首页 > 最新文献

Infant and Child Development最新文献

英文 中文
Exploring Inter-Brain Coherence Between Fathers and Infants During Maternal Storytelling: An fNIRS Hyperscanning Study
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-26 DOI: 10.1002/icd.70002
Xin Zhou, Xuancu Hong, Patrick C. M. Wong

The current study examined the inter-brain coherence (IBC) between 34 dyads of fathers and infants 7–9 months of age using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). We specifically focused on father–infant IBC to broaden the empirical base beyond the mother–infant connections, as the former has received limited attention. There were three conditions: a baseline condition and two task conditions when the infant and the adult participant jointly listened to maternal storytelling in Cantonese in infant-directed speech (IDS) and adult-directed speech (ADS). Father–infant IBC was compared with stranger–infant IBC in the same experimental settings. Our results found that father–infant IBC was greater in the baseline and ADS conditions but not in the IDS condition, compared to stranger–infant IBC. Further, stranger–infant dyads showed greater IBC in the IDS condition than in the ADS condition, with no significance in father–infant IBC between the two speech conditions. These results identified different inter-brain connection mechanisms between the two dyads. The IBC pattern in stranger–infant dyads is driven by neural entrainment to mothers' speech, whereas father–infant IBC is more resistant to mothers' behaviours in the co-presence of both parents.

本研究使用功能性近红外光谱(fNIRS)检查了 34 对 7-9 个月大的父亲和婴儿之间的脑间连贯性(IBC)。我们特别关注父亲与婴儿之间的 IBC,以扩大母婴联系以外的经验基础,因为前者受到的关注有限。研究共分为三个条件:基线条件和两个任务条件,即婴儿和成人共同聆听母亲用粤语以婴儿为主导的语音(IDS)和成人为主导的语音(ADS)讲故事。在相同的实验条件下,父亲-婴儿 IBC 与陌生人-婴儿 IBC 进行了比较。我们的结果发现,与陌生人-婴儿 IBC 相比,父亲-婴儿 IBC 在基线和 ADS 条件下更大,但在 IDS 条件下不大。此外,陌生人-婴儿二人组在 IDS 条件下的 IBC 比在 ADS 条件下大,而父婴 IBC 在两种语言条件下没有显著差异。这些结果表明,两对父婴之间存在不同的脑间连接机制。陌生人-婴儿二人组中的 IBC 模式是由母亲言语的神经诱导驱动的,而父亲-婴儿的 IBC 则在父母双方共同在场的情况下对母亲的行为更具抵抗力。
{"title":"Exploring Inter-Brain Coherence Between Fathers and Infants During Maternal Storytelling: An fNIRS Hyperscanning Study","authors":"Xin Zhou,&nbsp;Xuancu Hong,&nbsp;Patrick C. M. Wong","doi":"10.1002/icd.70002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.70002","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study examined the inter-brain coherence (IBC) between 34 dyads of fathers and infants 7–9 months of age using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). We specifically focused on father–infant IBC to broaden the empirical base beyond the mother–infant connections, as the former has received limited attention. There were three conditions: a baseline condition and two task conditions when the infant and the adult participant jointly listened to maternal storytelling in Cantonese in infant-directed speech (IDS) and adult-directed speech (ADS). Father–infant IBC was compared with stranger–infant IBC in the same experimental settings. Our results found that father–infant IBC was greater in the baseline and ADS conditions but not in the IDS condition, compared to stranger–infant IBC. Further, stranger–infant dyads showed greater IBC in the IDS condition than in the ADS condition, with no significance in father–infant IBC between the two speech conditions. These results identified different inter-brain connection mechanisms between the two dyads. The IBC pattern in stranger–infant dyads is driven by neural entrainment to mothers' speech, whereas father–infant IBC is more resistant to mothers' behaviours in the co-presence of both parents.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.70002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143497173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Children's Worlds Psychological Well-Being Scale in Children Aged 10 and 12 From 30 Countries: Analysis From Classical Test Theory and Item Response Theory
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-20 DOI: 10.1002/icd.70003
Rodrigo Moreta-Herrera, Xavier Oriol-Granado, Mònica González, Jose A. Rodas

This study evaluates the Children's Worlds Psychological Well-Being Scale (CW-PSWBS) within a diverse international cohort of children aged 10 and 12, utilising Classical Test Theory (CTT) and Item Response Theory (IRT) methodologies. Through a detailed psychometric analysis, this research assesses the CW-PSWBS's structural integrity, focusing on its unidimensionality and measurement invariance across gender and age groups. The study also analyses the scale's item discrimination and difficulty levels using IRT. Participants: The sample consisted of children from 30 countries, aged 10 and 12 years, offering a broad perspective on psychological well-being across different cultures and developmental stages. The CW-PSWBS demonstrates a unidimensional structure, ensuring consistent measurement across genders and age groups. The scale's items exhibit strong discrimination and appropriate difficulty, highlighting its effectiveness in capturing the latent trait of psychological well-being, particularly at low and average score ranges. Validated by a comprehensive analysis grounded in both CTT and IRT, the CW-PSWBS emerges as a reliable tool for assessing the psychological well-being of children aged 10 and 12 across a wide array of cultural contexts. The study affirms the scale's robustness and cross-cultural validity, making a significant contribution to the field of child psychology.

{"title":"The Children's Worlds Psychological Well-Being Scale in Children Aged 10 and 12 From 30 Countries: Analysis From Classical Test Theory and Item Response Theory","authors":"Rodrigo Moreta-Herrera,&nbsp;Xavier Oriol-Granado,&nbsp;Mònica González,&nbsp;Jose A. Rodas","doi":"10.1002/icd.70003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.70003","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This study evaluates the Children's Worlds Psychological Well-Being Scale (CW-PSWBS) within a diverse international cohort of children aged 10 and 12, utilising Classical Test Theory (CTT) and Item Response Theory (IRT) methodologies. Through a detailed psychometric analysis, this research assesses the CW-PSWBS's structural integrity, focusing on its unidimensionality and measurement invariance across gender and age groups. The study also analyses the scale's item discrimination and difficulty levels using IRT. Participants: The sample consisted of children from 30 countries, aged 10 and 12 years, offering a broad perspective on psychological well-being across different cultures and developmental stages. The CW-PSWBS demonstrates a unidimensional structure, ensuring consistent measurement across genders and age groups. The scale's items exhibit strong discrimination and appropriate difficulty, highlighting its effectiveness in capturing the latent trait of psychological well-being, particularly at low and average score ranges. Validated by a comprehensive analysis grounded in both CTT and IRT, the CW-PSWBS emerges as a reliable tool for assessing the psychological well-being of children aged 10 and 12 across a wide array of cultural contexts. The study affirms the scale's robustness and cross-cultural validity, making a significant contribution to the field of child psychology.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-02-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143456014","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Socialisation of Emotion Regulation in Preschool Classrooms: How Do Peers Matter?
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-20 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2566
Tatiana Diebold, Ann-Kathrin Jaggy, Sonja Perren

The development of emotional competence is an important milestone during early childhood. Beyond early experience within the family, the (preschool) classroom is a relevant socialisation context, and both teachers and peers may contribute to children's emotion-related outcomes. Tracking changes in the emotion regulation competence of N = 173 preschool children (age in months: M = 43.3, SD = 6.6, 45% girls) over 6 months, the current study investigated whether and how peers contribute to differences in the development of emotion regulation in preschool classrooms. The research questions were addressed by examining three different kinds of peer groups: Classroom-level peer group, friendship group, and high-status peers (social network centrality). Teachers rated children's emotion regulation competence and empathic co-regulation behaviour—referring to preschoolers' supportive responses to their peers' emotional expressions—at three assessment points. Multilevel growth modelling provided evidence that friendship-group empathic co-regulation (empathy, helping, and comforting behaviour) predicted changes in preschoolers' emotion regulation over time, confirming that friendship groups influence young children's emotional development.

{"title":"Socialisation of Emotion Regulation in Preschool Classrooms: How Do Peers Matter?","authors":"Tatiana Diebold,&nbsp;Ann-Kathrin Jaggy,&nbsp;Sonja Perren","doi":"10.1002/icd.2566","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2566","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The development of emotional competence is an important milestone during early childhood. Beyond early experience within the family, the (preschool) classroom is a relevant socialisation context, and both teachers and peers may contribute to children's emotion-related outcomes. Tracking changes in the emotion regulation competence of <i>N</i> = 173 preschool children (age in months: <i>M</i> = 43.3, SD = 6.6, 45% girls) over 6 months, the current study investigated whether and how peers contribute to differences in the development of emotion regulation in preschool classrooms. The research questions were addressed by examining three different kinds of peer groups: Classroom-level peer group, friendship group, and high-status peers (social network centrality). Teachers rated children's emotion regulation competence and empathic co-regulation behaviour—referring to preschoolers' supportive responses to their peers' emotional expressions—at three assessment points. Multilevel growth modelling provided evidence that friendship-group empathic co-regulation (empathy, helping, and comforting behaviour) predicted changes in preschoolers' emotion regulation over time, confirming that friendship groups influence young children's emotional development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-02-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2566","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143456013","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Methods Used to Assess Early Mathematical Skills in 2-Year-Olds: A Review of the Literature
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-19 DOI: 10.1002/icd.70001
Hanna Weiers, Sohnia Ghattaura, Gaia Scerif, Francesco Sella, Victoria Simms, Iro Xenidou-Dervou, Camilla Gilmore

When children are aged around 2 years, many of the early foundations of mathematical skills are developing. Understanding this is important to shed light on theories of mathematical development. Nevertheless, little research has investigated 2-year-olds' early mathematical abilities, with most research focussing on either infants (aged 0–1 years) or pre-schoolers (aged 3–5 years). One possible reason for this lack of research may be the challenges associated with the methods used with this age group. Here, we review the methods used to assess 2-year-olds' basic mathematical skills and identify the gaps and issues with those methods. Our findings indicate several issues, both with individual methods used to test specific skills but also some common challenges applicable across all measures and skills. We discuss the identified issues and highlight a need for more appropriate approaches and for alternative methods of administration to better evaluate 2-year-olds' mathematical skills.

{"title":"Methods Used to Assess Early Mathematical Skills in 2-Year-Olds: A Review of the Literature","authors":"Hanna Weiers,&nbsp;Sohnia Ghattaura,&nbsp;Gaia Scerif,&nbsp;Francesco Sella,&nbsp;Victoria Simms,&nbsp;Iro Xenidou-Dervou,&nbsp;Camilla Gilmore","doi":"10.1002/icd.70001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.70001","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When children are aged around 2 years, many of the early foundations of mathematical skills are developing. Understanding this is important to shed light on theories of mathematical development. Nevertheless, little research has investigated 2-year-olds' early mathematical abilities, with most research focussing on either infants (aged 0–1 years) or pre-schoolers (aged 3–5 years). One possible reason for this lack of research may be the challenges associated with the methods used with this age group. Here, we review the methods used to assess 2-year-olds' basic mathematical skills and identify the gaps and issues with those methods. Our findings indicate several issues, both with individual methods used to test specific skills but also some common challenges applicable across all measures and skills. We discuss the identified issues and highlight a need for more appropriate approaches and for alternative methods of administration to better evaluate 2-year-olds' mathematical skills.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-02-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.70001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143439152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning in Dos Idiomas: The Impact of Codeswitching on Children's Noun and Verb Learning
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-04 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2568
Emma Libersky, Caitlyn Slawny, Margarita Kaushanskaya

Codeswitching is a common feature of bilingual language practices, yet its impact on word learning is poorly understood. Critically, processing costs associated with codeswitching may extend to learning. Moreover, verbs tend to be more difficult to learn than nouns, and the challenges of learning verbs could compound with processing costs associated with codeswitching. In two Experiments, we taught 4–5-year-old bilingual children verbs (Experiment 1, N = 25, 13 girls, 69% white, 83% Hispanic) and nouns (Experiment 2, N = 29, 12 girls, 72% while, 68% Hispanic) within English-only and Spanish-English codeswitched learning contexts. Children's language ability across Spanish and English was modeled continuously, allowing us to test the impact of language ability on learning. Children learned novel words successfully in both experiments, with no difference between conditions. However, children with higher language ability demonstrated steeper learning rates l in the English-only condition relative to the codeswitched condition in both Experiment 1 (standardized coefficient of 0.19) and Experiment 2 (standardized coefficient of 0.14). Our findings indicate that bilingual children can successfully learn both verbs and nouns in codeswitched contexts but learning is modulated by a child's language skills as well as the number of learning opportunities they have had.

{"title":"Learning in Dos Idiomas: The Impact of Codeswitching on Children's Noun and Verb Learning","authors":"Emma Libersky,&nbsp;Caitlyn Slawny,&nbsp;Margarita Kaushanskaya","doi":"10.1002/icd.2568","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2568","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Codeswitching is a common feature of bilingual language practices, yet its impact on word learning is poorly understood. Critically, processing costs associated with codeswitching may extend to learning. Moreover, verbs tend to be more difficult to learn than nouns, and the challenges of learning verbs could compound with processing costs associated with codeswitching. In two Experiments, we taught 4–5-year-old bilingual children verbs (Experiment 1, <i>N</i> = 25, 13 girls, 69% white, 83% Hispanic) and nouns (Experiment 2, <i>N</i> = 29, 12 girls, 72% while, 68% Hispanic) within English-only and Spanish-English codeswitched learning contexts. Children's language ability across Spanish and English was modeled continuously, allowing us to test the impact of language ability on learning. Children learned novel words successfully in both experiments, with no difference between conditions. However, children with higher language ability demonstrated steeper learning rates l in the English-only condition relative to the codeswitched condition in both Experiment 1 (standardized coefficient of 0.19) and Experiment 2 (standardized coefficient of 0.14). Our findings indicate that bilingual children can successfully learn both verbs and nouns in codeswitched contexts but learning is modulated by a child's language skills as well as the number of learning opportunities they have had.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2568","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143111618","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Brain Morphometry of Toddlers With Language Delay: An Exploratory Study
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-23 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2558
Courtney A. Filippi, Elizabeth Smith, Elizabeth Redcay, Heather Hazlett, Lauren Thompson, Stacy S. Manwaring, Precilla D'Souza, Audrey Thurm

While studies have documented neural correlates of language delay in toddlers with developmental conditions, those at genetic risk for language delay, and those born premature, no studies have examined neural correlates in toddlers exhibiting early language delay without known aetiology. This study examines brain morphometry in toddlers with and without early language delay. To do so, we collected magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) on toddlers with language delay (LD; n = 7, Mage = 19.67 months, 3 female, 2 Hispanic and 4 non-caucasian) and a typically developing (TD; n = 17, Mage = 22.73 months, 8 female, 2 Hispanic and 3 non-caucasian) comparison group. Exploratory analyses examined group differences in total brain volume, cortical thickness and cortical surface area using both a whole-brain and region of interest (Broca's and Wernicke's areas) approach. Results showed no gross brain anatomical differences between groups. However, there were group differences in cortical surface area in the temporal cortex (including Wernicke's area and left middle temporal gyrus, hedges' g = − 0.35) and Broca's area thickness. Results are reported using multiple analytic methods, age matching and exclusion of children later diagnosed with autism. While this exploratory study has a limited sample size, it provides novel findings that can be utilised to guide hypothesis-driven imaging studies on toddler language delay.

{"title":"Brain Morphometry of Toddlers With Language Delay: An Exploratory Study","authors":"Courtney A. Filippi,&nbsp;Elizabeth Smith,&nbsp;Elizabeth Redcay,&nbsp;Heather Hazlett,&nbsp;Lauren Thompson,&nbsp;Stacy S. Manwaring,&nbsp;Precilla D'Souza,&nbsp;Audrey Thurm","doi":"10.1002/icd.2558","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2558","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While studies have documented neural correlates of language delay in toddlers with developmental conditions, those at genetic risk for language delay, and those born premature, no studies have examined neural correlates in toddlers exhibiting early language delay without known aetiology. This study examines brain morphometry in toddlers with and without early language delay. To do so, we collected magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) on toddlers with language delay (LD; <i>n</i> = 7, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 19.67 months, 3 female, 2 Hispanic and 4 non-caucasian) and a typically developing (TD; <i>n</i> = 17, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 22.73 months, 8 female, 2 Hispanic and 3 non-caucasian) comparison group. Exploratory analyses examined group differences in total brain volume, cortical thickness and cortical surface area using both a whole-brain and region of interest (Broca's and Wernicke's areas) approach. Results showed no gross brain anatomical differences between groups. However, there were group differences in cortical surface area in the temporal cortex (including Wernicke's area and left middle temporal gyrus, hedges' <i>g</i> = − 0.35) and Broca's area thickness. Results are reported using multiple analytic methods, age matching and exclusion of children later diagnosed with autism. While this exploratory study has a limited sample size, it provides novel findings that can be utilised to guide hypothesis-driven imaging studies on toddler language delay.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2558","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143030915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Feasibility and Acceptability of the Mazi Umntanakho Digital Tool in South African Settings: A Qualitative Evaluation 南非设置Mazi Umntanakho数字工具的可行性和可接受性:定性评价
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-19 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2567
Catherine E. Draper, Caylee J. Cook, Elizabeth A. Ankrah, Jesus A. Beltran, Franceli L. Cibrian, Kimberley D. Lakes, Hanna Mofid, Lucretia Williams, Gillian R. Hayes

To address the need for interventions targeting social emotional development and mental health of young children in South Africa, the Mazi Umntanakho (‘know your child’) digital tool was co-designed, and piloted with caregivers and 3–5-year-old children involved in home visiting programmes promoting early childhood development. The aim of this study was to qualitatively evaluate the feasibility and acceptability of this tool in four urban and four rural low-income communities, from the perspective of home visitors and caregivers. Focus groups were conducted with home visitors (n = 117) and caregivers (n = 72). Issues relating to the feasibility of implementing the tool included timing of using the tool, technical and social implementation challenges, WhatsApp as the preferred platform, and participants' recommendations for future implementation. Overall, the tool was acceptable for home visitors and caregivers and was perceived to have a positive impact on children's development, and the relationship between caregivers and children.

为了解决针对南非幼儿社会情感发展和心理健康的干预措施的需求,Mazi Umntanakho(“了解你的孩子”)数字工具被共同设计,并与参与促进幼儿发展的家访计划的护理人员和3-5岁儿童一起进行了试点。本研究的目的是从家庭访客和护理人员的角度,定性地评估该工具在四个城市和四个农村低收入社区的可行性和可接受性。焦点小组包括家访(n = 117)和护理人员(n = 72)。与实施工具可行性相关的问题包括使用工具的时间,技术和社会实施挑战,WhatsApp作为首选平台,以及参与者对未来实施的建议。总的来说,这个工具是可以接受的家庭访客和照顾者,并被认为对儿童的发展和照顾者与儿童之间的关系有积极的影响。
{"title":"Feasibility and Acceptability of the Mazi Umntanakho Digital Tool in South African Settings: A Qualitative Evaluation","authors":"Catherine E. Draper,&nbsp;Caylee J. Cook,&nbsp;Elizabeth A. Ankrah,&nbsp;Jesus A. Beltran,&nbsp;Franceli L. Cibrian,&nbsp;Kimberley D. Lakes,&nbsp;Hanna Mofid,&nbsp;Lucretia Williams,&nbsp;Gillian R. Hayes","doi":"10.1002/icd.2567","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2567","url":null,"abstract":"<p>To address the need for interventions targeting social emotional development and mental health of young children in South Africa, the <i>Mazi Umntanakho</i> (‘know your child’) digital tool was co-designed, and piloted with caregivers and 3–5-year-old children involved in home visiting programmes promoting early childhood development. The aim of this study was to qualitatively evaluate the feasibility and acceptability of this tool in four urban and four rural low-income communities, from the perspective of home visitors and caregivers. Focus groups were conducted with home visitors (<i>n</i> = 117) and caregivers (<i>n</i> = 72). Issues relating to the feasibility of implementing the tool included timing of using the tool, technical and social implementation challenges, WhatsApp as the preferred platform, and participants' recommendations for future implementation. Overall, the tool was acceptable for home visitors and caregivers and was perceived to have a positive impact on children's development, and the relationship between caregivers and children.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-01-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2567","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142989848","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Defining Parent Responsiveness: A Systematic Review-Based Theoretical Model 定义父母回应:一个基于系统回顾的理论模型
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-23 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2565
Emily Campi, Elizabeth Nye, Grace T. Baranek

Parent responsiveness to infants' cues may support attachment, mental health, and neurodevelopment. Across studies, parent responsiveness includes various behaviours (e.g., verbal responses, sensitivity, timeliness, physical proximity). Mixed evidence regarding associations between parent responsiveness and child development may be partially attributable to variable conceptualizations of parent responsiveness. Additionally, divergent perspectives on parent responsiveness may limit cross-disciplinary collaborations, which are important to best support families in developing responsive parenting skills. Thus, the purpose of this systematic review of articles published from 1952 to 2022 was to establish a multidimensional definition of parent responsiveness. Inclusion criteria for the 463 articles included in this review were: (a) primary focus on behavioural parent responsiveness; (b) average infant age 6–18 months; and (c) in-depth definition of parent responsiveness. We completed a qualitative content analysis of the definitions of parent responsiveness extracted from each included article. The resulting definition contains 15 dimensions of parent responsiveness across four levels of behaviour that comprehensively describe the complexity of parent-infant interactions. This model is foundational for future studies of parent responsiveness, its impact on child development, and factors that influence parents' ability to respond to their infants' cues.

父母对婴儿线索的反应可能支持依恋、心理健康和神经发育。在研究中,父母的反应包括各种行为(例如,口头反应、敏感性、及时性、身体接近)。关于父母反应性与儿童发展之间的关联的混合证据可能部分归因于父母反应性的可变概念。此外,关于父母反应性的不同观点可能会限制跨学科合作,这对于最好地支持家庭发展反应性育儿技能非常重要。因此,对1952年至2022年发表的文章进行系统回顾的目的是建立父母响应性的多维定义。本综述纳入的463篇文章的纳入标准是:(a)主要关注父母的行为反应;(b)婴儿平均年龄6-18个月;(c)父母响应性的深度定义。我们完成了从每篇纳入的文章中提取的父母响应性定义的定性内容分析。由此产生的定义包含了父母反应的15个维度,跨越四个行为水平,全面描述了父母-婴儿互动的复杂性。该模型为今后研究父母反应性、其对儿童发展的影响以及影响父母对婴儿暗示的反应能力的因素奠定了基础。
{"title":"Defining Parent Responsiveness: A Systematic Review-Based Theoretical Model","authors":"Emily Campi,&nbsp;Elizabeth Nye,&nbsp;Grace T. Baranek","doi":"10.1002/icd.2565","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2565","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Parent responsiveness to infants' cues may support attachment, mental health, and neurodevelopment. Across studies, parent responsiveness includes various behaviours (e.g., verbal responses, sensitivity, timeliness, physical proximity). Mixed evidence regarding associations between parent responsiveness and child development may be partially attributable to variable conceptualizations of parent responsiveness. Additionally, divergent perspectives on parent responsiveness may limit cross-disciplinary collaborations, which are important to best support families in developing responsive parenting skills. Thus, the purpose of this systematic review of articles published from 1952 to 2022 was to establish a multidimensional definition of parent responsiveness. Inclusion criteria for the 463 articles included in this review were: (a) primary focus on behavioural parent responsiveness; (b) average infant age 6–18 months; and (c) in-depth definition of parent responsiveness. We completed a qualitative content analysis of the definitions of parent responsiveness extracted from each included article. The resulting definition contains 15 dimensions of parent responsiveness across four levels of behaviour that comprehensively describe the complexity of parent-infant interactions. This model is foundational for future studies of parent responsiveness, its impact on child development, and factors that influence parents' ability to respond to their infants' cues.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142884284","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Strengthening Developmental Science With Psychometric Evaluations: An Examination of the Spatial Arrangement Method as a Measure of Semantic Structure in Children 通过心理测量评估加强发展科学:将空间排列法作为儿童语义结构测量方法的研究
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-16 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2560
Catarina Vales, Zach Branson, Anna V. Fisher

Cognitive tasks are seldom evaluated on their ability to provide valid and reliable measurements of the construct they intend to measure. This scarcity of psychometric evaluations makes it challenging to evaluate replications of experimental effects and to relate performance in cognitive tasks to other constructs of interest. In developmental science, these issues are compounded by the often-imprecise measures derived from tasks completed by child participants. Here, we focus on the spatial arrangement method when used to assess semantic structure in children and evaluate its psychometric properties. Using a new analytic approach to capture individual variability in participants' arrangement in this task, we show that the spatial arrangement method has appropriate construct validity (β = 0.40), internal consistency (r2 = 0.20), and test–retest reliability (r2 = 0.41; ICC = 0.56) when used to evaluate semantic structure in U.S. children (4–9 years of age; N = 200 across 4 datasets). We discuss the implications of these findings for examining semantic structure in children and for strengthening methodological practices in developmental science more broadly.

认知任务很少被评估为他们提供有效和可靠的测量他们想要测量的结构的能力。心理测量评估的缺乏使得评估实验效果的重复性以及将认知任务中的表现与其他感兴趣的构念联系起来具有挑战性。在发展科学中,这些问题往往因儿童参与者完成的任务而产生的不精确测量而复杂化。本文主要研究了空间排列法在儿童语义结构评估中的应用,并对其心理测量特性进行了评价。利用新的分析方法,我们发现空间安排方法具有适当的结构效度(β = 0.40)、内部一致性(r2 = 0.20)和重测信度(r2 = 0.41);ICC = 0.56),用于评估美国儿童(4-9岁;N = 200(横跨4个数据集)。我们将讨论这些发现对检查儿童语义结构的影响,并在更广泛的发展科学中加强方法实践。
{"title":"Strengthening Developmental Science With Psychometric Evaluations: An Examination of the Spatial Arrangement Method as a Measure of Semantic Structure in Children","authors":"Catarina Vales,&nbsp;Zach Branson,&nbsp;Anna V. Fisher","doi":"10.1002/icd.2560","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2560","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Cognitive tasks are seldom evaluated on their ability to provide valid and reliable measurements of the construct they intend to measure. This scarcity of psychometric evaluations makes it challenging to evaluate replications of experimental effects and to relate performance in cognitive tasks to other constructs of interest. In developmental science, these issues are compounded by the often-imprecise measures derived from tasks completed by child participants. Here, we focus on the spatial arrangement method when used to assess semantic structure in children and evaluate its psychometric properties. Using a new analytic approach to capture individual variability in participants' arrangement in this task, we show that the spatial arrangement method has appropriate construct validity (<i>β</i> = 0.40), internal consistency (<i>r</i><sup>2</sup> = 0.20), and test–retest reliability (<i>r</i><sup>2</sup> = 0.41; ICC = 0.56) when used to evaluate semantic structure in U.S. children (4–9 years of age; <i>N</i> = 200 across 4 datasets). We discuss the implications of these findings for examining semantic structure in children and for strengthening methodological practices in developmental science more broadly.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2560","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142841939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Comparison of Sleep and Settle Behaviours Across Twins and Singletons at 5 Months of Age 5个月大的双胞胎和单胎婴儿睡眠和安定行为的比较
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2564
Charlotte Viktorsson, Angelica Ronald, Terje Falck-Ytter

Twin studies are important for research on genetic and environmental influences on child development, but it is imperative to test whether findings can be generalised from twins to singletons. Since the first months of life are defined by the emergence of important sleep behaviours, we compared 451 (54.8% females) twins and 77 singletons (48.1% females) on a range of sleep, settle and crying behaviours at 5 months of age. No significant differences were found regarding duration of crying or time until settled. However, singletons were reported to wake up more frequently during nighttime than twins (F(1, 509) = 35.10, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.065), suggesting that, when reported in twin studies, this measure might be slightly underestimated at a mean level in relation to singletons. In conclusion, despite the unique challenges and additional caregiver load of infant twins, there seem to be few differences between twins and singletons regarding parent-reported settle and crying behaviours in early infancy.

双胞胎研究对于研究遗传和环境对儿童发育的影响非常重要,但当务之急是检验研究结果能否从双胞胎推广到单胎。由于出生后的头几个月是重要睡眠行为出现的时期,因此我们对 451 对双胞胎(54.8% 为女性)和 77 对单胎(48.1% 为女性)在 5 个月大时的一系列睡眠、安顿和哭闹行为进行了比较。结果发现,双胞胎在哭闹持续时间或安顿时间方面没有明显差异。不过,据报道,单胎婴儿夜间醒来的次数比双胞胎婴儿多(F(1,509)= 35.10,p < 0.001,ηp2 = 0.065),这表明在双胞胎研究中,这一指标的平均水平可能会略微低估单胎婴儿。总之,尽管双胞胎婴儿面临着独特的挑战和额外的照顾负担,但在父母报告的婴儿早期定居和哭闹行为方面,双胞胎和单胎婴儿之间似乎没有什么差异。
{"title":"A Comparison of Sleep and Settle Behaviours Across Twins and Singletons at 5 Months of Age","authors":"Charlotte Viktorsson,&nbsp;Angelica Ronald,&nbsp;Terje Falck-Ytter","doi":"10.1002/icd.2564","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2564","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Twin studies are important for research on genetic and environmental influences on child development, but it is imperative to test whether findings can be generalised from twins to singletons. Since the first months of life are defined by the emergence of important sleep behaviours, we compared 451 (54.8% females) twins and 77 singletons (48.1% females) on a range of sleep, settle and crying behaviours at 5 months of age. No significant differences were found regarding duration of crying or time until settled. However, singletons were reported to wake up more frequently during nighttime than twins (<i>F</i>(1, 509) = 35.10, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001, <i>η</i><sub><i>p</i></sub><sup>2</sup> = 0.065), suggesting that, when reported in twin studies, this measure might be slightly underestimated at a mean level in relation to singletons. In conclusion, despite the unique challenges and additional caregiver load of infant twins, there seem to be few differences between twins and singletons regarding parent-reported settle and crying behaviours in early infancy.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2564","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142820662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant and Child Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1