Constructional contamination in morphology and syntax

IF 0.8 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Constructions and Frames Pub Date : 2018-12-31 DOI:10.1075/CF.00021.PIJ
Dirk Pijpops, Isabeau De Smet, F. Velde
{"title":"Constructional contamination in morphology and syntax","authors":"Dirk Pijpops, Isabeau De Smet, F. Velde","doi":"10.1075/CF.00021.PIJ","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"\n In every-day language use, two or more structurally unrelated constructions may occasionally give rise to strings\n that look very similar on the surface. As a result of this superficial resemblance, a subset of instances of one of these\n constructions may deviate in the probabilistic preference for either of several possible formal variants. This effect is called\n ‘constructional contamination’, and was introduced in Pijpops & Van de Velde\n (2016). Constructional contamination bears testimony to the hypothesis that language users do not always execute a full\n parse of the utterances they interpret, but instead often rely on ‘shallow parsing’ and the storage of large, unanalyzed chunks of\n language in memory, as proposed in Ferreira, Bailey, & Ferraro (2002), Ferreira & Patson (2007), and Dąbrowska\n (2014).\n \n Pijpops & Van de Velde (2016) investigated a single case study in\n depth, namely the Dutch partitive genitive. This case study is reviewed, and three new case studies are added, namely the\n competition between long and bare infinitives, word order variation in verbal clusters, and preterite formation. We find evidence\n of constructional contamination in all case studies, albeit in varying degrees. This indicates that constructional contamination\n is not a particularity of the Dutch partitive genitive but appears to be more wide-spread, affecting both morphology and syntax.\n Furthermore, we distinguish between two forms of constructional contamination, viz. first degree and second degree contamination,\n with first degree contamination producing greater effects than second degree contamination.","PeriodicalId":42321,"journal":{"name":"Constructions and Frames","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.8000,"publicationDate":"2018-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"15","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Constructions and Frames","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1075/CF.00021.PIJ","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"0","JCRName":"LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15

Abstract

In every-day language use, two or more structurally unrelated constructions may occasionally give rise to strings that look very similar on the surface. As a result of this superficial resemblance, a subset of instances of one of these constructions may deviate in the probabilistic preference for either of several possible formal variants. This effect is called ‘constructional contamination’, and was introduced in Pijpops & Van de Velde (2016). Constructional contamination bears testimony to the hypothesis that language users do not always execute a full parse of the utterances they interpret, but instead often rely on ‘shallow parsing’ and the storage of large, unanalyzed chunks of language in memory, as proposed in Ferreira, Bailey, & Ferraro (2002), Ferreira & Patson (2007), and Dąbrowska (2014). Pijpops & Van de Velde (2016) investigated a single case study in depth, namely the Dutch partitive genitive. This case study is reviewed, and three new case studies are added, namely the competition between long and bare infinitives, word order variation in verbal clusters, and preterite formation. We find evidence of constructional contamination in all case studies, albeit in varying degrees. This indicates that constructional contamination is not a particularity of the Dutch partitive genitive but appears to be more wide-spread, affecting both morphology and syntax. Furthermore, we distinguish between two forms of constructional contamination, viz. first degree and second degree contamination, with first degree contamination producing greater effects than second degree contamination.
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
形态学和句法中的结构污染
在日常语言使用中,两个或两个以上结构上不相关的结构偶尔会产生表面上看起来非常相似的字符串。由于这种表面上的相似性,这些结构之一的实例的子集可能会偏离几种可能的形式变体中的任何一种的概率偏好。这种影响被称为“建筑污染”,Pijpops&Van de Velde(2016)对此进行了介绍。正如Ferreira、Bailey和Ferraro(2002)、Ferreira和Patson(2007)以及Dãbrowska(2014)所提出的那样,结构污染证明了这样一种假设,即语言使用者并不总是对他们所解释的话语进行完整的解析,而是经常依赖于“浅解析”和在记忆中存储大量未分析的语言块。Pijpops&Van de Velde(2016)深入调查了一个单一的案例研究,即荷兰的部分属格。对这一案例研究进行了回顾,并增加了三个新的案例研究,即长不定式和裸不定式之间的竞争、言语簇中的语序变化和仪式前形成。我们在所有案例研究中都发现了建筑污染的证据,尽管程度不同。这表明结构污染并不是荷兰语部分属格的特殊性,而是更广泛地传播,影响了形态和句法。此外,我们区分了两种形式的建筑污染,即一级污染和二级污染,一级污染比二级污染产生更大的影响。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Constructions and Frames
Constructions and Frames LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS-
CiteScore
1.30
自引率
0.00%
发文量
9
期刊最新文献
From data to theory Constructional change and frameelement selection A multilingual approach to the interaction between frames and constructions Frame integration and head-switching Old English V-initial and þa-VS main clauses
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1