Agency, activity, and biocybernetics: On The Evolution of Agency by Michael Tomasello

IF 1.8 3区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Mind Culture and Activity Pub Date : 2023-01-02 DOI:10.1080/10749039.2023.2246947
M. Falikman
{"title":"Agency, activity, and biocybernetics: On The Evolution of Agency by Michael Tomasello","authors":"M. Falikman","doi":"10.1080/10749039.2023.2246947","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"In his new book, Michael Tomasello addresses agency, a topic that has recently become a focus of interest for philosophers (see Bayne, 2013) and neuroscientists (e.g., Grünbaum & Christensen, 2020; Haggard, 2017) and that has always been a key topic for activity theory in psychology (Leontiev, 1978). Agency refers to the capacity to perform intentional actions, or, more generally, goal-directed behavior. In the cultural historical activity theory (CHAT), the most recent discussions of agency go far beyond this basic definition, encompassing such phenomena as civic engagement, activism, and social change (Hopwood, 2022; Stetsenko, 2020). However, the definition of agency as “initiative and commitment to transform the context(s) of [. . .] activity” (Kajamaa & Kumpulainen, 2019, p. 201), as will be shown below, is in line with the general trend of the evolution of agency outlined by Tomasello. A renowned researcher of shared intentionality as a key feature of Homo sapiens and its evolution (e.g., Tomasello & Carpenter, 2007; Tomasello et al., 2005), Tomasello here turns to a more basic question of intentionality itself. He puts forward an ambitious goal to trace the evolution of agency which he considers the “first principle” of psychology, or “the organizational framework within which both behavioral and mental processes operate” (Tomasello, 2022, p. 134). Even more importantly, this principle incorporates both ecological and social factors, bridging biological and cultural evolution – an endeavor also intrinsic to activity theory (Engeström, 1987; Leontyev, 1981). Since the beginning of the 20th century, American psychology and CHAT have evolved separately, with a breakdown in communication between American and Soviet researchers. In the middle of that century, CHAT research surged mostly in the Soviet Union, behind the “iron curtain” (for a more detailed discussion, see Toulmin, 1978), and we still observe its consequences. It is somewhat surprising that Tomasello, who has obviously been aware of the quest for the origins and hallmarks of the human mind in the Vygotskian cultural historical approach and who even contributed to its substantiation (e.g., Moll & Tomasello, 2007), sets this framework aside in his new book. At the same time, it is tempting to compare some of his speculations and hypotheses to the evolutionary aspects of activity theory, which is rooted not only in Marxist philosophy but also in Russian evolutionary and comparative biology and neuroscience of the first half of the previous century. Tomasello’s book reads more like a detailed and somewhat repetitive essay elaborating on a couple of basic principles to explain agency, namely: feedback (or multiple comparisons between a goal state and a current state) and levels (“tiers”) of the regulation of behavior, or its hierarchical structure. Still fighting the ghost of behaviorism and its linear stimulus-response paradigm, with its over a centurylong dominance in American psychology, Tomasello emphasizes the importance of agency in behavioral flexibility under uncertainty, produced by the environment and first of all by other agents, as an evolutionarily beneficial property of behavior. For discussion of the main steps in the development of agency through evolution, Tomasello selects four “model objects,” partly representing extinct species which are unavailable for immediate investigation: lizards as an example of early agency subserving foraging behavior, squirrels, primates, and humans. Each species embodies the rise of a new form of agentive organization, which Tomasello labels as goal-directed agency, intentional agency, rational","PeriodicalId":51588,"journal":{"name":"Mind Culture and Activity","volume":"30 1","pages":"90 - 96"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8000,"publicationDate":"2023-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Mind Culture and Activity","FirstCategoryId":"95","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/10749039.2023.2246947","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

In his new book, Michael Tomasello addresses agency, a topic that has recently become a focus of interest for philosophers (see Bayne, 2013) and neuroscientists (e.g., Grünbaum & Christensen, 2020; Haggard, 2017) and that has always been a key topic for activity theory in psychology (Leontiev, 1978). Agency refers to the capacity to perform intentional actions, or, more generally, goal-directed behavior. In the cultural historical activity theory (CHAT), the most recent discussions of agency go far beyond this basic definition, encompassing such phenomena as civic engagement, activism, and social change (Hopwood, 2022; Stetsenko, 2020). However, the definition of agency as “initiative and commitment to transform the context(s) of [. . .] activity” (Kajamaa & Kumpulainen, 2019, p. 201), as will be shown below, is in line with the general trend of the evolution of agency outlined by Tomasello. A renowned researcher of shared intentionality as a key feature of Homo sapiens and its evolution (e.g., Tomasello & Carpenter, 2007; Tomasello et al., 2005), Tomasello here turns to a more basic question of intentionality itself. He puts forward an ambitious goal to trace the evolution of agency which he considers the “first principle” of psychology, or “the organizational framework within which both behavioral and mental processes operate” (Tomasello, 2022, p. 134). Even more importantly, this principle incorporates both ecological and social factors, bridging biological and cultural evolution – an endeavor also intrinsic to activity theory (Engeström, 1987; Leontyev, 1981). Since the beginning of the 20th century, American psychology and CHAT have evolved separately, with a breakdown in communication between American and Soviet researchers. In the middle of that century, CHAT research surged mostly in the Soviet Union, behind the “iron curtain” (for a more detailed discussion, see Toulmin, 1978), and we still observe its consequences. It is somewhat surprising that Tomasello, who has obviously been aware of the quest for the origins and hallmarks of the human mind in the Vygotskian cultural historical approach and who even contributed to its substantiation (e.g., Moll & Tomasello, 2007), sets this framework aside in his new book. At the same time, it is tempting to compare some of his speculations and hypotheses to the evolutionary aspects of activity theory, which is rooted not only in Marxist philosophy but also in Russian evolutionary and comparative biology and neuroscience of the first half of the previous century. Tomasello’s book reads more like a detailed and somewhat repetitive essay elaborating on a couple of basic principles to explain agency, namely: feedback (or multiple comparisons between a goal state and a current state) and levels (“tiers”) of the regulation of behavior, or its hierarchical structure. Still fighting the ghost of behaviorism and its linear stimulus-response paradigm, with its over a centurylong dominance in American psychology, Tomasello emphasizes the importance of agency in behavioral flexibility under uncertainty, produced by the environment and first of all by other agents, as an evolutionarily beneficial property of behavior. For discussion of the main steps in the development of agency through evolution, Tomasello selects four “model objects,” partly representing extinct species which are unavailable for immediate investigation: lizards as an example of early agency subserving foraging behavior, squirrels, primates, and humans. Each species embodies the rise of a new form of agentive organization, which Tomasello labels as goal-directed agency, intentional agency, rational
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
代理、活动和生物控制论:迈克尔·托马塞洛的《代理的进化》
在他的新书中,Michael Tomasello谈到了代理,这个话题最近成为哲学家(见Bayne, 2013)和神经科学家(例如,gr nbaum & Christensen, 2020;Haggard, 2017),这一直是心理学活动理论的一个关键话题(Leontiev, 1978)。能动性是指执行有意行为的能力,或者更一般地说,是指目标导向的行为。在文化历史活动理论(CHAT)中,最近关于能动性的讨论远远超出了这一基本定义,包括公民参与、行动主义和社会变革等现象(Hopwood, 2022;Stetsenko, 2020)。然而,将代理定义为“改变活动背景的主动性和承诺”(Kajamaa & Kumpulainen, 2019, p. 201),如下所示,这与托马塞洛概述的代理演变的总体趋势是一致的。共同意向性作为智人及其进化的关键特征的著名研究者(例如,Tomasello & Carpenter, 2007;Tomasello et al., 2005), Tomasello在这里转向了意向性本身的一个更基本的问题。他提出了一个雄心勃勃的目标来追踪代理的演变,他认为代理是心理学的“第一原则”,或者是“行为和心理过程运作的组织框架”(Tomasello, 2022, p. 134)。更重要的是,这一原则结合了生态和社会因素,连接了生物和文化进化——这也是活动理论固有的一项努力(Engeström, 1987;Leontyev, 1981)。自20世纪初以来,美国心理学和CHAT分别发展,美国和苏联研究人员之间的交流中断。在那个世纪中叶,CHAT研究主要在“铁幕”后面的苏联激增(更详细的讨论,见Toulmin, 1978),我们仍然观察到它的后果。托马塞洛显然已经意识到维果茨基文化历史方法对人类思维起源和特征的探索,甚至为其实证做出了贡献(例如,Moll & Tomasello, 2007),但他在新书中把这个框架放在一边,这有点令人惊讶。与此同时,我们很容易将他的一些推测和假设与活动理论的进化方面进行比较,活动理论不仅植根于马克思主义哲学,而且植根于上世纪上半叶的俄罗斯进化生物学、比较生物学和神经科学。托马塞洛的书读起来更像是一篇详细而有些重复的文章,阐述了解释代理的几个基本原则,即:反馈(或目标状态与当前状态之间的多重比较)和行为调节的水平(“层次”),或其等级结构。托马塞洛仍然在与行为主义及其在美国心理学中统治了一个多世纪的线性刺激-反应范式的幽灵作斗争,他强调代理在不确定性下的行为灵活性中的重要性,这种灵活性是由环境产生的,首先是由其他代理产生的,作为一种进化上有益的行为属性。为了讨论通过进化发展代理的主要步骤,托马塞洛选择了四个“模型对象”,这些对象部分代表了无法立即进行调查的灭绝物种:蜥蜴作为早期代理觅食行为的例子,松鼠,灵长类动物和人类。每个物种都体现了一种新形式的代理组织的兴起,托马塞洛将其称为目标导向的代理,有意识的代理,理性的代理
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Mind Culture and Activity
Mind Culture and Activity EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH-
CiteScore
4.00
自引率
15.80%
发文量
24
期刊介绍: Mind, Culture, and Activity (MCA) is an interdisciplinary, international journal devoted to the study of the human mind in its cultural and historical contexts. Articles appearing in MCA draw upon research and theory in a variety of disciplines, including anthropology, cognitive science, education, linguistics, psychology, and sociology. Particular emphasis is placed upon research that seeks to resolve methodological problems associated with the analysis of human action in everyday activities and theoretical approaches that place culture and activity at the center of attempts to understand human nature.
期刊最新文献
Creative spaces in and beyond education Hacker education and transformative agency: an empirical study in hacker communities and in the school case of Conexão Escola-Mundo Making the library of the future identifying the zone of proximal development for a Norwegian public library makerspace Action Research in STEM and English Language Learning: An Integrated Approach for Developing Teacher Researchers La Mariposita ¿La recuerda?: the affective and moral dimensions of professional vision in learning to play the violin
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1