Reassessing the role of language dominance in n-2 language repetition costs as a marker of inhibition in multilingual language switching.

IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI:10.1037/xlm0001333
Iring Koch, Mathieu Declerck, Greta Petersen, Daniel Rister, Wolfgang Scharke, Andrea M Philipp
{"title":"Reassessing the role of language dominance in n-2 language repetition costs as a marker of inhibition in multilingual language switching.","authors":"Iring Koch, Mathieu Declerck, Greta Petersen, Daniel Rister, Wolfgang Scharke, Andrea M Philipp","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001333","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Speaking two or more languages shows bilingual flexibility, but flexible switching requires language control and often incurs performance costs. We examined inhibitory control assessing <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs when switching three languages (L1 [German], L2 [English], L3 [French]). These costs denote worse performance in <i>n</i>-2 repetitions (e.g., L2-L3-L2) than in <i>n</i>-2 nonrepetitions (e.g., L1-L3-L2), indicating persisting inhibition. In two experiments (<i>n</i> = 28 in Experiment 1; <i>n</i> = 44 in Experiment 2), <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs were observed, but only for L2. Looking into L2 trials specifically, we found <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs when switching back to L2 from the still weaker L3 but not when returning from the stronger L1, suggesting that L2 is a strong competitor for L3 (requiring L2 inhibition) but less so for L1. Finding <i>n</i>-2 repetition costs supports an inhibitory account of language control in general, but our study shows only partial evidence for the theoretically assumed more specific relation between language dominance and language inhibition (i.e., only for dominance relations with respect to L1 and L3 when switching back to L2). Taken together, the findings thus suggest the need for further refinement of the concept of language dominance and its relation to inhibition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2000,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001333","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"2024/2/22 0:00:00","PubModel":"Epub","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Speaking two or more languages shows bilingual flexibility, but flexible switching requires language control and often incurs performance costs. We examined inhibitory control assessing n-2 repetition costs when switching three languages (L1 [German], L2 [English], L3 [French]). These costs denote worse performance in n-2 repetitions (e.g., L2-L3-L2) than in n-2 nonrepetitions (e.g., L1-L3-L2), indicating persisting inhibition. In two experiments (n = 28 in Experiment 1; n = 44 in Experiment 2), n-2 repetition costs were observed, but only for L2. Looking into L2 trials specifically, we found n-2 repetition costs when switching back to L2 from the still weaker L3 but not when returning from the stronger L1, suggesting that L2 is a strong competitor for L3 (requiring L2 inhibition) but less so for L1. Finding n-2 repetition costs supports an inhibitory account of language control in general, but our study shows only partial evidence for the theoretically assumed more specific relation between language dominance and language inhibition (i.e., only for dominance relations with respect to L1 and L3 when switching back to L2). Taken together, the findings thus suggest the need for further refinement of the concept of language dominance and its relation to inhibition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
重新评估语言优势在 n-2 语言重复成本中的作用,将其作为多语言转换中的抑制标记。
会说两种或两种以上语言显示了双语的灵活性,但灵活切换需要语言控制,而且往往会产生表现成本。我们研究了抑制控制,评估了转换三种语言(L1 [德语]、L2 [英语]、L3 [法语])时的 n-2 重复成本。这些代价表明,n-2 次重复(如 L2-L3-L2)的成绩比 n-2 次不重复(如 L1-L3-L2)的成绩差,这表明抑制作用持续存在。在两个实验中(实验 1,n = 28;实验 2,n = 44),我们观察到了 n-2 重复成本,但仅针对 L2。具体到 L2 试验,我们发现从仍然较弱的 L3 切换回 L2 时有 n-2 的重复成本,而从较强的 L1 切换回 L2 时则没有,这表明 L2 是 L3 的强竞争者(需要 L2 抑制),而 L1 的竞争者则较少。n-2重复成本的发现支持了语言控制的一般抑制性解释,但我们的研究只部分证明了理论上假设的语言优势和语言抑制之间更具体的关系(即只证明了切换回L2时L1和L3的优势关系)。综上所述,研究结果表明,有必要进一步完善语言优势的概念及其与语言抑制的关系。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
4.30
自引率
3.80%
发文量
163
审稿时长
4-8 weeks
期刊介绍: The Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition publishes studies on perception, control of action, perceptual aspects of language processing, and related cognitive processes.
期刊最新文献
Direct and indirect effects of fluid intelligence on the retrieval practice effect. Delayed testing in directed forgetting dissociates active and passive forms of forgetting. Judgments of learning (JOLs) impact item memory but not source memory: Insights into JOL reactivity using a multinomial model. Mapping the reliability multiverse of contextual cuing. Modulations of response activation contribute to block-wide control: Evidence from proportion congruency effects in the prime-probe task.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1