{"title":"Repeated letters increase the ambiguity of strings: Evidence from identification, priming and same-different tasks","authors":"Iliyana V. Trifonova , James S. Adelman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101445","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Letters are often repeated in words in many languages. The present work explored the mechanisms underlying processing of repeated and unique letters in strings across three experimental paradigms. In a 2AFC perceptual identification task, the insertion but not the deletion of a letter was harder to detect when it was repeated than when it was unique (Exp. 1). In a masked primed same-different task, deletion primes produced the same priming effect regardless of deletion type (repeated, unique; Exp. 2), but insertion primes were more effective when the additional inserted letter created a repetition than when it did not (Exp. 3). In a same-different perceptual identification task, foils created by modifying a repetition, by either repeating the wrong letter or substituting a repeated letter, were harder to reject than foils created by modifying unique letters (Exp. 4). Thus, repetition effects were task-dependent. Since considering representations alone would suggest repetition effects would always occur or never occur, this indicates the importance of modelling task-specific processes. The similarity calculations embedded in the Overlap Model (Gomez et al., 2008) appeared to always predict a repetition effect, but its decision rule for the task of Experiment 1 allowed it to predict the asymmetry between insertions and deletions. In the Letters in Time and Retinotopic Space (LTRS; Adelman, 2011) model, repetition effects arise only from briefly presented stimuli as their perception is incomplete. It was therefore consistent with Experiments 2–4 but required a task-specific response bias to account for the insertion-deletion asymmetry of Experiment 1.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"132 ","pages":"Article 101445"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0000,"publicationDate":"2022-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"2","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Cognitive Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010028521000682","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Abstract
Letters are often repeated in words in many languages. The present work explored the mechanisms underlying processing of repeated and unique letters in strings across three experimental paradigms. In a 2AFC perceptual identification task, the insertion but not the deletion of a letter was harder to detect when it was repeated than when it was unique (Exp. 1). In a masked primed same-different task, deletion primes produced the same priming effect regardless of deletion type (repeated, unique; Exp. 2), but insertion primes were more effective when the additional inserted letter created a repetition than when it did not (Exp. 3). In a same-different perceptual identification task, foils created by modifying a repetition, by either repeating the wrong letter or substituting a repeated letter, were harder to reject than foils created by modifying unique letters (Exp. 4). Thus, repetition effects were task-dependent. Since considering representations alone would suggest repetition effects would always occur or never occur, this indicates the importance of modelling task-specific processes. The similarity calculations embedded in the Overlap Model (Gomez et al., 2008) appeared to always predict a repetition effect, but its decision rule for the task of Experiment 1 allowed it to predict the asymmetry between insertions and deletions. In the Letters in Time and Retinotopic Space (LTRS; Adelman, 2011) model, repetition effects arise only from briefly presented stimuli as their perception is incomplete. It was therefore consistent with Experiments 2–4 but required a task-specific response bias to account for the insertion-deletion asymmetry of Experiment 1.
在许多语言中,字母经常在单词中重复出现。本研究在三个实验范式中探讨了字符串中重复和唯一字母的加工机制。在2AFC感知识别任务中,字母的插入而非删除在重复时比在唯一时更难被检测到(实验1)。在屏蔽启动相同-不同任务中,删除启动产生相同的启动效应,无论删除类型(重复,唯一;实验2),但是当额外插入的字母产生重复时,插入启动比没有(实验3)时更有效。在相同-不同的感知识别任务中,通过修改重复产生的箔,通过重复错误的字母或替换重复的字母,比修改唯一字母产生的箔更难被拒绝(实验4)。因此,重复效应是任务依赖的。由于单独考虑表征会表明重复效应总是会发生或永远不会发生,这表明了建模任务特定过程的重要性。重叠模型(Gomez et al., 2008)中嵌入的相似性计算似乎总是预测重复效应,但其对实验1任务的决策规则允许它预测插入和删除之间的不对称性。在时间和视网膜位置空间(LTRS)中的信件Adelman, 2011)模型,重复效应只产生于短暂呈现的刺激,因为他们的感知是不完整的。因此,这与实验2-4一致,但需要任务特异性反应偏差来解释实验1的插入-删除不对称。
期刊介绍:
Cognitive Psychology is concerned with advances in the study of attention, memory, language processing, perception, problem solving, and thinking. Cognitive Psychology specializes in extensive articles that have a major impact on cognitive theory and provide new theoretical advances.
Research Areas include:
• Artificial intelligence
• Developmental psychology
• Linguistics
• Neurophysiology
• Social psychology.