Sobering up after "Partisan Intoxication or Policy Voting?"

IF 1.5 3区 社会学 Q2 POLITICAL SCIENCE Quarterly Journal of Political Science Pub Date : 2020-04-08 DOI:10.1561/100.00019039
Steven Rogers
{"title":"Sobering up after \"Partisan Intoxication or Policy Voting?\"","authors":"Steven Rogers","doi":"10.1561/100.00019039","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"“Partisan Intoxication or Policy Voting?” raises questions central to understanding the extent to which individuals vote their partisanship and brings important attention to the potential observational equivalence between partisan and policy voting. In this response, I affirm some of Fowler’s arguments but also build upon existing studies to highlight that tests of the policy voting hypothesis need to seriously consider both the direct and indirect effects of partisanship to understand the relative role of policy versus partisanship. Such consideration is particularly significant as partisanship’s indirect effects can have troubling implications for democracy. I also reexamine the southern realignment and voters’ responses to hypothetical candidate policy positions, and when accounting for elite decision-making and complex information environments, I find voters respond less to candidate ideology and policy positions than suggested by Fowler’s original analyses. Together, my findings underscore the point that “policy voting and partisan intoxication are neither exhaustive nor mutually exclusive explanations” of voter behavior (Fowler 2019, 5). 1 I thank Michael Barber, Andrew Englehardt, Tyler King, Nolan McCarty, Seth McKee, and John Sides for their helpful feedback and assistance for this response. (2) “Partisan Intoxication or Policy Voting?” raises questions central to understanding the extent to which individuals vote their partisanship and casts doubt that “partisanship is a hell of a drug.” I encourage readers to seriously consider Fowler’s challenges and critiques, which shed important light on what we know about partisanship’s and policy’s role in voter decision making. In this response, I affirm some of Fowler’s assertions but also provide nuance to Fowler’s arguments to bring greater attention to an underemphasized point: “policy voting and partisan intoxication are neither exhaustive nor mutually exclusive explanations” of voter behavior (Fowler 2019, 5) a point that can be at times lost in this intoxicating debate. I respond to many of Fowler’s points in the order originally made. I first broaden Fowler’s challenge to voter behavior scholars and argue that we not only need to find measures of partisanship independent of policy preferences but also continue to understand partisanship’s indirect effects on voting via policy opinions. Second, I build on Fowler’s study of the southern realignment to show that even though older and younger voters experienced the same realignment, those who came of political age prior to the civil rights movement exhibit more intoxicated voting behavior, which would be puzzling if we were in a purely policy driven world. I further highlight that the southern realignment is a story of both voter and elite electoral behavior, and once we account for elite behavior, voters respond less to candidate ideology than suggested by Fowler’s original analyses. Third, I reexamine the survey experiment considered by Fowler and provide new evidence to support the policy voting hypothesis. I also discover survey respondents are less likely to exhibit policy motivated voting behavior in more complex information environments and respond to non-policy characteristics, such as candidates’ age, race, and religion, consistent with explanations of voting behavior rooted in social groups. I conclude by encouraging political scientists from each side of this debate to avoid becoming tribal ourselves, as we may miss important contributions from members of the other “tribe” in the partisanship and policy debate. The New Coke Challenge Fowler poses a challenge to voter behavior scholars, analogous to Krehbiel’s (1993) challenge to those who study Congress. Members of Congress likely join political parties based on their policy preferences, leading to observed correlations between party membership and roll-call behavior. Voters similarly may adopt","PeriodicalId":51622,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Political Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5000,"publicationDate":"2020-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1561/100.00019039","citationCount":"5","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Quarterly Journal of Political Science","FirstCategoryId":"90","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1561/100.00019039","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"POLITICAL SCIENCE","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5

Abstract

“Partisan Intoxication or Policy Voting?” raises questions central to understanding the extent to which individuals vote their partisanship and brings important attention to the potential observational equivalence between partisan and policy voting. In this response, I affirm some of Fowler’s arguments but also build upon existing studies to highlight that tests of the policy voting hypothesis need to seriously consider both the direct and indirect effects of partisanship to understand the relative role of policy versus partisanship. Such consideration is particularly significant as partisanship’s indirect effects can have troubling implications for democracy. I also reexamine the southern realignment and voters’ responses to hypothetical candidate policy positions, and when accounting for elite decision-making and complex information environments, I find voters respond less to candidate ideology and policy positions than suggested by Fowler’s original analyses. Together, my findings underscore the point that “policy voting and partisan intoxication are neither exhaustive nor mutually exclusive explanations” of voter behavior (Fowler 2019, 5). 1 I thank Michael Barber, Andrew Englehardt, Tyler King, Nolan McCarty, Seth McKee, and John Sides for their helpful feedback and assistance for this response. (2) “Partisan Intoxication or Policy Voting?” raises questions central to understanding the extent to which individuals vote their partisanship and casts doubt that “partisanship is a hell of a drug.” I encourage readers to seriously consider Fowler’s challenges and critiques, which shed important light on what we know about partisanship’s and policy’s role in voter decision making. In this response, I affirm some of Fowler’s assertions but also provide nuance to Fowler’s arguments to bring greater attention to an underemphasized point: “policy voting and partisan intoxication are neither exhaustive nor mutually exclusive explanations” of voter behavior (Fowler 2019, 5) a point that can be at times lost in this intoxicating debate. I respond to many of Fowler’s points in the order originally made. I first broaden Fowler’s challenge to voter behavior scholars and argue that we not only need to find measures of partisanship independent of policy preferences but also continue to understand partisanship’s indirect effects on voting via policy opinions. Second, I build on Fowler’s study of the southern realignment to show that even though older and younger voters experienced the same realignment, those who came of political age prior to the civil rights movement exhibit more intoxicated voting behavior, which would be puzzling if we were in a purely policy driven world. I further highlight that the southern realignment is a story of both voter and elite electoral behavior, and once we account for elite behavior, voters respond less to candidate ideology than suggested by Fowler’s original analyses. Third, I reexamine the survey experiment considered by Fowler and provide new evidence to support the policy voting hypothesis. I also discover survey respondents are less likely to exhibit policy motivated voting behavior in more complex information environments and respond to non-policy characteristics, such as candidates’ age, race, and religion, consistent with explanations of voting behavior rooted in social groups. I conclude by encouraging political scientists from each side of this debate to avoid becoming tribal ourselves, as we may miss important contributions from members of the other “tribe” in the partisanship and policy debate. The New Coke Challenge Fowler poses a challenge to voter behavior scholars, analogous to Krehbiel’s (1993) challenge to those who study Congress. Members of Congress likely join political parties based on their policy preferences, leading to observed correlations between party membership and roll-call behavior. Voters similarly may adopt
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
“党派迷醉还是政策投票?”
“党派中毒还是政策投票?”提出了理解个人对其党派投票程度的核心问题,并引起了人们对党派投票和政策投票之间潜在的观察等效性的重要关注。在这一回应中,我肯定了福勒的一些论点,但也在现有研究的基础上强调,政策投票假说的测试需要认真考虑党派偏见的直接和间接影响,以理解政策与党派偏见的相对作用。这种考虑尤其重要,因为党派之争的间接影响可能会对民主产生令人不安的影响。我还重新审视了南方的调整和选民对假设的候选人政策立场的反应,当考虑到精英决策和复杂的信息环境时,我发现选民对候选人意识形态和政策立场的回应比福勒最初的分析所暗示的要少。总之,我的研究结果强调了一点,即“政策投票和党派陶醉既不是对选民行为的详尽解释,也不是相互排斥的解释”(Fowler 2019,5)。1我感谢Michael Barber、Andrew Englehardt、Tyler King、Nolan McCarty、Seth McKee和John Sides为本回复提供的有用反馈和帮助。(2) “党派迷醉还是政策投票?”提出了理解个人在多大程度上投票支持其党派的核心问题,并让人怀疑“党派是一种地狱般的毒品”。我鼓励读者认真考虑福勒的挑战和批评,这为我们所了解的党派偏见和政策在选民决策中的作用提供了重要的线索。在这一回应中,我肯定了福勒的一些断言,但也为福勒的论点提供了细微之处,以引起人们对一个未被充分强调的观点的更多关注:“政策投票和党派陶醉既不是详尽的,也不是相互排斥的解释”(Fowler 2019,5),这一点有时会在这场令人陶醉的辩论中丢失。我按照最初的顺序回应了福勒的许多观点。我首先将福勒的挑战扩大到选民行为学者,并认为我们不仅需要找到独立于政策偏好的党派偏见衡量标准,还需要继续了解党派偏见通过政策意见对投票的间接影响。其次,我以福勒对南部重新调整的研究为基础,表明尽管年长和年轻的选民都经历了同样的重新调整,但那些在民权运动之前达到政治年龄的选民表现出了更为陶醉的投票行为,如果我们生活在一个纯粹由政策驱动的世界里,这将是令人困惑的。我进一步强调,南部的重新调整是一个选民和精英选举行为的故事,一旦我们考虑到精英行为,选民对候选人意识形态的反应就比福勒最初的分析所暗示的要少。第三,我重新审视了Fowler的调查实验,并为政策投票假说提供了新的证据。我还发现,调查对象在更复杂的信息环境中不太可能表现出出于政策动机的投票行为,也不太可能对候选人的年龄、种族和宗教等非政策特征做出反应,这与植根于社会群体的投票行为的解释相一致。最后,我鼓励辩论各方的政治科学家避免自己成为部落,因为我们可能会错过另一个“部落”成员在党派和政策辩论中的重要贡献。新可乐挑战福勒对选民行为学者提出了挑战,类似于Krehbiel(1993)对国会研究人员提出的挑战。国会议员可能会根据他们的政策偏好加入政党,从而观察到党员身份和点名行为之间的相关性。选民同样可以采用
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
3.50
自引率
5.90%
发文量
18
期刊介绍: In the last half-century, social scientists have engaged in a methodologically focused and substantively far-reaching mission to make the study of politics scientific. The mutually reinforcing components in this pursuit are the development of positive theories and the testing of their empirical implications. Although this paradigm has been associated with many advances in the understanding of politics, no leading journal of political science is dedicated primarily to the publication of positive political science.
期刊最新文献
The Reputation Politics of the Filibuster A Letter from the Editors-in-Chief Social Conflict and the Predatory State Overreacting and Posturing: How Accountability and Ideology Shape Executive Policies A Gap in Our Understanding? Reconsidering the Evidence for Partisan Knowledge Gaps
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1