significant domains that they have yet no expertise in (see also Oláh et al., 2019; Soley & Aldan, 2020) In this vein, shared language1 can reliably guide naïve learners to the informants who could be the optimal teachers from whom they can learn culturally relevant knowledge.

This is directly demonstrated by studies on how children learn artefact functions—given that artefacts, human-made tools surrounding our daily lives, are fundamental to our material cultural knowledge. Upon being demonstrated the use of a novel artefact, children represent the demonstrated function as the target artefact's enduring function and avoid using the artefact for any other function (Casler & Kelemen, 2005). Children also appreciate that the proper function of artefacts—that is, how to use a human-made object—is not an idiosyncratic affair, but generalizable and normatively prescribed (Rakoczy & Schmidt, 2013; Schmidt & Tomasello, 2012). Upon being taught what the novel artefact is for, they protest (as a third-party observer) against an agent who uses that target artefact differently (Casler et al., 2009). Young children also show selective protest against ingroup rather than outgroup individuals in the context of violations of conventional norms on how to act on human-made artefacts (Schmidt et al., 2012). They can reason that artefacts ought to be used in conventionally agreed-upon ways despite appearing to afford other functions, and there is a proper way in which ‘we’ ought to use them (Chaigneau et al., 2016 on normative accounts of artefact functions; see also Schmidt & Rakoczy, 2019, 2023).

Previous research has shown that even toddlers understand that the conventional use of familiar objects is part of and linked to the broader shared knowledge. Two-year-olds expect the agent to be the source of a foreign language if they have previously seen the agent using an artefact unconventionally, but not if the agent has used it conventionally (Oláh et al., 2014). Toddlers also expect those who speak their language to use the same artefact for the same function, suggesting that they understand the social consensus behind the normative use of novel artefacts (Pető et al., 2021). They form representations of artefacts and their functions in inflexible ways when these are demonstrated by their native language speakers, as revealed by their persistent endorsement of tools that are presented by members of their own language group, even though the tool does not effectively achieve the intended purpose (Oláh et al., 2016). Around the age of 5, children also evaluate a novel tool as serving only one function but only when this tool use demonstration is delivered by their linguistic group member, a person who speaks the same language as they do, leading enduring mutually exclusive tool-function mappings (Pető et al., 2018).

Considering the accumulating evidence on the role of native language influencing the way children map the functions to novel artefacts, and how artefact use is rooted in cultural learning as the way they are used is shaped by conventional agreement, here we are using the classic paradigm of Bonawitz et al. (2011) to investigate how children limit their exploratory behaviour on a novel artefact following a demonstration by linguistic ingroup versus outgroup. The study has two aims. Our first aim is to conceptually replicate the original study of Bonawitz et al. (2011) in a different cultural setting, in [ANONYMIZED] sample of children. Secondly, we aim to extend the interpretation of the original findings by arguing that children's spontaneous restriction of their exploration of novel artefacts may be a consequence of their strong motivation to rapidly acquire relevant cultural knowledge from their linguistic ingroups.

Different lines of research suggest that young children have a tendency to over-attribute functions and norms of proper use to novel artefacts, even if there are alternative and equally feasible ways of how to use the artefact (promiscuous teleology and normativity; Casler & Kelemen, Casler & Kelemen, 2005; Kelemen, Kelemen, 1999, 2004; Schmidt et al., Schmidt et al., 2011, Schmidt et al., 2016). Considering this, we propose using an unfamiliar apparatus with multiple functions as it was in the original study of Bonawitz et al. (2011). We would argue that such apparatus mimics everyday artefacts that have one intended function but multiple possible uses (e.g., a fork is to eat with, but it can be used to brush one's hair or to pull something out from underneath the drawer) and thus it can be a suitable tool for investigating cultural learning. Thus, as in Bonawitz and her colleagues' study, children will view a novel artefact with four different functions. In the same-language demonstration, they will observe a female adult speaking the same language as themselves demonstrating one function of the artefact (henceforth ‘linguistic ingroup’ condition). In the foreign-language demonstration, they will observe the same function demonstration by another female adult who speaks a different language than them (‘linguistic outgroup’ condition). Importantly, the demonstrations will be nonverbal but incorporate ostensive signals to reveal the demonstrators' teaching intention. When the demonstration phase is done, children will be given the artefact, and we will measure children's exploratory behaviours on it. We predict that children will be fixating more on the demonstrated function of the artefact, thus limiting their exploratory behaviour and discovery when the demonstration is delivered by their linguistic ingroup teacher. We expect children to widely explore the artefact and also discover its non-demonstrated functions when the demonstration is delivered by a linguistic outgroup teacher.

In line with our main hypothesis, we predict that children will limit their explorative behaviour on the target artefact upon viewing the function demonstration by their linguistic ingroup. We expect this to be evident by the greater proportion of time children will spend on the demonstrated function in the linguistic ingroup condition than in the linguistic outgroup condition. We will carry out an independent samples t-test following the previous work of Gweon et al. (2014) comparing the proportion of time children will spend on the demonstrated function in two conditions. Critically, we predict that children in both conditions will use the artefact realising its demonstrated function at least once during the response phase. To test this, we will compare the number of children doing so in the two conditions using Fisher's exact test.

Moreover, we expect children to discover fewer target functions after the linguistic ingroup's demonstration than after the linguistic outgroup demonstration. We also predict that children in the linguistic ingroup condition will perform fewer unique actions on the artefact than children in the linguistic outgroup condition. Both the number of target functions children will discover and the unique actions they will perform on the artefact in two conditions will be contrasted by an independent samples t-test.

Given Bonawitz et al. (2011) findings, we also predict condition differences in the overall amount of time children will spend playing. The overall time children will spend interacting with the artefact in the linguistic ingroup condition will be shorter than in the linguistic outgroup condition. This will be again contrasted with independent samples t-tests. However, we do not predict that children will be paying differential attention to two demonstrations (as these will be nonverbal and identical except for the identity of the demonstrator in two conditions), nor do we predict that there will be any differences between the experimental conditions in the latency they act on the artefact. We will contrast the overt attention children display to the demonstration and the latency they act on the artefact in two conditions using independent samples t-tests.

When it comes to the first action and first function children would realise we expect no differences between the two conditions (see Altınok et al., 2020 for a similar pattern of findings in an over-imitation paradigm in children's imitative behaviour after linguistic ingroup and outgroup models). Whilst we anticipate that children in both conditions will first act on the artefact using the button to make it squeak, as demonstrated, it is also likely that children would be performing a different action or bringing about a different function than the one demonstrated as the first action or as the first function they will realise. Depending on the number of children who do so, we might also compare the number of children who faithfully adhere to the demonstrated action and to the function as the first action and the first function they perform between the experimental conditions using Fisher's exact test.

Regarding the baseline condition, our prediction is that the children in the baseline condition will explore the artefact similarly to those children in the linguistic outgroup condition, as will be evident in the overall playtime, in the number of target functions discovered, and in the number of unique actions performed on the artefact. We plan to run one-way ANOVAs for these variables between the three conditions using a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Following the original paper, we may also compare the three conditions using planned linear contrasts by applying the weights of 2, −1 and −1, respectively for the conditions of linguistic ingroup, linguistic outgroup and baseline for the outlined pattern of predictions explained above (namely for the overall play time, for the number of target functions discovered and for the number of unique actions performed).

We predict that children will be more likely to successfully bring about the target function in both experimental conditions than in the baseline condition. The number of children who successfully bring about the function (‘making the toy squeak’) will be contrasted by Fisher's exact test with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. When it comes to the first action and the first function children would realise, we also predict that children in the two experimental conditions will be prioritising the demonstration in their reenactments whilst we do not predict this for the baseline. Again, we will compare the number of children in three conditions using Fisher's exact test with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Since we aim to also have a fixed time-window coding (i.e., first 30 s) to match the duration of time children interact with the artefact across conditions, we will present the analyses for this time window separately in the Supplementary Materials for the number of unique actions children perform on the apparatus and the total number of target functions children will discover in three conditions.

Children show the propensity to assign functions to novel, unfamiliar objects from a very young age and rapidly form enduring tool-function mappings (Casler & Kelemen, 2005, 2007; Deborah et al., 2004). They also show sensitivity to the inherently cultural nature of artefact function, interpreting artefact use as reflecting conventional agreement behind it (Casler et al., 2009; German et al., 2007; Siegel & Callanan, 2007; see also Tomasello, 1999). Whilst pedagogical demonstrations limit their exploration of artefact functions, leading them to fixate more on the demonstrated function despite the artefact potentially serving other purposes (Bonawitz et al., 2011), it remains unclear whether relevant demonstrator characteristics, such as the language the demonstrator speaks, could also influence children's exploration behaviour on novel artefacts. Given that the same-language spoken is one direct social cue signalling that the teacher is likely to sample evidence that would be indicative of culturally relevant information worth acquiring, here with this proposed study, we predict that children will rely on their linguistic ingroup members for efficient information gain about artefact functions. When the demonstrator speaks the same language as children, they will be more likely to spend more time operating the novel artefact using the demonstrated function, showing limited exploratory behaviour on the artefact. When the demonstration is delivered by a foreign language speaker, we predict that children will be more likely to explore the artefact extensively.

If the data support our predictions, then the restrictive force the linguistic ingroup teacher demonstration exerts on children's exploration would suggest that children are making strong normative cultural inferences about the functions of artefacts (and more generally about how we should act in our cultural groups). However, if the linguistic group membership brings no additional interpretation of relevance about the demonstrated function then this planned study will still be valuable in conceptually replicating the original paradigm in a different cultural setting: given the nonverbal ostensive signals used by both demonstrators, children should be perceiving both demonstrations as relevant teaching episodes tailored for them and should limit their exploration after both teacher's demonstration independently of the language the demonstrators previously spoke. In this vein, we believe our baseline comparison to the planned experimental conditions will be critical in painting the full picture of whether or how children limit their exploratory behaviour in a cultural context outside of North America (but see Shneidman et al., 2016 for a study with Yucatec Mayan children) and expand our understanding of the ways cultural factors may influence preschool-aged children's understanding of artefact functions in general.

Nazlı Altınok: Conceptualization; data curation; formal analysis; investigation; methodology; project administration; writing – original draft. Rebeka Anna Zsoldos: Conceptualization; data curation; investigation; methodology; resources; writing – review and editing. Krisztina Andrási: Formal analysis; methodology; writing – review and editing. Ildikó Király: Conceptualization; funding acquisition; methodology; resources; supervision; writing – review and editing. Marco F. H. Schmidt: Conceptualization; funding acquisition; methodology; supervision; writing – review and editing.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Abstract Image

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
不要再往下看了模型的语言群体成员身份如何限制学龄前儿童的探索和发现
儿童从小就有强烈的动机向他人学习 "我们 "是如何在我们的文化社区和各种领域做事的(Csibra &amp; Gergely, 2009; Schmidt &amp; Tomasello, 2012; Tomasello, 2016)。人类从他人那里快速获取知识的独特倾向使我们能够发展并维持大量的信仰体系、任意约定、文化工具和复杂的制度(Boyd et al.)然而,当儿童观察他人的行为时,他们所观察到的某些行为可能是错误的、故意欺骗的,或者在某些情况下根本就是无关紧要的。想想人类制造的人工制品,其因果结构和预期功能对于天真的学习者来说往往是不透明的(Hernik &amp; Csibra, 2009; Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, 2023)。筷子可能是为吃饭而设计的,但它还有其他潜在的功能:你可以用它来挠背,可以用它来整理头发,可以用它来搅拌食物或拿起够不着的东西。根据当前的社会文化学习理论,人类儿童倾向于以共享和集体的形式("我们模式")进行认知,从而获取和建构文化规范性知识(Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, 2018, 2019; Tomasello, 2019)。儿童还能注意到他人显示的、表明其教学意图的表面交流信号(如眼神接触和婴儿引导式言语)(自然教学法理论,Csibra &amp; Gergely, 2009; Csibra &amp; Gergely, 2011)。密集交际信号体现了对相关性的假定(Sperber &amp; Wilson, 1986)和对受众的认识信任(Gergely et al.)因此,识别他人的教学意图有助于幼儿在嘈杂的社会学习环境中选择性地关注并从值得信赖的资源中获取知识。有关教学的计算建模工作进一步表明,天真的学习者期望教师有目的地生成样本,以增加他们对所要学习的假设的信念:天真的学习者有一个基本的认识论期望,即教师会为他们提供有用的数据样本(Bonawitz 等人,2011 年;Buchsbaum 等人,2011 年;Shafto &amp; Pennsylvania 等人,2011 年;Shafto &amp; Pennsylvania 等人,2011 年;Shafto &amp; Pennsylvania 等人,2011 年)、虽然迄今为止,有多种实证证据表明,儿童确实认为教师会有选择地展示证据,以促进高效、快速地获取信息,从而进行归纳推理(例如,Butler &amp; Markman, 2012, 2014),但先前的研究也发现,教学情境限制了儿童的探索行为(Bonawitz et al.)然而,一个重要的问题依然存在:儿童是否会考虑到自己的语言群体成员身份,进而考虑到教师示范的社会相关性,对教师行使情境敏感的认识警觉?我们认为,当涉及到人工制品功能(即一种规范性文化知识;Casler 等人,2009;Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, 2018)的演示时,儿童会利用有关教师社会群体成员身份的明确(语言)线索来迅速获取相关文化知识,从而在看到与他们来自同一社会群体的教师演示人工制品功能时限制他们对人工制品的探索行为。换句话说,我们预测,如果儿童观察到演示者用自己的母语在具有多种潜在能力的新奇人工制品上演示特定功能,那么他们就更有可能在自己的重演中优先考虑所演示的功能。另一方面,当他们看到会说外语的人对人工制品进行同样的功能演示时,他们会更倾向于探索人工制品以实现其他潜在功能,因为他们不会把演示理解为与他们相关的物品的常规、规范使用。在此,我们将重点放在语言群体成员身份上,因为与种族或性别不同,基于语言的社会类别能可靠地标记不同社会群体的边界(Baker,2001;Giles &amp; Billings,2004;Henrich &amp; Henrich,2007;Moya &amp; Henrich,2016;Pietraszewski &amp; Schwartz,2014a;Pietraszewski &amp; Schwartz,2014b)。即使婴儿在很小的时候就表现出对种族或性别维度的敏感性,正如他们对这些社会类别区别的不同视觉关注所反映的那样(Bar-Haim 等人,2006 年;Quinn 等人,2002 年),我们仍然认为,语言群体成员身份在儿童向他人学习的过程中扮演着特殊的角色。 考虑到越来越多的证据表明,母语对儿童将功能映射到新奇人工制品的方式具有影响作用,以及人工制品的使用方式是如何根植于文化学习的,因为它们的使用方式是由传统协议形成的,在此,我们采用 Bonawitz 等人(2011 年)的经典范式,研究儿童在语言内群体与语言外群体的演示之后,如何限制他们对新奇人工制品的探索行为。这项研究有两个目的。我们的第一个目的是在不同的文化背景下,以[匿名]儿童为样本,从概念上复制 Bonawitz 等人(2011 年)的原始研究。其次,我们旨在扩展对原始研究结果的解释,认为儿童自发地限制对新奇人工制品的探索,可能是由于他们有强烈的动机从自己的语言群体中快速获取相关的文化知识。不同的研究表明,幼儿倾向于过度赋予新人工制品以功能和正确使用的规范,即使该人工制品有其他同样可行的使用方法(promiscuous teleology and normativity; Casler &amp; Kelemen, Casler &amp; Kelemen, 2005; Kelemen, Kelemen, 1999, 2004; Schmidt et al、施密特等人,2011;施密特等人,2016)。考虑到这一点,我们建议使用具有多种功能的陌生仪器,就像博纳维茨等人(2011 年)最初研究中使用的仪器一样。我们认为,这种器具模仿的是具有一种预期功能但可能有多种用途的日常用品(例如,叉子是用来吃饭的,但也可以用来梳头或从抽屉下面拿出东西),因此,它可以成为研究文化学习的合适工具。因此,在 Bonawitz 及其同事的研究中,孩子们会看到一个具有四种不同功能的新工具。在同种语言演示中,他们将观察一位与自己说同种语言的女性成人演示该人工制品的一种功能(以下称 "语言同群 "条件)。在外语演示中,他们将观察另一位与他们说不同语言的成年女性演示相同的功能("语言外群 "条件)。重要的是,这些示范将是非语言的,但会结合暗示信号来揭示示范者的教学意图。示范阶段结束后,儿童将获得人工制品,我们将测量儿童对人工制品的探索行为。我们预测,如果示范者是语言上的同组教师,那么儿童就会更多地关注人工制品的功能,从而限制了他们的探索行为和发现。我们预计,当语言外群教师进行演示时,儿童会广泛探索人工制品,并发现其非演示功能。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Infant and Child Development
Infant and Child Development PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL-
CiteScore
2.90
自引率
9.10%
发文量
93
期刊介绍: Infant and Child Development publishes high quality empirical, theoretical and methodological papers addressing psychological development from the antenatal period through to adolescence. The journal brings together research on: - social and emotional development - perceptual and motor development - cognitive development - language development atypical development (including conduct problems, anxiety and depressive conditions, language impairments, autistic spectrum disorders, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders)
期刊最新文献
Who Am I? A Longitudinal Investigation of the Multidimensional Self Children presume confident informants will be accurate (until proven otherwise) Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development for Turkish Children: Cultural Adaptation, Validity and Reliability Analysis Qualities That Mexican, Dominican and African American US Mothers Attribute to ‘Good’ and ‘Bad’ Mothers and Fathers Infants adapt their pointing frequency to experimentally manipulated parent responsiveness but not parent pointing
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1