首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Marking prosodic prominence for voice assistant and human addressees. 为语音助手和人类受话人标注前音突出度
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001396
Eleonora Beier, Michelle Cohn, Timothy Trammel, Fernanda Ferreira, Georgia Zellou

Prosodic prominence (realized with phonetic features such as increased intensity, duration, and pitch, among others) is thought to guide listeners' attention by focusing new information. This study investigates production and perception of prosodic prominence toward two types of addressees: a human and a voice assistant interlocutor. We examine how the language system adapts to this increasingly common technology, by testing whether prosodic prominence is subject to audience design when addressing an interlocutor that is consistently rated as having less communicative ability. Stimuli consisted of question-answer pairs, where California English speakers read identical sentences (e.g., "Jude saw the sun") in response to interlocutors' questions probing different foci (e.g., "Who saw the sun?"). Experiment 1 reveals consistent acoustic adjustments to mark focus on either the subject or the object of a sentence. In Experiment 2, we find that listeners reliably infer the intended information structure based on these acoustic adjustments. Across both experiments, we see no consistent difference in focus marking by type of interlocutor (human vs. voice assistant). Nonetheless, listeners associate particular features (slower speech rate) with speech directed at voice assistants. Taken together, our findings suggest that while speakers apply communicative strategies from human-human interaction when addressing voice assistants, listeners expect a device-specific register. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们认为,前音突出(通过增加强度、持续时间和音高等语音特征来实现)可以通过集中新信息来引导听者的注意力。本研究调查了两种类型的受话者对前音突出的产生和感知:人类和语音助理对话者。我们检验了语言系统如何适应这种日益普遍的技术,并测试了在向一直被评为交际能力较弱的对话者说话时,前音突出是否受受众设计的影响。刺激物包括问答对,加州英语使用者在回答对话者提出的不同焦点问题(如 "谁看到了太阳")时,要朗读相同的句子(如 "Jude saw the sun")。实验 1 显示了一致的声学调整,以标记句子中的主语或宾语。在实验 2 中,我们发现听者能根据这些声音调整可靠地推断出预期的信息结构。在这两项实验中,我们发现不同类型的对话者(人类与语音助手)在重点标记方面没有一致的差异。然而,听者会将特定的特征(语速较慢)与针对语音助手的语音联系起来。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,虽然说话者在对语音助手讲话时采用了人与人之间的交流策略,但听者却期望使用特定设备的语域。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Marking prosodic prominence for voice assistant and human addressees.","authors":"Eleonora Beier, Michelle Cohn, Timothy Trammel, Fernanda Ferreira, Georgia Zellou","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001396","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001396","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prosodic prominence (realized with phonetic features such as increased intensity, duration, and pitch, among others) is thought to guide listeners' attention by focusing new information. This study investigates production and perception of prosodic prominence toward two types of addressees: a human and a voice assistant interlocutor. We examine how the language system adapts to this increasingly common technology, by testing whether prosodic prominence is subject to <i>audience design</i> when addressing an interlocutor that is consistently rated as having less communicative ability. Stimuli consisted of question-answer pairs, where California English speakers read identical sentences (e.g., \"Jude saw the sun\") in response to interlocutors' questions probing different foci (e.g., \"Who saw the sun?\"). Experiment 1 reveals consistent acoustic adjustments to mark focus on either the subject or the object of a sentence. In Experiment 2, we find that listeners reliably infer the intended information structure based on these acoustic adjustments. Across both experiments, we see no consistent difference in focus marking by type of interlocutor (human vs. voice assistant). Nonetheless, listeners associate particular features (slower speech rate) with speech directed at voice assistants. Taken together, our findings suggest that while speakers apply communicative strategies from human-human interaction when addressing voice assistants, listeners expect a device-specific register. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142649418","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direct and indirect effects of fluid intelligence on the retrieval practice effect. 流体智力对检索练习效应的直接和间接影响。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001401
Marcos Felipe Rodrigues de Lima, Luciano Grüdtner Buratto

In this study, participants (N = 144) first studied 40 word pairs, then restudied half of the word pairs and practiced retrieval with feedback on the other half. In separate sessions, they then completed cued-recall and fluid intelligence (gF) tests. Three main objectives were addressed. First, we sought to generalize two findings reported by M. Minear et al. (2018): During the final-test phase, the high gF group exhibited a greater retrieval practice effect for difficult items compared to easy items, while the opposite pattern was observed for the low gF group; and, during the practice phase, the advantage of the high gF group over the low gF group increased across cycles for difficult items but not for easy items. Overall, we successfully extended their results. Second, we investigated whether gF is related to the amount of new items recalled during the practice phase. Consistent positive relationships were found in Cycles 1-3 (rs between .30 and .43). Third, we tested and found an indirect effect of gF on the retrieval practice effect mediated by performance during the practice phase. One possibility is that learners with higher gF may be particularly skilled at generating effective mediators and at monitoring and replacing less effective ones after retrieval failures. We recommend the following research agenda: measure the production, shift, and retrieval of mediators; manipulate the number of retrieval practice opportunities; probe the retrieval practice effect with free-recall tests; and adopt procedures based on learning to a criterion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在这项研究中,参与者(144 人)首先学习了 40 个词对,然后重新学习了一半的词对,并在另一半词对的反馈下进行了检索练习。然后,他们在不同的环节中分别完成了提示记忆和流体智力(gF)测试。我们的目标主要有三个。首先,我们试图推广 M. Minear 等人(2018 年)报告的两项发现:在最终测试阶段,与简单项目相比,高gF组在困难项目上表现出更大的检索练习效应,而低gF组则观察到相反的模式;在练习阶段,高gF组与低gF组相比,在困难项目上的优势在不同周期都会增加,但在简单项目上则不会。总之,我们成功地扩展了他们的结果。其次,我们研究了 gF 是否与练习阶段回忆的新项目数量有关。在第 1-3 周期中发现了一致的正相关关系(rs 在 0.30 和 0.43 之间)。第三,我们测试并发现了 gF 对检索练习效果的间接影响,这种影响是由练习阶段的成绩所中介的。一种可能性是,gF 较高的学习者可能特别善于产生有效的中介物,并在检索失败后监控和替换效果较差的中介物。我们建议以下研究议程:测量中介物的产生、转移和检索;操纵检索练习机会的数量;通过自由回忆测试探究检索练习效应;以及采用基于学习标准的程序。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Direct and indirect effects of fluid intelligence on the retrieval practice effect.","authors":"Marcos Felipe Rodrigues de Lima, Luciano Grüdtner Buratto","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001401","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001401","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, participants (<i>N</i> = 144) first studied 40 word pairs, then restudied half of the word pairs and practiced retrieval with feedback on the other half. In separate sessions, they then completed cued-recall and fluid intelligence (gF) tests. Three main objectives were addressed. First, we sought to generalize two findings reported by M. Minear et al. (2018): During the final-test phase, the high gF group exhibited a greater retrieval practice effect for difficult items compared to easy items, while the opposite pattern was observed for the low gF group; and, during the practice phase, the advantage of the high gF group over the low gF group increased across cycles for difficult items but not for easy items. Overall, we successfully extended their results. Second, we investigated whether gF is related to the amount of new items recalled during the practice phase. Consistent positive relationships were found in Cycles 1-3 (<i>r</i>s between .30 and .43). Third, we tested and found an indirect effect of gF on the retrieval practice effect mediated by performance during the practice phase. One possibility is that learners with higher gF may be particularly skilled at generating effective mediators and at monitoring and replacing less effective ones after retrieval failures. We recommend the following research agenda: measure the production, shift, and retrieval of mediators; manipulate the number of retrieval practice opportunities; probe the retrieval practice effect with free-recall tests; and adopt procedures based on learning to a criterion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142631795","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When confidence reveals more than recognition performance does: The case of context load. 当信心比识别成绩更能说明问题时:语境负荷案例
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001391
Oliwia Zaborowska, Beatrice G Kuhlmann, Katarzyna Zawadzka, Maciej Hanczakowski

Context in which events are embedded is often hypothesized to serve as an independent cue for retrieval. This means that any effects of context need to obey two basic principles of cue-dependent memory: Memory retrieval should be augmented when, first, encoding context is reinstated and, second, this context uniquely specifies individual items stored in memory. Both of these regularities are well supported for recall tests, but they remain contentious in recognition tests. Here, in three experiments, we assess whether unique and nonunique contexts affect memory processes when reinstated during recognition. However, rather than focusing on measures of recognition performance, we looked at confidence judgments collected during recognition that should be particularly sensitive to recollective effects resulting from context cuing. Experiments 1 and 2, using old/new and forced-choice recognition tests, respectively, documented positive effects of context reinstatement on confidence in correct recognition identifications, but only for contexts uniquely associated with individual items. These effects emerged even when there were no reliable context effects in recognition performance measures. Experiment 3 showed the same effect of context reinstatement, moderated by context load, when spontaneous recognition of a previous study episode occurred during restudy. These results demonstrate the role of context as an independent retrieval cue both in deliberate and spontaneous recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

嵌入事件的语境通常被假定为检索的独立线索。这意味着语境的任何影响都需要遵守线索依赖记忆的两个基本原则:首先,当编码情境被恢复时,记忆检索应该会增强;其次,当这种情境唯一指定了存储在记忆中的单个项目时,记忆检索应该会增强。这两个规律在回忆测试中都得到了很好的支持,但在识别测试中却仍然存在争议。在这里,我们通过三个实验来评估独特和非独特的语境在识别过程中被恢复时是否会影响记忆过程。不过,我们并没有把重点放在识别成绩的测量上,而是研究了在识别过程中收集到的信心判断,这种判断应该对情境诱导导致的回忆效应特别敏感。实验一和实验二分别使用了新旧识别测试和强迫选择识别测试,结果表明情境恢复对正确识别的信心有积极影响,但仅限于与单个项目唯一相关的情境。即使在识别成绩测量中没有可靠的情境效应时,这些效应也会出现。实验 3 显示,当在复习过程中自发识别以前的学习情节时,情境恢复也会产生同样的效应,但会受到情境负荷的调节。这些结果证明了情境在有意识别和自发识别中作为独立检索线索的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"When confidence reveals more than recognition performance does: The case of context load.","authors":"Oliwia Zaborowska, Beatrice G Kuhlmann, Katarzyna Zawadzka, Maciej Hanczakowski","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001391","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001391","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Context in which events are embedded is often hypothesized to serve as an independent cue for retrieval. This means that any effects of context need to obey two basic principles of cue-dependent memory: Memory retrieval should be augmented when, first, encoding context is reinstated and, second, this context uniquely specifies individual items stored in memory. Both of these regularities are well supported for recall tests, but they remain contentious in recognition tests. Here, in three experiments, we assess whether unique and nonunique contexts affect memory processes when reinstated during recognition. However, rather than focusing on measures of recognition performance, we looked at confidence judgments collected during recognition that should be particularly sensitive to recollective effects resulting from context cuing. Experiments 1 and 2, using old/new and forced-choice recognition tests, respectively, documented positive effects of context reinstatement on confidence in correct recognition identifications, but only for contexts uniquely associated with individual items. These effects emerged even when there were no reliable context effects in recognition performance measures. Experiment 3 showed the same effect of context reinstatement, moderated by context load, when spontaneous recognition of a previous study episode occurred during restudy. These results demonstrate the role of context as an independent retrieval cue both in deliberate and spontaneous recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1722-1739"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394847","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bilingual parafoveal processing: Children and adults preprocess orthographic information of the upcoming word during sentence reading in their first and second language. 双语视网膜旁处理:儿童和成人在用第一语言和第二语言阅读句子时,对即将出现的单词的正字法信息进行预处理。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001346
Simon P Tiffin-Richards

Readers of different ages and across different languages routinely process information of upcoming words in a sentence, before their eyes move to fixate them directly (parafoveal processing). However, there is inconsistent evidence of similar parafoveal processing in a reader's second language (L2). In this eye movement study, the gaze-contingent boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975a) was used to test whether parafoveal processing of orthographic information is an integral part of both beginning and proficient L2 reading. The eye movements of beginning L2-learners (n = 53, aged 11-14 years) and highly proficient L2-users (n = 56, aged 19-65 years) were recorded while they read sentences in their first language (L1) German and L2 English. Sentences each contained a cognate target word (e.g., English: tunnel, German: Tunnel). The parafoveal preview of the targets either (a) preserved the spelling and meaning of the target (identity condition), (b) preserved letter identities but transposed the position of two adjacent letters (transposed-letter [TL] condition, e.g., tunenl/Tunenl), or substituted the identity of two adjacent letters (substituted-letter condition, e.g., tunocl/Tunocl). TL previews elicited longer early first-pass reading times than identity previews in both L1 and L2 reading in children and adults, suggesting that letter position was processed parafoveally. Substituted-letter previews resulted in longer reading times than TL previews in children and adults in L1 and L2, suggesting that letter identity information was processed independently of position information. These results suggest that letter position and identity information are extracted from the parafovea during L1 and L2 reading, facilitating word recognition in children and adults. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

不同年龄段和不同语言的读者在眼睛直接注视句子中即将出现的单词之前,都会对这些单词的信息进行常规处理(视网膜旁处理)。然而,在读者的第二语言(L2)中也存在类似的视线旁处理,但证据并不一致。在这项眼动研究中,我们采用了凝视--权变边界范式(Rayner,1975a)来测试正字法信息的视线旁处理是否是初学和精通第二语言阅读不可或缺的一部分。研究人员记录了初学 L2 学习者(n = 53,年龄 11-14 岁)和精通 L2 学习者(n = 56,年龄 19-65 岁)在阅读第一语言(L1)德语和第二语言英语句子时的眼球运动。每个句子都包含一个同义目标词(例如,英语:隧道,德语:隧道)。目标词的视网膜旁预览要么(a)保留目标词的拼写和含义(同一性条件),要么(b)保留字母同一性,但调换两个相邻字母的位置(调换字母[TL]条件,如 tunenl/Tunenl),要么(调换字母条件,如 tunocl/Tunocl)替换两个相邻字母的同一性。在儿童和成人的 L1 和 L2 阅读中,TL 预览比同一性预览引起的早期一读时间更长,这表明字母位置是在视网膜旁处理的。在儿童和成人的 L1 和 L2 阅读中,替换字母预览比 TL 预览的阅读时间更长,这表明字母身份信息的处理与位置信息无关。这些结果表明,在 L1 和 L2 阅读过程中,字母位置和特征信息是从视网膜旁提取的,这有助于儿童和成人的单词识别。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Bilingual parafoveal processing: Children and adults preprocess orthographic information of the upcoming word during sentence reading in their first and second language.","authors":"Simon P Tiffin-Richards","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001346","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001346","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Readers of different ages and across different languages routinely process information of upcoming words in a sentence, before their eyes move to fixate them directly (parafoveal processing). However, there is inconsistent evidence of similar parafoveal processing in a reader's second language (L2). In this eye movement study, the gaze-contingent boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975a) was used to test whether parafoveal processing of orthographic information is an integral part of both beginning and proficient L2 reading. The eye movements of beginning L2-learners (<i>n</i> = 53, aged 11-14 years) and highly proficient L2-users (<i>n</i> = 56, aged 19-65 years) were recorded while they read sentences in their first language (L1) German and L2 English. Sentences each contained a cognate target word (e.g., English: tunnel, German: Tunnel). The parafoveal preview of the targets either (a) preserved the spelling and meaning of the target (identity condition), (b) preserved letter identities but transposed the position of two adjacent letters (transposed-letter [TL] condition, e.g., tunenl/Tunenl), or substituted the identity of two adjacent letters (substituted-letter condition, e.g., tunocl/Tunocl). TL previews elicited longer early first-pass reading times than identity previews in both L1 and L2 reading in children and adults, suggesting that letter position was processed parafoveally. Substituted-letter previews resulted in longer reading times than TL previews in children and adults in L1 and L2, suggesting that letter identity information was processed independently of position information. These results suggest that letter position and identity information are extracted from the parafovea during L1 and L2 reading, facilitating word recognition in children and adults. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1844-1861"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140854406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of face race on metamemory: Examining its robustness and underlying mechanisms. 人脸竞赛对元记忆的影响:研究其稳健性和内在机制
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001392
Tomás A Palma, Alexandre Vieira, Francisco Cruz, André Mata

Perceivers typically exhibit better recognition memory for same-race faces than for cross-race faces, a phenomenon known as the cross-race effect (CRE). Despite its ubiquity, it is yet unclear whether people are metacognitively aware of the CRE. This research thoroughly investigates perceivers' metacognitive awareness of the CRE across five experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that both prospective (judgments of learning) and retrospective (confidence) metamemory judgments are sensitive to variations in the racial category and prototypicality of faces. Experiment 3 showed that participants' item-level prospective judgments are informed by beliefs about the impact of face race on memory performance. Experiment 4 revealed that global predictions are influenced by face race in the absence of direct stimulus experience, emphasizing the role of preexisting beliefs. Experiment 5 extended these findings by showing large crossover interactions between face race and participant race in both global predictions and item-level prospective judgments, indicating that both White and Black participants have higher metamemory estimates for ingroup faces. This experiment further showed that preexisting beliefs intensify the impact of face race on metamemory judgments yet do not fully account for it. Collectively, these experiments provide robust evidence of good metamemory accuracy for faces varying in racial categories and prototypicality among White participants and demonstrate that beliefs underlie the effect of face race on metamemory judgments among both White and Black participants, though this may not be the only mechanism involved. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

知觉者对同种族面孔的识别记忆通常优于对跨种族面孔的识别记忆,这种现象被称为跨种族效应(CRE)。尽管跨种族效应无处不在,但人们是否在元认知上意识到跨种族效应尚不清楚。本研究通过五项实验深入调查了感知者对 CRE 的元认知意识。实验 1 和 2 表明,前瞻性(学习判断)和回顾性(信心)元认知判断对人脸的种族类别和原型的变化都很敏感。实验 3 表明,受试者的项目级前瞻性判断受到了关于人脸种族对记忆表现影响的信念的影响。实验 4 显示,在没有直接刺激经验的情况下,全局预测会受到人脸种族的影响,这强调了预先存在的信念的作用。实验 5 扩展了这些发现,在全局预测和项目层面的前瞻性判断中,人脸种族和参与者种族之间都有很大的交叉互动,表明白人和黑人参与者对内群体人脸都有较高的元记忆估计。该实验进一步表明,预先存在的信念会加强人脸种族对元记忆判断的影响,但并不能完全解释这种影响。总之,这些实验提供了有力的证据,证明白人参与者对不同种族类别和原型性的面孔具有良好的元记忆准确性,并证明信念是面孔种族对白人和黑人参与者元记忆判断影响的基础,尽管这可能不是唯一的机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effect of face race on metamemory: Examining its robustness and underlying mechanisms.","authors":"Tomás A Palma, Alexandre Vieira, Francisco Cruz, André Mata","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001392","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001392","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceivers typically exhibit better recognition memory for same-race faces than for cross-race faces, a phenomenon known as the cross-race effect (CRE). Despite its ubiquity, it is yet unclear whether people are metacognitively aware of the CRE. This research thoroughly investigates perceivers' metacognitive awareness of the CRE across five experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that both prospective (judgments of learning) and retrospective (confidence) metamemory judgments are sensitive to variations in the racial category and prototypicality of faces. Experiment 3 showed that participants' item-level prospective judgments are informed by beliefs about the impact of face race on memory performance. Experiment 4 revealed that global predictions are influenced by face race in the absence of direct stimulus experience, emphasizing the role of preexisting beliefs. Experiment 5 extended these findings by showing large crossover interactions between face race and participant race in both global predictions and item-level prospective judgments, indicating that both White and Black participants have higher metamemory estimates for ingroup faces. This experiment further showed that preexisting beliefs intensify the impact of face race on metamemory judgments yet do not fully account for it. Collectively, these experiments provide robust evidence of good metamemory accuracy for faces varying in racial categories and prototypicality among White participants and demonstrate that beliefs underlie the effect of face race on metamemory judgments among both White and Black participants, though this may not be the only mechanism involved. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1811-1843"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142114321","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A grain of truth in the grain size effect: Retrieval practice is more effective when interspersed during learning. 粒度效应中的一粒真理:在学习过程中穿插检索练习更有效。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001382
Hilary J Don, Shaun Boustani, Chunliang Yang, David R Shanks

Retrieval practice is a powerful method for consolidating long-term learning. When learning takes place over an extended period, how should tests be scheduled to obtain the maximal benefit? In an end-test schedule, all material is studied prior to a large practice test on all studied material, whereas in an interim test schedule, learning is divided into multiple study/test cycles in which each test is smaller and only assesses material from the preceding study block. Past investigations have generally found a difference between these schedules during practice but not during a final assessment, although they may have been underpowered. Five experiments confirmed that final assessment performance was better in students taught using interim than end tests in list (Experiments 1, 2, and 5) and paired associate (Experiments 3 and 4) learning, with a meta-analysis of all available studies (k = 19) yielding a small- to medium-sized effect, g = 0.25, 95% confidence interval [0.09, 0.42]. Experiment 5 finds that the higher level of practice retrieval success in interim tests contributes to the grain size effect, but the effect is eliminated if these tests are too easy. Additional analyses also suggest that the forward testing effect, in which tests promote subsequent learning, may be a major cause of the grain size effect. The practical and theoretical implications of these demonstrations of robust grain size effects are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

复述练习是巩固长期学习成果的有效方法。当学习持续较长时间时,应如何安排测试才能获得最大收益?在终结测试计划中,在对所有学习材料进行一次大型练习测试之前,要对所有材料进行学习;而在中期测试计划中,学习被分成多个学习/测试周期,其中每次测试的规模较小,只对前一个学习阶段的材料进行评估。过去的调查一般都发现,在练习过程中,这两种计划之间存在差异,但在最终评估过程中却没有发现差异,尽管这些调查可能力量不足。五项实验证实,在列表学习(实验 1、2 和 5)和配对联想学习(实验 3 和 4)中,使用中期测试而非期末测试教学的学生的期末评估成绩更好,对所有可用研究(k = 19)的荟萃分析得出了中小型效应,g = 0.25,95% 置信区间[0.09, 0.42]。实验 5 发现,临时测试中较高的练习检索成功率有助于粒度效应的产生,但如果这些测试过于简单,这种效应就会消失。其他分析还表明,前向测试效应(测试促进后续学习)可能是粒度效应的主要原因。本文讨论了这些稳健粒度效应的实践和理论意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A grain of truth in the grain size effect: Retrieval practice is more effective when interspersed during learning.","authors":"Hilary J Don, Shaun Boustani, Chunliang Yang, David R Shanks","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001382","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001382","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Retrieval practice is a powerful method for consolidating long-term learning. When learning takes place over an extended period, how should tests be scheduled to obtain the maximal benefit? In an end-test schedule, all material is studied prior to a large practice test on all studied material, whereas in an interim test schedule, learning is divided into multiple study/test cycles in which each test is smaller and only assesses material from the preceding study block. Past investigations have generally found a difference between these schedules during practice but not during a final assessment, although they may have been underpowered. Five experiments confirmed that final assessment performance was better in students taught using interim than end tests in list (Experiments 1, 2, and 5) and paired associate (Experiments 3 and 4) learning, with a meta-analysis of all available studies (k = 19) yielding a small- to medium-sized effect, g = 0.25, 95% confidence interval [0.09, 0.42]. Experiment 5 finds that the higher level of practice retrieval success in interim tests contributes to the grain size effect, but the effect is eliminated if these tests are too easy. Additional analyses also suggest that the forward testing effect, in which tests promote subsequent learning, may be a major cause of the grain size effect. The practical and theoretical implications of these demonstrations of robust grain size effects are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"50 11","pages":"1791-1810"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142649420","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direction-specific reading experience shapes perceptual span. 特定方向的阅读经验塑造了知觉跨度。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001340
Ming Yan, Reinhold Kliegl, Jinger Pan

Perceptual span in reading, the spatial extent for effective information extraction during a single fixation, provides a critical foundation to all studies for sentence reading. However, it is not understood fully how the perceptual span is influenced by direction-specific reading experience. Traditional Chinese sentences can be written horizontally from left to right or vertically downward, offering the best opportunity to explore readers' perceptual span in different text directions, free of possible confounding with language proficiency and cross-participant differences. Using a within-item and within-subject design, eye movements of traditional Chinese readers were recorded during their reading of horizontally and vertically presented sentences. Additionally, regardless of text direction, a gaze-contingent moving-window technique was adopted to restrict visible texts within a virtual window that moved in synchrony with the reader's eye gaze, while characters outside the window were masked. Among several critical results, most importantly, asymptotic reading performance was observed in a smaller window condition for vertical reading than for horizontal reading, suggesting an overall smaller perceptual span in the former case. In addition, the size of the vertical perceptual span increased as a function of the readers' familiarity with vertical text. We conclude that factors beyond orthographic complexity and readers' language proficiency can influence the way in which humans read. Readers' direction-specific perceptual experiences can influence their perceptual span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

阅读中的感知跨度是指在单次定影过程中有效提取信息的空间范围,它为所有句子阅读研究提供了重要基础。然而,感知跨度如何受到特定方向阅读经验的影响,目前尚不完全清楚。繁体中文句子可以从左到右横向书写,也可以从上到下纵向书写,这为研究读者在不同文字方向上的感知跨度提供了最佳机会,同时也避免了语言能力和跨参与者差异可能造成的混淆。通过项目内和被试内设计,我们记录了繁体中文读者在阅读水平和垂直呈现的句子时的眼球运动。此外,无论文字的方向如何,我们都采用了一种凝视视变移动窗口技术,将可见文字限制在一个虚拟窗口内,该窗口会随着读者眼睛的凝视同步移动,而窗口外的字符则被遮挡。在几项关键结果中,最重要的是,与水平阅读相比,垂直阅读的窗口条件更小,这表明前者的总体感知跨度更小。此外,垂直感知跨度的大小随读者对垂直文本的熟悉程度而增加。我们的结论是,除了正字法的复杂性和读者的语言能力之外,其他因素也会影响人类的阅读方式。读者对特定方向的感知经验会影响他们的感知跨度。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Direction-specific reading experience shapes perceptual span.","authors":"Ming Yan, Reinhold Kliegl, Jinger Pan","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001340","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001340","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceptual span in reading, the spatial extent for effective information extraction during a single fixation, provides a critical foundation to all studies for sentence reading. However, it is not understood fully how the perceptual span is influenced by direction-specific reading experience. Traditional Chinese sentences can be written horizontally from left to right or vertically downward, offering the best opportunity to explore readers' perceptual span in different text directions, free of possible confounding with language proficiency and cross-participant differences. Using a within-item and within-subject design, eye movements of traditional Chinese readers were recorded during their reading of horizontally and vertically presented sentences. Additionally, regardless of text direction, a gaze-contingent moving-window technique was adopted to restrict visible texts within a virtual window that moved in synchrony with the reader's eye gaze, while characters outside the window were masked. Among several critical results, most importantly, asymptotic reading performance was observed in a smaller window condition for vertical reading than for horizontal reading, suggesting an overall smaller perceptual span in the former case. In addition, the size of the vertical perceptual span increased as a function of the readers' familiarity with vertical text. We conclude that factors beyond orthographic complexity and readers' language proficiency can influence the way in which humans read. Readers' direction-specific perceptual experiences can influence their perceptual span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1740-1748"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140856790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Saccadic targeting in the Landolt-C task: Implications for Chinese reading. Landolt-C任务中的游标瞄准:对中文阅读的启示。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001343
Xinyi Xia, Qin Liu, Erik D Reichle, Yanping Liu

Participants in an eye-movement experiment performed a modified version of the Landolt-C paradigm (Williams & Pollatsek, 2007) to determine if there are preferred viewing locations when they searched for target squares embedded in linear arrays of spatially contiguous clusters of squares (i.e., sequences of one to four squares having missing segments of variable size and orientation). The results of this experiment indicate that, although the peaks of the single- and first-of-multiple-fixation landing-site distributions were respectively located near the centers and beginnings of the clusters, thereby replicating previous patterns that have been interpreted as evidence for the default saccadic-targeting hypothesis, the same dissociation was evident on nonclusters (i.e., arbitrarily defined regions of analysis). Furthermore, properties of the clusters (e.g., character number and gap size) influenced fixation durations and forward saccade length, suggesting that ongoing stimulus processing affects decisions about when and where (i.e., how far) to move the eyes. Finally, results of simulations using simple oculomotor-based, default-targeting, and dynamic-adjustment models indicated that the latter performed better than the other two, suggesting that the dynamic-adjustment strategy likely reflects the basic perceptual and motor constraints shared by a variety of visual tasks, rather than being specific to Chinese reading. The theoretical implications of these results for existing and future accounts of eye-movement control are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在一项眼动实验中,受试者进行了改进版的 Landolt-C 范式(Williams & Pollatsek, 2007),以确定当他们搜索嵌入空间连续方格簇线性阵列中的目标方格(即由一到四个方格组成的序列,具有大小和方向可变的缺失片段)时,是否存在偏好的观察位置。实验结果表明,虽然单次定点和多次定点着陆点分布的峰值分别位于方格簇的中心和起点附近,从而复制了之前被解释为默认囊回定点假说证据的模式,但同样的分离在非方格簇(即任意定义的分析区域)上也很明显。此外,簇的属性(如字符数和间隙大小)也会影响固定持续时间和前向囊状移动长度,这表明正在进行的刺激处理会影响关于何时何地(即移动多远)移动眼睛的决策。最后,使用简单的基于眼球运动的模型、默认目标模型和动态调整模型进行模拟的结果表明,后者的表现优于其他两种模型,这表明动态调整策略很可能反映了各种视觉任务所共有的基本知觉和运动限制,而不是中文阅读所特有的。本文讨论了这些结果对现有和未来眼动控制理论的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Saccadic targeting in the Landolt-C task: Implications for Chinese reading.","authors":"Xinyi Xia, Qin Liu, Erik D Reichle, Yanping Liu","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001343","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001343","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Participants in an eye-movement experiment performed a modified version of the Landolt-C paradigm (Williams & Pollatsek, 2007) to determine if there are preferred viewing locations when they searched for target squares embedded in linear arrays of spatially contiguous clusters of squares (i.e., sequences of one to four squares having missing segments of variable size and orientation). The results of this experiment indicate that, although the peaks of the single- and first-of-multiple-fixation landing-site distributions were respectively located near the centers and beginnings of the clusters, thereby replicating previous patterns that have been interpreted as evidence for the default saccadic-targeting hypothesis, the same dissociation was evident on nonclusters (i.e., arbitrarily defined regions of analysis). Furthermore, properties of the clusters (e.g., character number and gap size) influenced fixation durations and forward saccade length, suggesting that ongoing stimulus processing affects decisions about when and where (i.e., how far) to move the eyes. Finally, results of simulations using simple oculomotor-based, default-targeting, and dynamic-adjustment models indicated that the latter performed better than the other two, suggesting that the dynamic-adjustment strategy likely reflects the basic perceptual and motor constraints shared by a variety of visual tasks, rather than being specific to Chinese reading. The theoretical implications of these results for existing and future accounts of eye-movement control are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1749-1771"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867073","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changing-state irrelevant speech disrupts visual-verbal but not visual-spatial serial recall. 变化状态的无关语音会干扰视觉-语言序列记忆,但不会干扰视觉-空间序列记忆。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001360
John E Marsh, Mark J Hurlstone, Alexandre Marois, Linden J Ball, Stuart B Moore, François Vachon, Sabine J Schlittmeier, Jan Philipp Röer, Axel Buchner, Frederik Aust, Raoul Bell

In an influential article, Jones et al. (1995) provide evidence that auditory distraction by changing relative to repetitive auditory distracters (the changing-state effect) did not differ between a visual-verbal and visual-spatial serial recall task, providing evidence for an amodal mechanism for the representation of serial order in short-term memory that transcends modalities. This finding has been highly influential for theories of short-term memory and auditory distraction. However, evidence vis-à-vis the robustness of this result is sorely lacking. Here, two high-powered replications of Jones et al.'s (1995) crucial Experiment 4 were undertaken. In the first partial replication (n = 64), a fully within-participants design was adopted, wherein participants undertook both the visual-verbal and visual-spatial serial recall tasks under different irrelevant sound conditions, without a retention period. The second near-identical replication (n = 128), incorporated a retention period and implemented the task-modality manipulation as a between-participants factor, as per the original Jones et al. (1995; Experiment 4) study. In both experiments, the changing-state effect was observed for visual-verbal serial recall but not for visual-spatial serial recall. The results are consistent with modular and interference-based accounts of distraction and challenge some aspects of functional equivalence accounts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在一篇颇具影响力的文章中,Jones 等人(1995 年)提供了证据,证明相对于重复性听觉干扰物的变化所产生的听觉干扰(变化状态效应)在视觉-语言和视觉-空间序列回忆任务中并无差异,从而为超越模态的序列顺序在短时记忆中的表征的模态机制提供了证据。这一发现对短时记忆和听觉分心理论具有重大影响。然而,有关这一结果的稳健性的证据却非常缺乏。在此,我们对 Jones 等人(1995 年)的关键实验 4 进行了两次高功率复制。在第一个部分重复实验(n = 64)中,采用了完全的参与者内部设计,即参与者在不同的无关声音条件下进行视觉-语言和视觉-空间序列回忆任务,不设保持期。第二项几乎相同的重复实验(n = 128)则采用了保留期设计,并根据最初的 Jones 等人(1995 年;实验 4)研究,将任务-模式操纵作为参与者之间的因素。在这两项实验中,视觉-语言序列回忆都出现了变化状态效应,而视觉-空间序列回忆则没有。这些结果与分心的模块化和基于干扰的说法相一致,并对功能等效说法的某些方面提出了质疑。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Changing-state irrelevant speech disrupts visual-verbal but not visual-spatial serial recall.","authors":"John E Marsh, Mark J Hurlstone, Alexandre Marois, Linden J Ball, Stuart B Moore, François Vachon, Sabine J Schlittmeier, Jan Philipp Röer, Axel Buchner, Frederik Aust, Raoul Bell","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001360","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001360","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In an influential article, Jones et al. (1995) provide evidence that auditory distraction by changing relative to repetitive auditory distracters (the changing-state effect) did not differ between a visual-verbal and visual-spatial serial recall task, providing evidence for an amodal mechanism for the representation of serial order in short-term memory that transcends modalities. This finding has been highly influential for theories of short-term memory and auditory distraction. However, evidence vis-à-vis the robustness of this result is sorely lacking. Here, two high-powered replications of Jones et al.'s (1995) crucial Experiment 4 were undertaken. In the first partial replication (<i>n</i> = 64), a fully within-participants design was adopted, wherein participants undertook both the visual-verbal and visual-spatial serial recall tasks under different irrelevant sound conditions, without a retention period. The second near-identical replication (<i>n</i> = 128), incorporated a retention period and implemented the task-modality manipulation as a between-participants factor, as per the original Jones et al. (1995; Experiment 4) study. In both experiments, the changing-state effect was observed for visual-verbal serial recall but not for visual-spatial serial recall. The results are consistent with modular and interference-based accounts of distraction and challenge some aspects of functional equivalence accounts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1772-1790"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447460","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consonant beginnings and vowel endings lead to higher liking judgments. 辅音开头和元音结尾会导致更高的好感度判断。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-10 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001375
Ira Theresa Maschmann, Anita Körner, Sascha Topolinski

Across languages, syllables more likely begin with consonants (vs. vowels) and end with vowels (vs. consonants), so that words that follow (vs. do not follow) this pattern are more familiar. In six experiments (total N = 638), we investigated the influence of beginning and ending letters (vowels vs. consonants) of pseudowords on preferences. Pseudowords that begin with consonants (vs. vowels) were preferred; independently, pseudowords that end with vowels (vs. consonants) were also preferred. Both of these consonant-vowel-preference effects generalized across stimulus sets and across speakers of German and English (Experiments 1a-1c). Additionally, consistent with familiarity as the underlying mechanism, pseudowords with consonant (vs. vowel) beginnings and vowel (vs. consonant) endings were more frequently judged to be real words (Experiment 2). The word-ending effect-but surprisingly, not the word-beginning effect-generalized to auditory stimulus presentation (Experiments 3a-3b). Thus, we find that preferences for vowel (vs. consonant) at word endings are more robust than preferences for consonant (vs. vowel) at word beginnings. By showing that consonant-vowel structure systematically influences preferences, we demonstrate two new associations between word form and affective meaning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在各种语言中,音节更可能以辅音(相对于元音)开始,以元音(相对于辅音)结束,因此遵循(相对于不遵循)这种模式的词更容易被人熟悉。在六个实验中(总人数 = 638),我们研究了伪词的开头和结尾字母(元音与辅音)对偏好的影响。以辅音开头的伪词(相对于元音)更受青睐;另外,以元音结尾的伪词(相对于辅音)也更受青睐。这两种辅音-元音偏好效应在不同的刺激集以及不同的德语和英语使用者之间都有普遍性(实验 1a-1c)。此外,与熟悉效应作为基本机制相一致的是,辅音(相对于元音)开头和元音(相对于辅音)结尾的假词被更频繁地判定为真词(实验 2)。词尾效应--但令人惊讶的是,词头效应--在听觉刺激呈现时也普遍存在(实验 3a-3b)。因此,我们发现词尾的元音(相对于辅音)偏好比词首的辅音(相对于元音)偏好更强。通过证明辅音-元音结构对偏好的系统性影响,我们证明了词形与情感意义之间的两种新关联。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Consonant beginnings and vowel endings lead to higher liking judgments.","authors":"Ira Theresa Maschmann, Anita Körner, Sascha Topolinski","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001375","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001375","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across languages, syllables more likely begin with consonants (vs. vowels) and end with vowels (vs. consonants), so that words that follow (vs. do not follow) this pattern are more familiar. In six experiments (total <i>N</i> = 638), we investigated the influence of beginning and ending letters (vowels vs. consonants) of pseudowords on preferences. Pseudowords that begin with consonants (vs. vowels) were preferred; independently, pseudowords that end with vowels (vs. consonants) were also preferred. Both of these consonant-vowel-preference effects generalized across stimulus sets and across speakers of German and English (Experiments 1a-1c). Additionally, consistent with familiarity as the underlying mechanism, pseudowords with consonant (vs. vowel) beginnings and vowel (vs. consonant) endings were more frequently judged to be real words (Experiment 2). The word-ending effect-but surprisingly, not the word-beginning effect-generalized to auditory stimulus presentation (Experiments 3a-3b). Thus, we find that preferences for vowel (vs. consonant) at word endings are more robust than preferences for consonant (vs. vowel) at word beginnings. By showing that consonant-vowel structure systematically influences preferences, we demonstrate two new associations between word form and affective meaning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1862-1873"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479507","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1