Our evolved unique pleasure circuit makes humans different from apes: Reconsideration of data derived from animal studies.

Kenneth Blum, Marjorie Gondré-Lewis, Bruce Steinberg, Igor Elman, David Baron, Edward J Modestino, Rajendra D Badgaiyan, Mark S Gold
{"title":"Our evolved unique pleasure circuit makes humans different from apes: Reconsideration of data derived from animal studies.","authors":"Kenneth Blum,&nbsp;Marjorie Gondré-Lewis,&nbsp;Bruce Steinberg,&nbsp;Igor Elman,&nbsp;David Baron,&nbsp;Edward J Modestino,&nbsp;Rajendra D Badgaiyan,&nbsp;Mark S Gold","doi":"10.15761/JSIN.1000191","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The brain regions tied to pleasure can be triggered by engaging in sex, eating tasty food, watching a movie, accomplishments at school and athletics, consuming drugs, and noble efforts to help the community, the country, and the world. It is noteworthy that research suggests that the latter type of satisfaction, supporting the community, may result in the most substantial positive effects on our immune system. However, these pathways for these effects are not understood. Berridge and Kringelbach have suggested that pleasure is mediated by well-developed mesocorticolimbic circuitry and serves adaptive functions. In affective disorders, anhedonia (lack of pleasure) or dysphoria (negative affect) can result from a breakdown of that hedonic system. Most importantly, human neuroimaging investigations indicate that surprisingly similar circuitry is activated by quite diverse pleasures, suggesting a common neural pathway shared by all rewarding stimuli and behaviors. Over many years the controversy of dopamine involvement in pleasure/reward has led to confusion in terms, such as trying to separate motivation from pure pleasure (i.e., wanting versus liking). We take the position that animal studies cannot provide real clinical information that is described by self-reports in humans. On November 23<sup>rd</sup>, 2017, evidence for our concerns was revealed. A brain system involved in everything from addiction to autism appears to have evolved differently in humans than in apes, as reported by a large research team in the journal <i>Science</i>. To reiterate, the new findings by Sousa et al., also suggest the importance of not over-relying on rodent and even non-human primate studies. Extrapolations, when it comes to the concept of pleasure, dopamine, and reinforcement, are not supported by these data. Human experience and study are now much more critical and important. Extrapolations from non-humans to humans may be more fiction than fact. While this statement is bold it should not at all suggest that animal date is unimportant, that is not the case. It is extremely valuable in many aspects and we must encourage the development of animal models for disease. However, we must be cautious in our interpretation of results without leaping to conclusions that may be explained by follow-up human experiments and subsequent data. We are further proposing that in terms of overcoming a never -ending battle related to the current drug epidemic, the scientific community should realize that disturbing dopamine homeostasis by taking drugs or having a system compromised by genes or other epigenetic experiences, should be treated by alternative therapeutic modalities, expressed in this article as a realistic key goal. Application of genetic addiction risk (GARS™) testing and pro-dopamine regulation (KB220) should be considered along with other promising technologies including cognitive behavioral therapy, mind fullness, brain spotting and trauma therapy. Basic scientists have worked very hard to dis-entangle pleasure from incentive salience and learning signals in brain reward circuitry, but this work may be limited to animal models and rodents. A different consideration regarding the human reward systems is required.</p>","PeriodicalId":87318,"journal":{"name":"Journal of systems and integrative neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2018-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6446569/pdf/","citationCount":"14","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of systems and integrative neuroscience","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.15761/JSIN.1000191","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"2018/2/28 0:00:00","PubModel":"Epub","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14

Abstract

The brain regions tied to pleasure can be triggered by engaging in sex, eating tasty food, watching a movie, accomplishments at school and athletics, consuming drugs, and noble efforts to help the community, the country, and the world. It is noteworthy that research suggests that the latter type of satisfaction, supporting the community, may result in the most substantial positive effects on our immune system. However, these pathways for these effects are not understood. Berridge and Kringelbach have suggested that pleasure is mediated by well-developed mesocorticolimbic circuitry and serves adaptive functions. In affective disorders, anhedonia (lack of pleasure) or dysphoria (negative affect) can result from a breakdown of that hedonic system. Most importantly, human neuroimaging investigations indicate that surprisingly similar circuitry is activated by quite diverse pleasures, suggesting a common neural pathway shared by all rewarding stimuli and behaviors. Over many years the controversy of dopamine involvement in pleasure/reward has led to confusion in terms, such as trying to separate motivation from pure pleasure (i.e., wanting versus liking). We take the position that animal studies cannot provide real clinical information that is described by self-reports in humans. On November 23rd, 2017, evidence for our concerns was revealed. A brain system involved in everything from addiction to autism appears to have evolved differently in humans than in apes, as reported by a large research team in the journal Science. To reiterate, the new findings by Sousa et al., also suggest the importance of not over-relying on rodent and even non-human primate studies. Extrapolations, when it comes to the concept of pleasure, dopamine, and reinforcement, are not supported by these data. Human experience and study are now much more critical and important. Extrapolations from non-humans to humans may be more fiction than fact. While this statement is bold it should not at all suggest that animal date is unimportant, that is not the case. It is extremely valuable in many aspects and we must encourage the development of animal models for disease. However, we must be cautious in our interpretation of results without leaping to conclusions that may be explained by follow-up human experiments and subsequent data. We are further proposing that in terms of overcoming a never -ending battle related to the current drug epidemic, the scientific community should realize that disturbing dopamine homeostasis by taking drugs or having a system compromised by genes or other epigenetic experiences, should be treated by alternative therapeutic modalities, expressed in this article as a realistic key goal. Application of genetic addiction risk (GARS™) testing and pro-dopamine regulation (KB220) should be considered along with other promising technologies including cognitive behavioral therapy, mind fullness, brain spotting and trauma therapy. Basic scientists have worked very hard to dis-entangle pleasure from incentive salience and learning signals in brain reward circuitry, but this work may be limited to animal models and rodents. A different consideration regarding the human reward systems is required.

Abstract Image

Abstract Image

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
我们进化出的独特的快乐回路使人类不同于猿:重新考虑来自动物研究的数据。
大脑中与快乐相关的区域可以在性行为、吃美味的食物、看电影、在学校和体育运动中取得成就、吸毒以及为社区、国家和世界做出崇高的努力时被触发。值得注意的是,研究表明,后一种满足感,支持社区,可能会对我们的免疫系统产生最实质性的积极影响。然而,这些影响的途径尚不清楚。Berridge和Kringelbach认为,快乐是由发达的中皮质边缘回路调节的,具有适应功能。在情感性障碍中,快感缺乏症(缺乏快乐)或烦躁不安(负面影响)可能是由快感系统的崩溃引起的。最重要的是,人类神经成像研究表明,令人惊讶的相似回路被相当不同的快乐所激活,这表明所有有益的刺激和行为都有一个共同的神经通路。多年来,关于多巴胺参与快乐/奖励的争论导致了术语上的混乱,例如试图将动机与纯粹的快乐(即想要与喜欢)分开。我们的立场是,动物研究不能提供人类自我报告所描述的真实临床信息。2017年11月23日,我们的担忧得到了证实。一个大型研究团队在《科学》杂志上发表的研究报告称,人类大脑系统的进化方式似乎与类人猿不同,从成瘾到自闭症,人类大脑系统的进化方式与类人猿不同。重申一下,Sousa等人的新发现也表明,不要过度依赖啮齿动物甚至非人类灵长类动物研究的重要性。当涉及到快乐、多巴胺和强化的概念时,这些数据并不支持推断。人类的经验和研究现在更加关键和重要。从非人类到人类的推断可能更多是虚构的,而不是事实。虽然这句话很大胆,但它根本不应该表明动物日期不重要,事实并非如此。它在许多方面都非常有价值,我们必须鼓励疾病动物模型的发展。然而,我们在解释结果时必须谨慎,不要急于得出可以通过后续人体实验和后续数据解释的结论。我们进一步建议,为了克服与当前药物流行相关的无休止的战斗,科学界应该认识到,通过服用药物或通过基因或其他表观遗传经验损害系统来扰乱多巴胺稳态,应该通过替代治疗方式进行治疗,这在本文中表示为一个现实的关键目标。基因成瘾风险(GARS™)测试和多巴胺前调节(KB220)的应用应该与其他有前途的技术一起考虑,包括认知行为治疗、心智充实、大脑斑点和创伤治疗。基础科学家们一直在努力将快乐与大脑奖励回路中的激励显著性和学习信号分开,但这项工作可能仅限于动物模型和啮齿动物。我们需要对人类奖励系统进行不同的考虑。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
Bariatric surgery: Potential post-operative heightened sensitivity to substances or behaviors. Opioid and dopamine genes interact to predict precision naltrexone response in alcohol use disorder: Interpretation misfires. The cognitive profile in Slovak children with autism spectrum disorders Methyl jasmonate rescues structural alterations in cortico-limbic neurons produced by the unpredictable chronic mild stress model of depression Technical and statistical milestones and standards for construction, validation and/or comparison of Quantitative Electroencephalogram (QEEG) normative databases
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1