Christopher Dawes , Samuel Joy McGreal , Shivika Marwaha , Jose Prados , Antoine Reheis , Alin Dumitrescu , John L. Waddington , Paula M. Moran , Colm O'Tuathaigh
{"title":"Overshadowing and salience attribution in relation to cannabis use","authors":"Christopher Dawes , Samuel Joy McGreal , Shivika Marwaha , Jose Prados , Antoine Reheis , Alin Dumitrescu , John L. Waddington , Paula M. Moran , Colm O'Tuathaigh","doi":"10.1016/j.scog.2024.100315","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Aberrant attentional salience has been implicated in the cannabis-psychosis association. Here, history and frequency of cannabis use were examined against changes in overshadowing (OS), a cue competition paradigm that involves salience processing. Additionally, we examined the association between OS and alternative measures of aberrant salience, as well as schizotypy, in a non-clinical adult sample.</p><p>280 participants completed an online geometry learning-based OS task, while a subset (<em>N</em> = 149) also completed the Salience Attribution Task (SAT) measure of aberrant salience. All completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ), Aberrant Salience Inventory (ASI), and the modified Cannabis Experience Questionnaire (CEQmv). Differences across OS and SAT performance stages and between cannabis use groups were assessed using mixed ANOVAs. Multiple regression and correlational analyses assessed the relationships between OS and SAT task metrics and SPQ and ASI subscale scores.</p><p>Current cannabis users had significantly lower OS scores during the testing phase relative to those who do not use cannabis, at medium effect sizes. Schizotypy or ASI scores did not mediate this relationship. In the SAT, current cannabis users presented significantly higher implicit aberrant salience relative to non-users. Scores in the first training phase of the OS task significantly predicted higher explicit aberrant and adaptive salience scores in the SAT.</p><p>These data indicate an association between regular cannabis use and abnormalities in cue competition effects in a healthy adult sample. Comparisons of OS and SAT cast new light on putative overlapping mechanisms underlying performance across different measures of salience.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":38119,"journal":{"name":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3000,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000167/pdfft?md5=6fba5df38d9195c49dd128eb914dfdc4&pid=1-s2.0-S2215001324000167-main.pdf","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Schizophrenia Research-Cognition","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2215001324000167","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHIATRY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
Aberrant attentional salience has been implicated in the cannabis-psychosis association. Here, history and frequency of cannabis use were examined against changes in overshadowing (OS), a cue competition paradigm that involves salience processing. Additionally, we examined the association between OS and alternative measures of aberrant salience, as well as schizotypy, in a non-clinical adult sample.
280 participants completed an online geometry learning-based OS task, while a subset (N = 149) also completed the Salience Attribution Task (SAT) measure of aberrant salience. All completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ), Aberrant Salience Inventory (ASI), and the modified Cannabis Experience Questionnaire (CEQmv). Differences across OS and SAT performance stages and between cannabis use groups were assessed using mixed ANOVAs. Multiple regression and correlational analyses assessed the relationships between OS and SAT task metrics and SPQ and ASI subscale scores.
Current cannabis users had significantly lower OS scores during the testing phase relative to those who do not use cannabis, at medium effect sizes. Schizotypy or ASI scores did not mediate this relationship. In the SAT, current cannabis users presented significantly higher implicit aberrant salience relative to non-users. Scores in the first training phase of the OS task significantly predicted higher explicit aberrant and adaptive salience scores in the SAT.
These data indicate an association between regular cannabis use and abnormalities in cue competition effects in a healthy adult sample. Comparisons of OS and SAT cast new light on putative overlapping mechanisms underlying performance across different measures of salience.
注意力显著性异常与大麻-精神病的关联有一定联系。在此,我们针对阴影(OS)的变化研究了使用大麻的历史和频率,这是一种涉及显著性处理的线索竞争范式。此外,我们还在一个非临床成人样本中研究了OS与其他异常显著性测量方法以及精神分裂症之间的关联。280名参与者完成了基于几何学习的在线OS任务,其中一部分(N = 149)还完成了异常显著性测量方法 "显著性归因任务"(SAT)。所有参与者都完成了分裂型人格问卷(SPQ)、异常显著性量表(ASI)和修改后的大麻体验问卷(CEQmv)。使用混合方差分析评估了 OS 和 SAT 成绩阶段之间以及使用大麻组之间的差异。多元回归和相关分析评估了OS和SAT任务指标与SPQ和ASI分量表得分之间的关系。与不吸食大麻的人相比,目前吸食大麻的人在测试阶段的OS得分明显较低,影响程度为中等。精神分裂症或 ASI 分数并不影响这种关系。在 SAT 中,与不吸食大麻者相比,目前吸食大麻者的内隐异常显著性明显更高。这些数据表明,在健康的成人样本中,经常吸食大麻与线索竞争效应异常之间存在关联。对OS和SAT进行比较后,我们对不同显著性测量中可能存在的重叠机制有了新的认识。