Minding the (achievement) gap

IF 2.3 3区 管理学 Q2 ECONOMICS Journal of Policy Analysis and Management Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI:10.1002/pam.22577
Diane W. Schanzenbach
{"title":"Minding the (achievement) gap","authors":"Diane W. Schanzenbach","doi":"10.1002/pam.22577","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Fundamentally, Reeves and I agree about the importance of boys’ educational under-achievement and the need to openly discuss and address it. I emphasize that when boys fail to thrive in school, it has downstream consequences not only for their own lives but for our nation's economic growth. Further, boys’ success need not come at the expense of girls’ success. This is not zero-sum; we all benefit when children reach their potential.</p><p>I must push back against the narrative that these gaps are being hidden from public view and researchers have not taken them seriously. Nothing could be further from the truth.</p><p>An old adage in business circles is “what gets measured gets done.” Indeed, a pillar of the school accountability movement is providing clear, disaggregated data at the state, district, and local levels so that parents and community members can better understand and monitor student achievement.</p><p>Reeves argues that we often choose to ignore gender gaps, stating “At the extreme, gender-neutrality veers into gender-blind approach: some school districts, for example, do not even routinely track differences in outcomes by gender.” Such actions would be in violation of the law. Under the Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA; <span>2015</span>), states must publicly report school accountability data in a manner must be disaggregated by a number of categories, including gender.</p><p>In his Point in this exchange (and also in his book, Reeves, 2022), Reeves describes results from a randomized experiment aimed at improving academic performance among college students. In the experiment, students were offered academic support services, financial incentives ranging from $1000 to $5000 for attaining high grades, or both. While the treatment improved women's grades and academic standing, it had no effect on men's outcomes (Angrist et al., <span>2009</span>). Reeves goes on to conclude, “Josh Angrist and co-authors wrote in 2009: ‘These gender differences in the response to incentives and services constitute an important area for further study.’ They do indeed. But as far as I can see, nobody has heeded this call.”</p><p>On the contrary, many have heeded the call. The Angrist et al. (<span>2009</span>) article has more than 700 citations, many of these citations came from studies that have attempted to better understand gender gaps and how to design policies to improve outcomes for males and females. (Indeed, there is a large literature on this topic; see Croson and Gneezy, <span>2009</span>, for a review.) Many of these have been randomized-controlled trials, generally considered to be the gold standard of research. Curiously, none of these are cited in Reeves's work. Below I summarize primarily the work on higher education relevant to the Angrist et al. (<span>2009</span>) quote, but I also note that there is also substantial work on pre-K, elementary and secondary schools, and labor market outcomes.</p><p>When it comes to financial incentives for performance in higher education, the literature on gender effects is far from settled. Two prominent randomized experiments offered financial incentives to college students and found heterogeneous impacts by students’ achievement levels, which may be correlated with gender. In an experiment in the Netherlands, Leuven et al. (<span>2010</span>) found that financial incentives improve the likelihood that college students pass all first-year requirements within a year, but only among high-ability students. Incentives were found to have a negative impact on achievement of low-ability students. In an experiment in an Italian university, students were offered small (250 euros) or large (700 euros) monetary prizes for being among the highest performers on credits earned and examination grades in the first-year undergraduate class (De Paola et al., <span>2012</span>). The prizes improved student performance, though the differences across prize levels were quite modest with a 0.19 standard deviation increase in response to the large prize and a 0.16 standard deviation increase in response to the small one. Similar to the results from the Netherlands, the impacts were concentrated among high-ability students, with no impact among low-ability students. Consistent with differences in achievement levels, the Italian study found suggestive evidence of larger point estimates for females (De Paola et al., <span>2012</span>).</p><p>Performance-based scholarships, a different type of financial incentive, also have inconsistent impacts by gender. Mayer et al. (<span>2015</span>) found that modest scholarships appear to improve credits earned and enrollment more among males than females (though they do not test for statistical differences across groups). By contrast, Cohodes and Goodman (<span>2014</span>) found no differences in impact of merit aid on enrollment, college quality, or completion by gender. Dynarski et al. (<span>2021</span>) studied the impact of a randomized experiment that provided an early commitment of free tuition at a selective flagship university. Note the intervention did not increase aid, but instead provided information and resolved uncertainty. Targeted students were more likely to apply, be admitted, and enroll than the control group; while they find larger point estimates for women than men for admission and enrollment, the gender differences are not quite statistically significant (<i>p</i> = 0.15).</p><p>Access to academic support also has inconsistent impacts by gender. As previously mentioned, Angrist et al. (<span>2009</span>) found that academic support services helped women but not men. By contrast, Bettinger and Baker (<span>2014</span>) evaluated a randomized experiment in which college students were provided coaches who helped treated students develop clear, long-term goals and connect their daily activities to those goals, e.g., through improving skills in time-management, self-advocacy, and studying. The impacts on college persistence was positive, with the impact greater for males than for females, through 24 months post-assignment.</p><p>Building on these and other studies, Clark et al. (<span>2020</span>) conducted a series of fascinating experiments designed to better understand how to structure interventions to improve college outcomes, with special attention to gender differences. In particular, they compared the impacts of task-based goals (i.e., asking students to set a goal for the number of practice quizzes they would take over the course of the semester) against performance-based goals (i.e., asking students to set a goal final letter grade).</p><p>Clark et al. (<span>2020</span>) drew insights from the prior literature which suggests that males are expected to be more responsive to task-based goals for a variety of reasons. Males have been shown on average to have less self-control in educational environments than females do, suggesting that interventions aimed at improving self-control may have a stronger impact on them (Duckworth &amp; Seligman, <span>2005</span>; Duckworth et al., <span>2015</span>). Further, goal-setting interventions appear to be more successful for males overall and for students who think more about the present than the future (i.e., are “present-biased,” a trait also more common among males). In addition, there are gender differences in responses to competition (Gneezy et al., <span>2003</span>). For example, Azmat et al. (<span>2016</span>) have shown that girls tend to outperform boys on tests, but their advantage declines the stakes on a test become higher.</p><p>Consistent with the theoretical framework, the experiments conducted by Clark et al. (<span>2020</span>) found that males are more responsive to setting task-based goals and the intervention in turn has a positive impact on their final course grades. By contrast, females neither complete more tasks nor improve their course grades when assigned to set task-based goals. Performance-based goals had no effect on males or females.</p><p>To be sure, the literature is largely unsettled. In some cases, we still do not have solid evidence on <i>whether</i> interventions vary by gender. We surely do not know enough about <i>why</i> the impacts of some interventions vary by gender. But this reflects the difficulty of the challenge, and not a lack of serious attention to the problem.</p>","PeriodicalId":48105,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Policy Analysis and Management","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3000,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/pam.22577","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Policy Analysis and Management","FirstCategoryId":"91","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/pam.22577","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"ECONOMICS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Fundamentally, Reeves and I agree about the importance of boys’ educational under-achievement and the need to openly discuss and address it. I emphasize that when boys fail to thrive in school, it has downstream consequences not only for their own lives but for our nation's economic growth. Further, boys’ success need not come at the expense of girls’ success. This is not zero-sum; we all benefit when children reach their potential.

I must push back against the narrative that these gaps are being hidden from public view and researchers have not taken them seriously. Nothing could be further from the truth.

An old adage in business circles is “what gets measured gets done.” Indeed, a pillar of the school accountability movement is providing clear, disaggregated data at the state, district, and local levels so that parents and community members can better understand and monitor student achievement.

Reeves argues that we often choose to ignore gender gaps, stating “At the extreme, gender-neutrality veers into gender-blind approach: some school districts, for example, do not even routinely track differences in outcomes by gender.” Such actions would be in violation of the law. Under the Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA; 2015), states must publicly report school accountability data in a manner must be disaggregated by a number of categories, including gender.

In his Point in this exchange (and also in his book, Reeves, 2022), Reeves describes results from a randomized experiment aimed at improving academic performance among college students. In the experiment, students were offered academic support services, financial incentives ranging from $1000 to $5000 for attaining high grades, or both. While the treatment improved women's grades and academic standing, it had no effect on men's outcomes (Angrist et al., 2009). Reeves goes on to conclude, “Josh Angrist and co-authors wrote in 2009: ‘These gender differences in the response to incentives and services constitute an important area for further study.’ They do indeed. But as far as I can see, nobody has heeded this call.”

On the contrary, many have heeded the call. The Angrist et al. (2009) article has more than 700 citations, many of these citations came from studies that have attempted to better understand gender gaps and how to design policies to improve outcomes for males and females. (Indeed, there is a large literature on this topic; see Croson and Gneezy, 2009, for a review.) Many of these have been randomized-controlled trials, generally considered to be the gold standard of research. Curiously, none of these are cited in Reeves's work. Below I summarize primarily the work on higher education relevant to the Angrist et al. (2009) quote, but I also note that there is also substantial work on pre-K, elementary and secondary schools, and labor market outcomes.

When it comes to financial incentives for performance in higher education, the literature on gender effects is far from settled. Two prominent randomized experiments offered financial incentives to college students and found heterogeneous impacts by students’ achievement levels, which may be correlated with gender. In an experiment in the Netherlands, Leuven et al. (2010) found that financial incentives improve the likelihood that college students pass all first-year requirements within a year, but only among high-ability students. Incentives were found to have a negative impact on achievement of low-ability students. In an experiment in an Italian university, students were offered small (250 euros) or large (700 euros) monetary prizes for being among the highest performers on credits earned and examination grades in the first-year undergraduate class (De Paola et al., 2012). The prizes improved student performance, though the differences across prize levels were quite modest with a 0.19 standard deviation increase in response to the large prize and a 0.16 standard deviation increase in response to the small one. Similar to the results from the Netherlands, the impacts were concentrated among high-ability students, with no impact among low-ability students. Consistent with differences in achievement levels, the Italian study found suggestive evidence of larger point estimates for females (De Paola et al., 2012).

Performance-based scholarships, a different type of financial incentive, also have inconsistent impacts by gender. Mayer et al. (2015) found that modest scholarships appear to improve credits earned and enrollment more among males than females (though they do not test for statistical differences across groups). By contrast, Cohodes and Goodman (2014) found no differences in impact of merit aid on enrollment, college quality, or completion by gender. Dynarski et al. (2021) studied the impact of a randomized experiment that provided an early commitment of free tuition at a selective flagship university. Note the intervention did not increase aid, but instead provided information and resolved uncertainty. Targeted students were more likely to apply, be admitted, and enroll than the control group; while they find larger point estimates for women than men for admission and enrollment, the gender differences are not quite statistically significant (p = 0.15).

Access to academic support also has inconsistent impacts by gender. As previously mentioned, Angrist et al. (2009) found that academic support services helped women but not men. By contrast, Bettinger and Baker (2014) evaluated a randomized experiment in which college students were provided coaches who helped treated students develop clear, long-term goals and connect their daily activities to those goals, e.g., through improving skills in time-management, self-advocacy, and studying. The impacts on college persistence was positive, with the impact greater for males than for females, through 24 months post-assignment.

Building on these and other studies, Clark et al. (2020) conducted a series of fascinating experiments designed to better understand how to structure interventions to improve college outcomes, with special attention to gender differences. In particular, they compared the impacts of task-based goals (i.e., asking students to set a goal for the number of practice quizzes they would take over the course of the semester) against performance-based goals (i.e., asking students to set a goal final letter grade).

Clark et al. (2020) drew insights from the prior literature which suggests that males are expected to be more responsive to task-based goals for a variety of reasons. Males have been shown on average to have less self-control in educational environments than females do, suggesting that interventions aimed at improving self-control may have a stronger impact on them (Duckworth & Seligman, 2005; Duckworth et al., 2015). Further, goal-setting interventions appear to be more successful for males overall and for students who think more about the present than the future (i.e., are “present-biased,” a trait also more common among males). In addition, there are gender differences in responses to competition (Gneezy et al., 2003). For example, Azmat et al. (2016) have shown that girls tend to outperform boys on tests, but their advantage declines the stakes on a test become higher.

Consistent with the theoretical framework, the experiments conducted by Clark et al. (2020) found that males are more responsive to setting task-based goals and the intervention in turn has a positive impact on their final course grades. By contrast, females neither complete more tasks nor improve their course grades when assigned to set task-based goals. Performance-based goals had no effect on males or females.

To be sure, the literature is largely unsettled. In some cases, we still do not have solid evidence on whether interventions vary by gender. We surely do not know enough about why the impacts of some interventions vary by gender. But this reflects the difficulty of the challenge, and not a lack of serious attention to the problem.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
关注(成绩)差距
从根本上说,里夫斯和我都认为男孩教育成绩不佳的问题非常重要,有必要公开讨论并解决这个问题。我强调,如果男孩不能在学校茁壮成长,不仅会对他们的生活,而且会对我们国家的经济增长造成下游影响。此外,男孩的成功不必以女孩的成功为代价。这并不是零和;当孩子们发挥出他们的潜能时,我们都会受益。我必须反驳这样一种说法,即这些差距被隐藏在公众视线之外,研究人员也没有认真对待这些差距。商业界的一句老话是 "量力而行"。事实上,学校问责制运动的一个支柱就是在州、学区和地方层面提供清晰的分类数据,以便家长和社区成员更好地了解和监督学生的成绩。里夫斯认为,我们经常选择忽视性别差距,他说:"在极端情况下,性别中立会变成性别盲目的方法:例如,有些学区甚至不按性别对成绩差异进行常规跟踪。这种行为是违法的。根据《每个学生都能成功法案》(ESSA;2015 年),各州必须公开报告学校的问责数据,数据必须按照包括性别在内的多个类别进行分类。在此次交流的《观点》(以及他的著作《里夫斯,2022 年》)中,里夫斯介绍了一项旨在提高大学生学业成绩的随机实验的结果。在实验中,学生可以获得学业支持服务,也可以获得 1000 美元到 5000 美元不等的经济奖励,或者两者兼得。虽然这项措施提高了女生的成绩和学术地位,但对男生的结果却没有影响(Angrist 等人,2009 年)。里夫斯继续总结道:"乔希-安格里斯特和合著者在 2009 年写道:'在对激励措施和服务的反应方面存在的这些性别差异,构成了一个有待进一步研究的重要领域。的确如此。但就我所见,没有人响应这一号召。"恰恰相反,许多人响应了这一号召。安格里斯特等人(2009 年)的文章被引用了 700 多次,其中许多引用来自于试图更好地理解性别差距以及如何设计政策来改善男性和女性结果的研究。(事实上,有关这一主题的文献很多;参见 Croson 和 Gneezy,2009 年,综述)。其中许多都是随机对照试验,通常被认为是研究的黄金标准。奇怪的是,里夫斯的著作中没有引用这些试验。下面,我将主要总结与 Angrist 等人(2009 年)的引文相关的高等教育方面的研究,但我也要指出,学前教育、中小学和劳动力市场成果方面也有大量研究。有两个著名的随机实验为大学生提供了经济激励,结果发现不同学生的成绩水平会产生不同的影响,这可能与性别有关。在荷兰的一项实验中,Leuven 等人(2010 年)发现,经济激励措施提高了大学生在一年内通过所有一年级要求的可能性,但只针对高能力学生。激励措施对低能力学生的成绩有负面影响。在意大利一所大学进行的一项实验中,如果学生在本科一年级获得的学分和考试成绩名列前茅,就会获得小额(250 欧元)或大额(700 欧元)奖金(De Paola 等人,2012 年)。这些奖金提高了学生的学习成绩,但不同级别的奖金差异不大,大额奖金提高了 0.19 个标准差,小额奖金提高了 0.16 个标准差。与荷兰的结果类似,影响主要集中在高能力学生中,对低能力学生没有影响。与成绩水平的差异相一致,意大利的研究发现,有提示性证据表明女性的点估计值更大(De Paola et al.Mayer 等人(2015 年)发现,适度的奖学金似乎比女性更能提高男性获得的学分和入学率(尽管他们没有检验不同群体之间的统计差异)。相比之下,Cohodes 和 Goodman(2014 年)发现不同性别的学生在择优资助对入学率、大学质量或毕业率的影响方面没有差异。Dynarski 等人(2021 年)研究了一项随机实验的影响,该实验在一所精选的旗舰大学中提前承诺免学费。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
5.80
自引率
2.60%
发文量
82
期刊介绍: This journal encompasses issues and practices in policy analysis and public management. Listed among the contributors are economists, public managers, and operations researchers. Featured regularly are book reviews and a department devoted to discussing ideas and issues of importance to practitioners, researchers, and academics.
期刊最新文献
Unearthing the impact of earthquakes: A review of economic and social consequences Can destigmatizing mental health increase willingness to seek help? Experimental evidence from Nepal Issue Information Contents Introduction to the Research Articles
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1