Editorial: Culture and Health

IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Japanese Psychological Research Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI:10.1111/jpr.12420
Keiko Ishii, Yukiko Uchida
{"title":"Editorial: Culture and Health","authors":"Keiko Ishii,&nbsp;Yukiko Uchida","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12420","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The ongoing coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic, which presents a worldwide threat to humans, has focused people's attention on health and prompted the adoption of new behaviors to decrease infection risks. Concurrently, it has become clear that vulnerability to COVID-19 depends on the underlying social system and individual attitudes toward authority. For instance, many Japanese people may note that Americans protesting the government's COVID-19 restrictions and vaccine mandates claim freedom of choice, reflecting the dominant cultural value of individualism in U.S. society. Similarly, the cultural norm of collectivism is associated with people's acceptance of public health interventions (e.g., Lu et al., <span>2021</span>, regarding mask usage). In the <i>2021 World Happiness Report</i>, Sachs (<span>2021</span>) noted how cultural values influence health and preventive behavior.</p><p>Over the past 30 years, cultural psychologists investigating mutual relationships between culture and the mind have demonstrated culturally sanctioned aspects of the self, motivation, emotion, cognition, and so on (e.g., Cohen &amp; Kitayama, <span>2019</span>). With a growing body of evidence for cultural influences on health, the pandemic will hasten progress toward health research taking insights from a cultural psychological approach. While the 12 articles in the current special issue will not directly address the battle against COVID-19, we believe they will prove timely and suggestive for a better understanding of culture's role in health, given the focus on personal responsibility in the present period of crisis and uncertainty.</p><p>In the opening invited article, Miyamoto and Ryff (<span>2022</span>), two leading researchers in the field of culture and health, showed the theoretical and empirical relationships between culture and health. In “Culture and Health: Recent Developments and Future Directions,” they discussed the relationship between culture and health using publicly accessible comparable datasets from Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) and Midlife in Japan (MIDJA). They first reviewed the accumulated evidence on cultural influences on health. Next, they introduced both a micro perspective on biological factors and a macro perspective on socioeconomic inequalities and how these affect the link between culture and health. The review concluded by focusing on the changing historical context surrounding these cross-cultural investigations and the effect of growing economic inequality across cultures on the COVID-19 pandemic.</p><p>The other original papers in this issue can broadly be divided into three categories: “clinical–cultural psychology,” “cultural psychology with cross-cultural experimental datasets,” and “more global approaches using comparative cultures.” First, in the clinical area, in the article entitled “Japanese Clinical Psychologists' Consensus Beliefs about Mental Health: A Mixed-Methods Approach,” Sunohara et al. (<span>2022</span>) applied a research design using the cultural consensus theory (CCT) method to examine shared beliefs about mental health held by Japanese clinical psychologists. The CCT uses qualitative methods to identify culturally salient factors, then factor analysis to quantify the degree of sharing. The authors extracted terms in two areas: “acquisition of beliefs about mental health” and “reform of mental health care in Japan.” This methodology is useful in exploring how consensus about mental health is culturally formed.</p><p>Another example of a clinical-cultural perspective is presented by Norasakkunkit et al. (<span>2022</span>). In the paper entitled “Precarious Lives Predict Culturally Deviant Psychologies: Extending the Psychology of Marginalization from Japan to the US,” Norasakkunkit et al. examined the effects of cultural marginalization in an American population. Previous studies in Japan, using the NEET and Hikikomori Risk Scale (Uchida &amp; Norasakkunkit, <span>2015</span>), have shown that high-risk students have motivational tendencies that differ from the Japanese majority (e.g., Norasakkunkit &amp; Uchida, <span>2011</span>). In the current study, they examined from two other studies whether high-risk students in the United States would have different motivational tendencies from the majority. The studies showed that, as expected, high-risk American undergraduates differed from the majority tendency in their motivation. Based on this evidence, Norasakkunkit et al. discussed the relationship between cultural marginalization and psychological tendencies.</p><p>The fourth article by Han et al. (<span>2022</span>), entitled “Culture and Stress Coping: Cultural Variations in the Endorsement of Primary and Secondary Control Coping for Daily Stress Across European Canadians, East Asian Canadians, and the Japanese,” is a cross-cultural study. By testing European Canadian, Asian Canadian, and Japanese undergraduates, Han et al. examined cultural differences concerning whether people endorse primary and secondary control coping strategies to deal with stressful events. The participants were asked not only what strategy they used, but also what they considered to be the ideal control coping strategies. European Canadians endorsed more primary control than secondary control coping, while neither coping strategy differed in the remaining groups. Also, the cultural difference between primary and secondary control strategies was affected by individuals' levels of independence that differed across cultures. Interestingly, participants overall, in the ideal situation, preferred to use primary rather than secondary control coping, regardless of culture, though the trend implied Japanese people's emphasis on secondary control.</p><p>In the fifth article, “Cultural Differences in Self-Rated Health: The Role of Influence and Adjustment,” Choi and Miyamoto (<span>2022</span>) examined how influence and adjustment strategies contribute to cultural differences in self-rated health (SRH), which is a subjective evaluation of one's health. Previous studies have suggested that East Asian countries (Korea and Japan) tend to show relatively poor SRH, despite having higher life expectancy, compared to countries like the United States (OECD, <span>2019</span>). The results of structural equation modeling showed that Americans have higher influence and positive reappraisal and lower adjustment than Japanese people. These cultural differences partially explain why Americans' SRH is higher than that of Japanese people. These patterns were also detected for more objective health indicators (i.e., chronic disease conditions).</p><p>In the sixth article, “Sharing and Non-sharing Happiness: Evidence from Cross-Cultural Studies in the United States and Japan,” de Almeida et al. (<span>2022</span>) examined United States–Japan cross-cultural differences concerning events in which emotions are shared or not shared with others. With two studies that used a situation-sampling method, the authors suggested that Japanese people feel and express higher degrees of happiness when a situation is shared with others compared to when it is not shared, while this difference did not appear in Americans. This evidence is consistent with previous findings that suggested happiness in Japanese culture is less individualized and more likely to be connected with others' happiness (Fukushima et al., <span>2021</span>; Hitokoto &amp; Uchida, <span>2015</span>).</p><p>The next two papers in the series examined the interaction between environment and genes. In the seventh article, by Matsunaga et al. (<span>2022</span>), entitled “Serotonin Receptor (HTR2A) Gene Polymorphism Modulates Social Sharing of Happiness in Both American and Japanese Adults,” the authors explored the degree to which a serotonin 2A receptor polymorphism (HTR2A rs6311) affects the social sharing of happiness. Consistent with previous findings (Matsunaga et al., <span>2017</span>), the G-carriers in Japan were more likely to feel happy in the presence of a happy friend than those with the AA genotype. Interestingly, the effect of HTR2A was found to be similar in the United States. Given that Matsunaga et al. (<span>2017</span>) suggested a greater association between the social sharing of happiness and empathy in the G-carriers than those with the AA genotype, it is important in future work to investigate the underlying mechanism of the social sharing of happiness and the moderating role of HTR2A.</p><p>In the eighth article, entitled “Test of the Serotonin Transporter Gene × Early Life Stress Interaction Effect on Subjective Well-being and Loneliness Among Japanese Young Adults,” Ohtsubo et al. (<span>2022</span>) tested Japanese undergraduates and investigated the effect of early-life stress. The factors assessed included the quality of their parents' relationship during their childhood, parental violence, parental attention, and family income. The moderating role of serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) was also evaluated. They found that a better quality relationship between their parents, greater parental attention, and lower parental violence increased subjective perception of well-being and reduced loneliness. However, neither the main effect of 5-HTTLPR nor the interaction with early life stress on subjective well-being and loneliness was found, suggesting a failure to conceptually replicate previous findings based on gene and environment interaction effects (e.g., Caspi et al., <span>2003</span>). Given regional differences in the distribution of s and l alleles of 5-HTTLPR (Chiao &amp; Blizinsky <span>2010</span>), this research suggests further work is needed to test the mixed support for interaction between 5-HTTLPR and early-life stress in more controlled cultural contexts.</p><p>Each of the next two papers in the series measures interdependent happiness using the Interdependent Happiness Scale (IHS) developed by Hitokoto and Uchida (<span>2015</span>). In the article by Hommerich et al. (<span>2022</span>), entitled “Determinants of Interdependent Happiness Focusing on the Role of Social Capital: Empirical Insight From Japan,” the authors focused on the relationship between social capital and interdependent happiness (IH). Using data from a large survey conducted in Sapporo, Japan, the authors compared IH with Diener et al.'s (<span>1985</span>) Satisfaction With Life Scale. The authors found that IH has a higher connection with variables related to social capital, such as social affiliation, interpersonal reliance, and social support, than satisfaction with life. In particular, IH was more strongly associated with a sense of belonging to society, particularly when IH was associated with reciprocal norms. Based on this result, the authors argued that IH captures aspects of well-being related to social capital that are not covered by the standard subjective well-being index.</p><p>In the article entitled “Harmony and Aversion in the Face of a Pandemic,” Hitokoto and Adeclas (<span>2022</span>) studied Japanese and French cultural differences in the psychological processes of how people react to the COVID-19 threat. In comparing subjective COVID-19-related symptoms between Japanese and French participants in the spring of 2021, the authors examined a more individualized germ aversion and greater collective defense process through IH. The effect of aversion to infection explained the reduction in symptom occurrence in Japan but not in France. In addition, the effect of IH on symptom occurrence was common in both Japan and France. Thus, the authors argued that a more collectivist and cooperative psychological strategy may be effective in controlling infection during the COVID-19 pandemic.</p><p>In the last paper, entitled “The Cultural Variance Model of Organizational Shame and its Implications on Health and Well-being,” Akutsu et al. (<span>2022</span>) focused on shame in the organizational context. They examined the effect that organizational shame has on individuals' health and subjective well-being and the moderating role played by culture. Through a review of the literature, they proposed a cultural variance model of organizational shame consisting of conformity and status/competition domains. For instance, an organization with high conformity and low status/competition is likely to be found in collectivistic cultures, where the shame of nonconformity leads to the correction of employee behaviors. Conversely, an organization with low conformity and high status/competition is likely to be present in individualistic cultures, where status/competition shame functions as a cue to employees concerning whether staying with or leaving the organization is the best survival tactic in a performance-based and highly mobile labor market. The implications of organizational shame based on their model for occupational health are crucial, particularly given the different effects that nonconformity and status/competition shame have on physiological and stress responses.</p><p>As this special issue demonstrates, evidence on culture and health is emerging in a variety of new frameworks, including international comparisons, national analyses, individual health, and population health. In addition to traditional survey studies, more detailed indicators are becoming available through the measurement of genes and other health indicators, helping to advance the theory. In the current crisis of the worldwide COVID-19 pandemic, we hope these findings will be useful to establish cautions and methodologies for implementing countermeasures in other countries.</p>","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8000,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jpr.12420","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Japanese Psychological Research","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jpr.12420","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

The ongoing coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic, which presents a worldwide threat to humans, has focused people's attention on health and prompted the adoption of new behaviors to decrease infection risks. Concurrently, it has become clear that vulnerability to COVID-19 depends on the underlying social system and individual attitudes toward authority. For instance, many Japanese people may note that Americans protesting the government's COVID-19 restrictions and vaccine mandates claim freedom of choice, reflecting the dominant cultural value of individualism in U.S. society. Similarly, the cultural norm of collectivism is associated with people's acceptance of public health interventions (e.g., Lu et al., 2021, regarding mask usage). In the 2021 World Happiness Report, Sachs (2021) noted how cultural values influence health and preventive behavior.

Over the past 30 years, cultural psychologists investigating mutual relationships between culture and the mind have demonstrated culturally sanctioned aspects of the self, motivation, emotion, cognition, and so on (e.g., Cohen & Kitayama, 2019). With a growing body of evidence for cultural influences on health, the pandemic will hasten progress toward health research taking insights from a cultural psychological approach. While the 12 articles in the current special issue will not directly address the battle against COVID-19, we believe they will prove timely and suggestive for a better understanding of culture's role in health, given the focus on personal responsibility in the present period of crisis and uncertainty.

In the opening invited article, Miyamoto and Ryff (2022), two leading researchers in the field of culture and health, showed the theoretical and empirical relationships between culture and health. In “Culture and Health: Recent Developments and Future Directions,” they discussed the relationship between culture and health using publicly accessible comparable datasets from Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) and Midlife in Japan (MIDJA). They first reviewed the accumulated evidence on cultural influences on health. Next, they introduced both a micro perspective on biological factors and a macro perspective on socioeconomic inequalities and how these affect the link between culture and health. The review concluded by focusing on the changing historical context surrounding these cross-cultural investigations and the effect of growing economic inequality across cultures on the COVID-19 pandemic.

The other original papers in this issue can broadly be divided into three categories: “clinical–cultural psychology,” “cultural psychology with cross-cultural experimental datasets,” and “more global approaches using comparative cultures.” First, in the clinical area, in the article entitled “Japanese Clinical Psychologists' Consensus Beliefs about Mental Health: A Mixed-Methods Approach,” Sunohara et al. (2022) applied a research design using the cultural consensus theory (CCT) method to examine shared beliefs about mental health held by Japanese clinical psychologists. The CCT uses qualitative methods to identify culturally salient factors, then factor analysis to quantify the degree of sharing. The authors extracted terms in two areas: “acquisition of beliefs about mental health” and “reform of mental health care in Japan.” This methodology is useful in exploring how consensus about mental health is culturally formed.

Another example of a clinical-cultural perspective is presented by Norasakkunkit et al. (2022). In the paper entitled “Precarious Lives Predict Culturally Deviant Psychologies: Extending the Psychology of Marginalization from Japan to the US,” Norasakkunkit et al. examined the effects of cultural marginalization in an American population. Previous studies in Japan, using the NEET and Hikikomori Risk Scale (Uchida & Norasakkunkit, 2015), have shown that high-risk students have motivational tendencies that differ from the Japanese majority (e.g., Norasakkunkit & Uchida, 2011). In the current study, they examined from two other studies whether high-risk students in the United States would have different motivational tendencies from the majority. The studies showed that, as expected, high-risk American undergraduates differed from the majority tendency in their motivation. Based on this evidence, Norasakkunkit et al. discussed the relationship between cultural marginalization and psychological tendencies.

The fourth article by Han et al. (2022), entitled “Culture and Stress Coping: Cultural Variations in the Endorsement of Primary and Secondary Control Coping for Daily Stress Across European Canadians, East Asian Canadians, and the Japanese,” is a cross-cultural study. By testing European Canadian, Asian Canadian, and Japanese undergraduates, Han et al. examined cultural differences concerning whether people endorse primary and secondary control coping strategies to deal with stressful events. The participants were asked not only what strategy they used, but also what they considered to be the ideal control coping strategies. European Canadians endorsed more primary control than secondary control coping, while neither coping strategy differed in the remaining groups. Also, the cultural difference between primary and secondary control strategies was affected by individuals' levels of independence that differed across cultures. Interestingly, participants overall, in the ideal situation, preferred to use primary rather than secondary control coping, regardless of culture, though the trend implied Japanese people's emphasis on secondary control.

In the fifth article, “Cultural Differences in Self-Rated Health: The Role of Influence and Adjustment,” Choi and Miyamoto (2022) examined how influence and adjustment strategies contribute to cultural differences in self-rated health (SRH), which is a subjective evaluation of one's health. Previous studies have suggested that East Asian countries (Korea and Japan) tend to show relatively poor SRH, despite having higher life expectancy, compared to countries like the United States (OECD, 2019). The results of structural equation modeling showed that Americans have higher influence and positive reappraisal and lower adjustment than Japanese people. These cultural differences partially explain why Americans' SRH is higher than that of Japanese people. These patterns were also detected for more objective health indicators (i.e., chronic disease conditions).

In the sixth article, “Sharing and Non-sharing Happiness: Evidence from Cross-Cultural Studies in the United States and Japan,” de Almeida et al. (2022) examined United States–Japan cross-cultural differences concerning events in which emotions are shared or not shared with others. With two studies that used a situation-sampling method, the authors suggested that Japanese people feel and express higher degrees of happiness when a situation is shared with others compared to when it is not shared, while this difference did not appear in Americans. This evidence is consistent with previous findings that suggested happiness in Japanese culture is less individualized and more likely to be connected with others' happiness (Fukushima et al., 2021; Hitokoto & Uchida, 2015).

The next two papers in the series examined the interaction between environment and genes. In the seventh article, by Matsunaga et al. (2022), entitled “Serotonin Receptor (HTR2A) Gene Polymorphism Modulates Social Sharing of Happiness in Both American and Japanese Adults,” the authors explored the degree to which a serotonin 2A receptor polymorphism (HTR2A rs6311) affects the social sharing of happiness. Consistent with previous findings (Matsunaga et al., 2017), the G-carriers in Japan were more likely to feel happy in the presence of a happy friend than those with the AA genotype. Interestingly, the effect of HTR2A was found to be similar in the United States. Given that Matsunaga et al. (2017) suggested a greater association between the social sharing of happiness and empathy in the G-carriers than those with the AA genotype, it is important in future work to investigate the underlying mechanism of the social sharing of happiness and the moderating role of HTR2A.

In the eighth article, entitled “Test of the Serotonin Transporter Gene × Early Life Stress Interaction Effect on Subjective Well-being and Loneliness Among Japanese Young Adults,” Ohtsubo et al. (2022) tested Japanese undergraduates and investigated the effect of early-life stress. The factors assessed included the quality of their parents' relationship during their childhood, parental violence, parental attention, and family income. The moderating role of serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) was also evaluated. They found that a better quality relationship between their parents, greater parental attention, and lower parental violence increased subjective perception of well-being and reduced loneliness. However, neither the main effect of 5-HTTLPR nor the interaction with early life stress on subjective well-being and loneliness was found, suggesting a failure to conceptually replicate previous findings based on gene and environment interaction effects (e.g., Caspi et al., 2003). Given regional differences in the distribution of s and l alleles of 5-HTTLPR (Chiao & Blizinsky 2010), this research suggests further work is needed to test the mixed support for interaction between 5-HTTLPR and early-life stress in more controlled cultural contexts.

Each of the next two papers in the series measures interdependent happiness using the Interdependent Happiness Scale (IHS) developed by Hitokoto and Uchida (2015). In the article by Hommerich et al. (2022), entitled “Determinants of Interdependent Happiness Focusing on the Role of Social Capital: Empirical Insight From Japan,” the authors focused on the relationship between social capital and interdependent happiness (IH). Using data from a large survey conducted in Sapporo, Japan, the authors compared IH with Diener et al.'s (1985) Satisfaction With Life Scale. The authors found that IH has a higher connection with variables related to social capital, such as social affiliation, interpersonal reliance, and social support, than satisfaction with life. In particular, IH was more strongly associated with a sense of belonging to society, particularly when IH was associated with reciprocal norms. Based on this result, the authors argued that IH captures aspects of well-being related to social capital that are not covered by the standard subjective well-being index.

In the article entitled “Harmony and Aversion in the Face of a Pandemic,” Hitokoto and Adeclas (2022) studied Japanese and French cultural differences in the psychological processes of how people react to the COVID-19 threat. In comparing subjective COVID-19-related symptoms between Japanese and French participants in the spring of 2021, the authors examined a more individualized germ aversion and greater collective defense process through IH. The effect of aversion to infection explained the reduction in symptom occurrence in Japan but not in France. In addition, the effect of IH on symptom occurrence was common in both Japan and France. Thus, the authors argued that a more collectivist and cooperative psychological strategy may be effective in controlling infection during the COVID-19 pandemic.

In the last paper, entitled “The Cultural Variance Model of Organizational Shame and its Implications on Health and Well-being,” Akutsu et al. (2022) focused on shame in the organizational context. They examined the effect that organizational shame has on individuals' health and subjective well-being and the moderating role played by culture. Through a review of the literature, they proposed a cultural variance model of organizational shame consisting of conformity and status/competition domains. For instance, an organization with high conformity and low status/competition is likely to be found in collectivistic cultures, where the shame of nonconformity leads to the correction of employee behaviors. Conversely, an organization with low conformity and high status/competition is likely to be present in individualistic cultures, where status/competition shame functions as a cue to employees concerning whether staying with or leaving the organization is the best survival tactic in a performance-based and highly mobile labor market. The implications of organizational shame based on their model for occupational health are crucial, particularly given the different effects that nonconformity and status/competition shame have on physiological and stress responses.

As this special issue demonstrates, evidence on culture and health is emerging in a variety of new frameworks, including international comparisons, national analyses, individual health, and population health. In addition to traditional survey studies, more detailed indicators are becoming available through the measurement of genes and other health indicators, helping to advance the theory. In the current crisis of the worldwide COVID-19 pandemic, we hope these findings will be useful to establish cautions and methodologies for implementing countermeasures in other countries.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
社论:文化与健康
正在进行的冠状病毒(COVID-19)大流行对人类构成全球威胁,使人们的注意力集中在健康上,并促使采取新的行为方式来降低感染风险。与此同时,很明显,对COVID-19的脆弱性取决于潜在的社会制度和个人对权威的态度。例如,许多日本人可能会注意到,美国人抗议政府的COVID-19限制和疫苗要求,声称有选择的自由,这反映了美国社会中占主导地位的个人主义文化价值。同样,集体主义的文化规范与人们对公共卫生干预措施的接受程度有关(例如,Lu et al., 2021,关于口罩的使用)。在《2021年世界幸福报告》中,萨克斯(2021)指出了文化价值观如何影响健康和预防行为。在过去的30年里,文化心理学家研究了文化和心智之间的相互关系,证明了自我、动机、情感、认知等方面的文化认可(例如,Cohen &Kitayama, 2019)。随着越来越多的证据表明文化对健康的影响,大流行将加速从文化心理学方法中获得见解的健康研究的进展。虽然本期特刊中的12篇文章不会直接涉及抗击COVID-19的斗争,但我们相信,鉴于当前危机和不确定时期对个人责任的关注,它们将及时并有助于更好地理解文化在卫生中的作用。在开幕特邀文章中,Miyamoto和Ryff(2022)这两位文化与健康领域的主要研究者展示了文化与健康之间的理论和实证关系。在“文化与健康:最近的发展和未来的方向”中,他们使用来自美国中年(MIDUS)和日本中年(MIDJA)的可公开访问的可比数据集讨论了文化与健康之间的关系。他们首先回顾了文化对健康影响的累积证据。接下来,他们介绍了生物因素的微观视角和社会经济不平等的宏观视角,以及这些因素如何影响文化与健康之间的联系。该综述最后重点关注了这些跨文化调查不断变化的历史背景,以及不同文化之间日益严重的经济不平等对COVID-19大流行的影响。本期的其他原创论文可以大致分为三类:“临床文化心理学”、“跨文化实验数据集的文化心理学”和“使用比较文化的更全球化方法”。首先,在临床领域,在题为“日本临床心理学家关于心理健康的共识信念:一种混合方法”的文章中,Sunohara等人(2022)采用了一种使用文化共识理论(CCT)方法的研究设计来检查日本临床心理学家对心理健康的共同信念。CCT采用定性方法识别文化显著因素,然后通过因子分析量化共享程度。作者从两个方面提取了术语:“获得关于心理健康的信念”和“日本心理保健的改革”。这种方法在探索关于心理健康的共识是如何在文化上形成的方面是有用的。Norasakkunkit等人(2022)提出了另一个临床文化视角的例子。Norasakkunkit等人在题为《不稳定的生活预示着文化偏差心理:将边缘化心理学从日本扩展到美国》的论文中,研究了文化边缘化对美国人群的影响。先前在日本的研究,使用NEET和Hikikomori风险量表(Uchida &Norasakkunkit, 2015)的研究表明,高危学生的动机倾向与日本大多数人不同(例如,Norasakkunkit &田,2011)。在目前的研究中,他们从另外两项研究中考察了美国高危学生是否与大多数人有不同的动机倾向。研究表明,正如预期的那样,高风险的美国大学生的动机与大多数人的倾向不同。基于这一证据,Norasakkunkit等人讨论了文化边缘化与心理倾向之间的关系。Han等人(2022)的第四篇文章,题为“文化和压力应对:欧洲加拿大人、东亚加拿大人和日本人对日常压力的主要和次要控制应对的认可中的文化差异”,是一项跨文化研究。Han等人通过对欧洲裔加拿大人、亚裔加拿大人和日本本科生的测试,研究了人们在处理压力事件时是否支持主要和次要控制应对策略方面的文化差异。 利用在日本札幌进行的一项大型调查的数据,作者将IH与Diener等人(1985)的生活满意度量表进行了比较。作者发现,与生活满意度相比,IH与社会资本相关的变量(如社会隶属关系、人际依赖和社会支持)有更高的联系。特别是,IH与社会归属感联系更紧密,特别是当IH与互惠规范联系在一起时。基于这一结果,作者认为IH捕获了与社会资本相关的幸福方面,而这些方面没有被标准的主观幸福指数所涵盖。Hitokoto和Adeclas(2022)在题为“面对流行病的和谐与厌恶”的文章中,研究了日本和法国在人们如何应对COVID-19威胁的心理过程中的文化差异。在比较2021年春季日本和法国参与者的主观covid -19相关症状时,作者通过IH研究了更个性化的细菌厌恶和更大的集体防御过程。厌恶感染的影响解释了日本症状发生率的减少,而法国没有。此外,IH对症状发生的影响在日本和法国都很常见。因此,作者认为,在COVID-19大流行期间,一种更加集体主义和合作的心理策略可能有效地控制感染。在最后一篇题为“组织羞耻感的文化差异模型及其对健康和福祉的影响”的论文中,Akutsu等人(2022)专注于组织背景下的羞耻感。他们研究了组织羞耻感对个人健康和主观幸福感的影响,以及文化所起的调节作用。通过对文献的回顾,他们提出了一个由从众和地位/竞争域组成的组织羞耻感的文化差异模型。例如,在集体主义文化中,很可能会发现一个高度从众和低地位/竞争的组织,在集体主义文化中,不从众的羞耻导致了员工行为的纠正。相反,一个低顺从和高地位/竞争的组织很可能存在于个人主义文化中,在那里,地位/竞争羞耻作为一种暗示,让员工考虑在基于绩效和高度流动的劳动力市场中,留在还是离开组织是最好的生存策略。基于他们的职业健康模型的组织羞耻感的含义是至关重要的,特别是考虑到不服从和地位/竞争羞耻感对生理和压力反应的不同影响。正如本期特刊所示,有关文化和健康的证据正在各种新框架中出现,包括国际比较、国家分析、个人健康和人口健康。除了传统的调查研究外,通过测量基因和其他健康指标,可以获得更详细的指标,这有助于推进这一理论。在当前全球COVID-19大流行的危机中,我们希望这些发现将有助于制定其他国家实施对策的注意事项和方法。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Japanese Psychological Research
Japanese Psychological Research PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY-
CiteScore
2.30
自引率
0.00%
发文量
48
期刊介绍: Each volume of Japanese Psychological Research features original contributions from members of the Japanese Psychological Association and other leading international researchers. The journal"s analysis of problem-orientated research contributes significantly to all fields of psychology and raises awareness of psychological research in Japan amongst psychologists world-wide.
期刊最新文献
What Makes a Movement Human‐Like? Development of a Japanese Version of the Creative Mindset Scale (CMS) Using Item Response Theory Issue Information Issue Information Perceptual Discrepancies of Body Sizes in Japanese Female College Students: Using a 3‐D Silhouette Scale
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1