首页 > 最新文献

Japanese Psychological Research最新文献

英文 中文
What Makes a Movement Human‐Like? 是什么让运动变得像人?
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-08-09 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12542
Xiaoyue Yang, Miao Cheng, Ken Fujiwara, Y. Kitamura, Satoshi Shioiri, Chiahuei Tseng
With the advancement of AI‐generated human motion, it is of increasing importance to think about how we distinguish real human motion from machine‐generated movements. In this study, we recruited professional performers to use the whole body to make a short movement to inform potential observers that they are real humans (instead of machines). Their movements were captured with a motion capture system (Vicon) and later reduced to dynamic point‐like displays (biological motion). They were interviewed after the recording to provide their acting strategies. Naive observers who did not participate in the motion data collection were recruited to watch these videos and judge whether the biological motions looked human‐like or not (YES/NO), as well as to report their judging criteria. The major factors extracted from these reports include kinematics, context, body mechanics, and principles of physical laws. We discuss the impact of these criteria and how they may possibly help improve the future generation of human‐like motions.
随着人工智能生成人体动作技术的发展,思考如何将真实的人体动作与机器生成的动作区分开来变得越来越重要。在这项研究中,我们招募了专业表演者,让他们用整个身体做一个简短的动作,让潜在的观察者知道他们是真正的人类(而不是机器)。他们的动作由动作捕捉系统(Vicon)捕捉,随后还原成动态点状显示(生物动作)。录制结束后对他们进行访谈,以了解他们的表演策略。我们还招募了未参与动作数据收集的天真观察者观看这些视频,判断生物动作是否像人类(是/否),并报告他们的判断标准。从这些报告中提取的主要因素包括运动学、上下文、身体力学和物理定律原理。我们将讨论这些标准的影响,以及它们可能如何帮助改进未来类人动作的生成。
{"title":"What Makes a Movement Human‐Like?","authors":"Xiaoyue Yang, Miao Cheng, Ken Fujiwara, Y. Kitamura, Satoshi Shioiri, Chiahuei Tseng","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12542","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12542","url":null,"abstract":"With the advancement of AI‐generated human motion, it is of increasing importance to think about how we distinguish real human motion from machine‐generated movements. In this study, we recruited professional performers to use the whole body to make a short movement to inform potential observers that they are real humans (instead of machines). Their movements were captured with a motion capture system (Vicon) and later reduced to dynamic point‐like displays (biological motion). They were interviewed after the recording to provide their acting strategies. Naive observers who did not participate in the motion data collection were recruited to watch these videos and judge whether the biological motions looked human‐like or not (YES/NO), as well as to report their judging criteria. The major factors extracted from these reports include kinematics, context, body mechanics, and principles of physical laws. We discuss the impact of these criteria and how they may possibly help improve the future generation of human‐like motions.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141924112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development of a Japanese Version of the Creative Mindset Scale (CMS) Using Item Response Theory 运用项目反应理论编制日语版创意思维量表(CMS)
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12544
C. Ishiguro, Kazuki Matsumoto, Takumitsu Agata, Hiroyuki Noguchi, Takeshi Okada
The purpose of this study was to develop the Japanese version of the Creative Mindset Scale (CMS‐J) and examine its psychometric properties using classical test theory (CTT) and item response theory (IRT). Three surveys were conducted to examine the psychometric properties. Study 1 (n = 700) confirmed the conceptual structure of the CMS‐J. Confirmatory factor analyses of the CMS‐J suggested a two‐factor structure, as in the original scale. The test–retest reliability was examined based on data from 143 participants who responded to the CMS‐J questionnaire after a 7‐week interval. Study 2 (n = 1,114) confirmed the convergent and discriminant validities, as well as the internal consistency, of the CMS‐J. Finally, Study 3 (n = 1,300) confirmed the effective measurement of growth and fixed mindset of the CMS‐J using IRT analyses. The present findings suggest that the CMS‐J has a fundamental psychometric property.
本研究旨在开发日语版创造性思维量表(CMS-J),并运用经典测验理论(CTT)和项目反应理论(IRT)检验其心理测量学特性。我们进行了三次调查来检验其心理测量学特性。研究 1(n = 700)确认了 CMS-J 的概念结构。对 CMS-J 进行的确认性因素分析表明,与原始量表一样,CMS-J 具有双因素结构。根据 143 名参与者在间隔 7 周后回答 CMS-J 问卷的数据,对测试-再测可靠性进行了检验。研究 2(n = 1 114)证实了 CMS-J 的收敛效度和区分效度以及内部一致性。最后,研究 3(n = 1,300 人)通过 IRT 分析证实了 CMS-J 能有效测量成长型和固定型思维模式。本研究结果表明,CMS-J 具有基本的心理测量特性。
{"title":"Development of a Japanese Version of the Creative Mindset Scale (CMS) Using Item Response Theory","authors":"C. Ishiguro, Kazuki Matsumoto, Takumitsu Agata, Hiroyuki Noguchi, Takeshi Okada","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12544","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12544","url":null,"abstract":"The purpose of this study was to develop the Japanese version of the Creative Mindset Scale (CMS‐J) and examine its psychometric properties using classical test theory (CTT) and item response theory (IRT). Three surveys were conducted to examine the psychometric properties. Study 1 (n = 700) confirmed the conceptual structure of the CMS‐J. Confirmatory factor analyses of the CMS‐J suggested a two‐factor structure, as in the original scale. The test–retest reliability was examined based on data from 143 participants who responded to the CMS‐J questionnaire after a 7‐week interval. Study 2 (n = 1,114) confirmed the convergent and discriminant validities, as well as the internal consistency, of the CMS‐J. Finally, Study 3 (n = 1,300) confirmed the effective measurement of growth and fixed mindset of the CMS‐J using IRT analyses. The present findings suggest that the CMS‐J has a fundamental psychometric property.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141925561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual Discrepancies of Body Sizes in Japanese Female College Students: Using a 3‐D Silhouette Scale 日本女大学生对身体尺寸的感知差异:使用三维轮廓量表
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-15 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12534
Yuko Yamamiya, Tomohiro Suzuki, Takayo Mukai
Body image comprises a perceptual evaluation of one's body. We examined perceptual discrepancies of own and others' body sizes in young Japanese women. Moreover, we investigated if thin‐ideal internalization and appearance‐comparison tendency were associated with the discrepancies. A sample of 173 young Japanese women (Mage = 20.11 years, SD = 1.50 years) completed a figural/silhouette scale questionnaire created based on the national anthropometric data and measures of thin‐ideal internalization and appearance‐comparison tendency. Results indicated that the participants selected a larger figure for themselves than their body mass index (BMI) represented, while they selected a slimmer figure for their ideal body than their ideal BMI represented. In addition, the extent of the perceptual discrepancies was correlated with thin‐ideal internalization and appearance‐comparison tendency. Moreover, participants expressed body dissatisfaction to a greater extent when using a silhouette scale than when using BMI. Findings are discussed in relation to abundant visual images of attractive bodies in society, which may lead to body‐size perceptual discrepancies and misjudgments, body dissatisfaction, and eventual weight‐loss behaviors. Implications for intervention are also discussed.
身体形象包括对自己身体的感知评价。我们研究了日本年轻女性对自己和他人身体尺寸的感知差异。此外,我们还研究了瘦身理想的内在化和外貌比较倾向是否与差异有关。173 名日本年轻女性(年龄:20.11 岁,平均年龄:1.50 岁)完成了一份根据全国人体测量数据制作的形象/轮廓量表问卷,并对瘦身理想内化和外貌比较倾向进行了测量。结果表明,参与者为自己选择的形象比其体重指数(BMI)所代表的要大,而他们为理想身材选择的形象比其理想体重指数所代表的要苗条。此外,知觉差异的程度与瘦弱理想的内在化和外貌比较倾向相关。此外,与使用体重指数时相比,参与者在使用轮廓量表时对身体表示不满意的程度更高。研究结果与社会上大量有吸引力的身体视觉形象有关,这可能会导致身体尺寸感知差异和错误判断、身体不满意以及最终的减肥行为。此外,还讨论了干预的意义。
{"title":"Perceptual Discrepancies of Body Sizes in Japanese Female College Students: Using a 3‐D Silhouette Scale","authors":"Yuko Yamamiya, Tomohiro Suzuki, Takayo Mukai","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12534","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12534","url":null,"abstract":"Body image comprises a perceptual evaluation of one's body. We examined perceptual discrepancies of own and others' body sizes in young Japanese women. Moreover, we investigated if thin‐ideal internalization and appearance‐comparison tendency were associated with the discrepancies. A sample of 173 young Japanese women (Mage = 20.11 years, SD = 1.50 years) completed a figural/silhouette scale questionnaire created based on the national anthropometric data and measures of thin‐ideal internalization and appearance‐comparison tendency. Results indicated that the participants selected a larger figure for themselves than their body mass index (BMI) represented, while they selected a slimmer figure for their ideal body than their ideal BMI represented. In addition, the extent of the perceptual discrepancies was correlated with thin‐ideal internalization and appearance‐comparison tendency. Moreover, participants expressed body dissatisfaction to a greater extent when using a silhouette scale than when using BMI. Findings are discussed in relation to abundant visual images of attractive bodies in society, which may lead to body‐size perceptual discrepancies and misjudgments, body dissatisfaction, and eventual weight‐loss behaviors. Implications for intervention are also discussed.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141336825","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial Mimicry in Unstructured Dyadic Interactions: A Cross‐Cultural Study 非结构化二元互动中的面部模仿:跨文化研究
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-14 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12528
Yi‐Chun Chuang, Ken Fujiwara
Facial mimicry, known as the tendency to imitate other individuals' facial muscular movements, is key to establishing successful interpersonal relationships. Studies on various cultures have confirmed this tendency, suggesting that facial mimicry is a universal behavioral pattern in interpersonal communication. However, its cross‐cultural variability is expected because of the influence of cultural norms regarding facial expressions. By focusing on unstructured dyadic interactions, we addressed the unexplored cross‐cultural variability of facial mimicry through a secondary analysis of video data obtained in Japan and Spain and a further analysis of newly collected videos in Taiwan. We also explored the association between the Big Five personality traits and facial mimicry cross‐culturally. To measure facial mimicry, the intensity of each facial action unit movement was quantified using OpenFace, which underwent multidimensional dynamic time warping. First, employing the pseudo‐synchrony paradigm of random data shuffling, we confirmed that interactants displayed facial mimicry beyond chance during their conversation. Second, we found that the extent of mimicry was significantly different among the three cultures; that is, interactants from East Asian cultures (Taiwan, Japan) showed a greater extent of mimicry than those from Western cultures (Spain) even after controlling for unbalanced gender composition in the data. Third, conscientiousness was significantly associated with the degree of mimicry. However, the association between personality traits and mimicry was not consistent across cultures. Overall, this study discusses the importance of cross‐cultural research in facial mimicry, as in the study of facial expressions.
面部模仿,即模仿他人面部肌肉运动的倾向,是建立成功人际关系的关键。对不同文化的研究证实了这种倾向,表明面部模仿是人际交往中的一种普遍行为模式。然而,由于受到有关面部表情的文化规范的影响,其跨文化变异性也是意料之中的。通过对在日本和西班牙获得的视频数据进行二次分析,并对在台湾新收集的视频数据进行进一步分析,我们聚焦于非结构化的二人互动,探讨了面部模仿的跨文化变异性。我们还探讨了大五人格特质与面部模仿之间的跨文化关联。为了测量面部模仿,我们使用 OpenFace 对每个面部动作单元运动的强度进行了量化,并对其进行了多维动态时间扭曲。首先,利用随机数据洗牌的伪同步范式,我们证实了互动者在对话中表现出的面部模仿超出了偶然性。其次,我们发现模仿的程度在三种文化中存在显著差异;也就是说,即使控制了数据中不平衡的性别组成,来自东亚文化(台湾、日本)的互动者也比来自西方文化(西班牙)的互动者表现出更大程度的模仿。第三,自觉性与模仿程度显著相关。然而,不同文化中人格特质与模仿之间的关系并不一致。总之,本研究探讨了面部模仿的跨文化研究的重要性,正如面部表情研究一样。
{"title":"Facial Mimicry in Unstructured Dyadic Interactions: A Cross‐Cultural Study","authors":"Yi‐Chun Chuang, Ken Fujiwara","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12528","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12528","url":null,"abstract":"Facial mimicry, known as the tendency to imitate other individuals' facial muscular movements, is key to establishing successful interpersonal relationships. Studies on various cultures have confirmed this tendency, suggesting that facial mimicry is a universal behavioral pattern in interpersonal communication. However, its cross‐cultural variability is expected because of the influence of cultural norms regarding facial expressions. By focusing on unstructured dyadic interactions, we addressed the unexplored cross‐cultural variability of facial mimicry through a secondary analysis of video data obtained in Japan and Spain and a further analysis of newly collected videos in Taiwan. We also explored the association between the Big Five personality traits and facial mimicry cross‐culturally. To measure facial mimicry, the intensity of each facial action unit movement was quantified using OpenFace, which underwent multidimensional dynamic time warping. First, employing the pseudo‐synchrony paradigm of random data shuffling, we confirmed that interactants displayed facial mimicry beyond chance during their conversation. Second, we found that the extent of mimicry was significantly different among the three cultures; that is, interactants from East Asian cultures (Taiwan, Japan) showed a greater extent of mimicry than those from Western cultures (Spain) even after controlling for unbalanced gender composition in the data. Third, conscientiousness was significantly associated with the degree of mimicry. However, the association between personality traits and mimicry was not consistent across cultures. Overall, this study discusses the importance of cross‐cultural research in facial mimicry, as in the study of facial expressions.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141344574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Possible Futures for the Psychological Network Approach: Agenda for Clinical Scientists 心理网络方法的可能未来:临床科学家议程
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12538
Jun Kashihara, Daichi Sugawara, Yoshihiko Kunisato, Y. Takebayashi, Shun Nakajima, Masaya Ito
The psychological network approach, which conceptualizes mental disorders as complex systems and provides a statistical toolbox to estimate symptom networks, has evoked innovations in psychopathology research. This perspective paper discusses how to implement this approach in real‐world clinical settings, emphasizing that clinical scientists should motivate clinicians and clients to utilize it. To promote the clinical use of the psychological network approach, we provide the following five steps clinical scientists should undertake: (a) provide clinicians and clients with the necessary background knowledge, (b) encourage clinicians to use the narrative network models, (c) provide clinicians with user‐friendly software to conduct empirical network analyses, (d) explore clients' experiences during the pilot trials using the psychological network approach, and (e) develop guidelines for interventions using the psychological network approach through the collaboration of clinical scientists, clinicians, and clients. We close the paper by discussing the importance of bridging the academic field and clinical settings, in the hopes that other clinical scientists will participate in the discussion of possible futures for the psychological network approach.
心理网络方法将精神障碍概念化为复杂的系统,并提供了一个估算症状网络的统计工具箱,该方法在精神病理学研究中引发了创新。本视角论文讨论了如何在现实世界的临床环境中实施这种方法,强调临床科学家应激励临床医生和客户使用这种方法。为促进心理网络方法在临床上的应用,我们提出了临床科学家应采取的以下五个步骤:(a)为临床医生和客户提供必要的背景知识;(b)鼓励临床医生使用叙事网络模型;(c)为临床医生提供用户友好型软件以进行实证网络分析;(d)在使用心理网络方法的试点试验中探索客户的经验;以及(e)通过临床科学家、临床医生和客户的合作,制定使用心理网络方法进行干预的指南。最后,我们讨论了在学术领域和临床环境之间架起桥梁的重要性,希望其他临床科学家也能参与讨论心理网络疗法可能的未来。
{"title":"Possible Futures for the Psychological Network Approach: Agenda for Clinical Scientists","authors":"Jun Kashihara, Daichi Sugawara, Yoshihiko Kunisato, Y. Takebayashi, Shun Nakajima, Masaya Ito","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12538","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12538","url":null,"abstract":"The psychological network approach, which conceptualizes mental disorders as complex systems and provides a statistical toolbox to estimate symptom networks, has evoked innovations in psychopathology research. This perspective paper discusses how to implement this approach in real‐world clinical settings, emphasizing that clinical scientists should motivate clinicians and clients to utilize it. To promote the clinical use of the psychological network approach, we provide the following five steps clinical scientists should undertake: (a) provide clinicians and clients with the necessary background knowledge, (b) encourage clinicians to use the narrative network models, (c) provide clinicians with user‐friendly software to conduct empirical network analyses, (d) explore clients' experiences during the pilot trials using the psychological network approach, and (e) develop guidelines for interventions using the psychological network approach through the collaboration of clinical scientists, clinicians, and clients. We close the paper by discussing the importance of bridging the academic field and clinical settings, in the hopes that other clinical scientists will participate in the discussion of possible futures for the psychological network approach.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141344974","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effect of a Defusion Exercise Including a Hierarchical Procedure on Depressive Rumination 包括分层程序在内的排解练习对抑郁反刍的影响
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-11 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12533
Yuki Shigemoto, Ian Stewart, Takashi Muto
This study examined the effect of a defusion exercise with a hierarchical procedure in the context of depression, depressive rumination, and behavioral inflexibility. A total of 38 undergraduates were randomly assigned to either a defusion or control group. All participants answered questionnaires and engaged in a behavioral flexibility task before and after intervention. For the intervention, the defusion group engaged in a hierarchical defusion procedure. They sorted their depressive private events into either thoughts, feelings, or sensations, and all of them were integrated to “depression.” Subsequently, they engaged in word‐repetition exercises for “depression.” The control group read an emotionally neutral article on Japanese culture for 5 min. Results showed that the defusion exercise decreased cognitive fusion regarding depression. However, it did not decrease depressive rumination or promote behavioral flexibility. These results suggest that adjustments in the implementation of defusion exercises and other interventions may be necessary to reduce depressive rumination and promote behavioral flexibility.
本研究考察了在抑郁、抑郁性反刍和行为不灵活的背景下,采用分层程序进行化解练习的效果。共有 38 名本科生被随机分配到化解组或对照组。所有参与者在干预前后都回答了调查问卷并参与了行为灵活性任务。在干预过程中,化解组采用了分层化解程序。他们将自己的抑郁私人事件分类为想法、感受或感觉,并将所有事件整合为 "抑郁"。随后,他们进行了 "抑郁 "的词语重复练习。对照组阅读一篇关于日本文化的情绪中性文章,时间为 5 分钟。结果显示,化解练习减少了对抑郁的认知融合。然而,它并没有减少抑郁反刍或促进行为灵活性。这些结果表明,要减少抑郁性反刍和促进行为灵活性,可能需要调整化解练习和其他干预措施的实施。
{"title":"The Effect of a Defusion Exercise Including a Hierarchical Procedure on Depressive Rumination","authors":"Yuki Shigemoto, Ian Stewart, Takashi Muto","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12533","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12533","url":null,"abstract":"This study examined the effect of a defusion exercise with a hierarchical procedure in the context of depression, depressive rumination, and behavioral inflexibility. A total of 38 undergraduates were randomly assigned to either a defusion or control group. All participants answered questionnaires and engaged in a behavioral flexibility task before and after intervention. For the intervention, the defusion group engaged in a hierarchical defusion procedure. They sorted their depressive private events into either thoughts, feelings, or sensations, and all of them were integrated to “depression.” Subsequently, they engaged in word‐repetition exercises for “depression.” The control group read an emotionally neutral article on Japanese culture for 5 min. Results showed that the defusion exercise decreased cognitive fusion regarding depression. However, it did not decrease depressive rumination or promote behavioral flexibility. These results suggest that adjustments in the implementation of defusion exercises and other interventions may be necessary to reduce depressive rumination and promote behavioral flexibility.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141359394","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perception of Facial Cues to Trustworthiness in Infancy: Insights from the Spatial Frequency Filtering Approach 婴儿期对可信度面部线索的感知:空间频率过滤法的启示
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-11 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12531
Valentina Silvestri, Viola Macchi Cassia
A crucial aspect of human social competence is the ability to spontaneously and rapidly infer from facial cues whether others are likely to approach us with friendliness or hostility—that is, trustworthiness. The rapid and automatic nature of these inferences has prompted the claim that they may originate from evolutionary pressures to detect potential threats, thus enhancing our chances of survival. However, the developmental origins of this social skill remain a topic of ongoing debate. Recent evidence shows that infants' brains and looking time behaviors differentiate between faces varying along the trustworthiness continuum, but the question about the nature of the facial cues they rely on remains open. In this review, we propose the spatial frequency (SF) filtering approach (i.e., the selective removal of spatial frequency bands from the image) as a useful tool for investigating this question, and specifically whether infants' discrimination of facial expressions of emotion share common visual and neural mechanisms with discrimination of facial cues associated with trustworthiness inferences. The SF filtering approach could shed light on the neural and perceptual mechanisms underlying trustworthiness perception in infancy, providing insights into whether and how these mechanisms change across development. The evidence gathered through this method would prove critical to the understanding of the developmental origins of trustworthiness perception.
人类社交能力的一个重要方面是能够自发地、迅速地从面部线索中推断出他人是否可能以友好或敌意的态度接近我们,也就是推断出他人是否值得信任。这些推断的快速和自动性质促使人们认为,它们可能源于发现潜在威胁的进化压力,从而提高我们的生存机会。然而,这种社交技能的发展起源仍然是一个争论不休的话题。最近的证据表明,婴儿的大脑和观察时间行为能够区分可信度连续统一体中不同的面孔,但他们所依赖的面部线索的性质仍是一个未决问题。在这篇综述中,我们提出了空间频率(SF)过滤法(即有选择性地去除图像中的空间频率带)作为研究这一问题的有用工具,特别是婴儿对面部情绪表达的辨别是否与对与可信度推断相关的面部线索的辨别有着共同的视觉和神经机制。SF 滤波法可以揭示婴儿期可信度感知的神经和知觉机制,为了解这些机制是否以及如何在发育过程中发生变化提供启示。通过这种方法收集到的证据将被证明对理解可信度感知的发展起源至关重要。
{"title":"Perception of Facial Cues to Trustworthiness in Infancy: Insights from the Spatial Frequency Filtering Approach","authors":"Valentina Silvestri, Viola Macchi Cassia","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12531","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12531","url":null,"abstract":"A crucial aspect of human social competence is the ability to spontaneously and rapidly infer from facial cues whether others are likely to approach us with friendliness or hostility—that is, trustworthiness. The rapid and automatic nature of these inferences has prompted the claim that they may originate from evolutionary pressures to detect potential threats, thus enhancing our chances of survival. However, the developmental origins of this social skill remain a topic of ongoing debate. Recent evidence shows that infants' brains and looking time behaviors differentiate between faces varying along the trustworthiness continuum, but the question about the nature of the facial cues they rely on remains open. In this review, we propose the spatial frequency (SF) filtering approach (i.e., the selective removal of spatial frequency bands from the image) as a useful tool for investigating this question, and specifically whether infants' discrimination of facial expressions of emotion share common visual and neural mechanisms with discrimination of facial cues associated with trustworthiness inferences. The SF filtering approach could shed light on the neural and perceptual mechanisms underlying trustworthiness perception in infancy, providing insights into whether and how these mechanisms change across development. The evidence gathered through this method would prove critical to the understanding of the developmental origins of trustworthiness perception.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141355347","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neuronal Mechanisms Underlying Face Recognition in Non‐human Primates 非人灵长类动物人脸识别的神经元机制
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12530
Hidetoshi Amita, K. W. Koyano, Jun Kunimatsu
Humans and primates rely on visual face recognition for social interactions. Damage to specific brain areas causes prosopagnosia, a condition characterized by the inability to recognize familiar faces, indicating the presence of specialized brain areas for facial‐recognition processing. A breakthrough finding came from a non‐human primate (NHP) study conducted in the early 2000s; it was the first to identify multiple face‐processing areas in the temporal lobe, termed “face patches.” Subsequent studies have demonstrated the unique role of each face patch in the structural analysis of faces. More recent studies have expanded these findings by exploring the role of face‐patch networks in social and memory functions and the importance of early face exposure in the development of the system. In this review, we discuss the neuronal mechanisms responsible for analyzing facial features, categorizing faces, and associating faces with memory and social contexts within both the cerebral cortex and subcortical areas. Use of NHPs in neuropsychological and neurophysiological studies can highlight the mechanistic aspects of the neuronal circuit underlying face recognition at both the single‐neuron and whole‐brain network levels.
人类和灵长类动物依靠视觉人脸识别进行社会交往。特定脑区受损会导致面容失认症,这种病症的特征是无法识别熟悉的面孔,这表明存在专门处理面容识别的脑区。本世纪初进行的一项非人灵长类动物(NHP)研究取得了突破性发现;该研究首次发现了颞叶中的多个面部处理区域,这些区域被称为 "面部斑块"。随后的研究证明了每个面孔斑块在面孔结构分析中的独特作用。最近的研究扩展了这些发现,探讨了面孔网络在社交和记忆功能中的作用,以及早期面孔接触对该系统发展的重要性。在这篇综述中,我们将讨论大脑皮层和皮层下区域中负责分析面部特征、对人脸进行分类以及将人脸与记忆和社会环境联系起来的神经元机制。在神经心理学和神经生理学研究中使用 NHPs 可以在单神经元和全脑网络水平上突出人脸识别神经元回路的机制方面。
{"title":"Neuronal Mechanisms Underlying Face Recognition in Non‐human Primates","authors":"Hidetoshi Amita, K. W. Koyano, Jun Kunimatsu","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12530","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12530","url":null,"abstract":"Humans and primates rely on visual face recognition for social interactions. Damage to specific brain areas causes prosopagnosia, a condition characterized by the inability to recognize familiar faces, indicating the presence of specialized brain areas for facial‐recognition processing. A breakthrough finding came from a non‐human primate (NHP) study conducted in the early 2000s; it was the first to identify multiple face‐processing areas in the temporal lobe, termed “face patches.” Subsequent studies have demonstrated the unique role of each face patch in the structural analysis of faces. More recent studies have expanded these findings by exploring the role of face‐patch networks in social and memory functions and the importance of early face exposure in the development of the system. In this review, we discuss the neuronal mechanisms responsible for analyzing facial features, categorizing faces, and associating faces with memory and social contexts within both the cerebral cortex and subcortical areas. Use of NHPs in neuropsychological and neurophysiological studies can highlight the mechanistic aspects of the neuronal circuit underlying face recognition at both the single‐neuron and whole‐brain network levels.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141363688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Haptic Sensations Modulate Judgment of Words that Elicit Self‐referent Memory 触觉会调节对引起自我参照记忆的词语的判断
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12532
Toshiki Ikeda, Yuji Takeda
Previous studies have reported that soft haptic sensations influence higher‐order cognition, but few studies have investigated effects of soft haptic sensations on the recall of episodic memories. The present study examined whether or not holding a soft object influences the recall of episodes. Participants held either a soft or hard cushion and performed tasks in which they recalled episodic memories. On each trial, a pleasant or unpleasant trait word was presented. In Experiment 1, the participants judged whether or not the word elicited an autobiographical memory. In Experiment 2, the participants judged whether or not the word elicited a memory of an episode involving their caregiver. In Experiment 1, the number of self‐referent judgments for unpleasant words was smaller when participants held a soft cushion than when they held a hard cushion. In Experiment 2, holding soft or hard cushions had no significant effect on the number of caregiver‐referent judgments. These results suggest that effects of soft haptic sensations on the recall of positive and negative autobiographical memories are asymmetrical.
以往的研究表明,柔软的触觉会影响高阶认知,但很少有研究调查柔软的触觉对回忆情节记忆的影响。本研究探讨了手持柔软物体是否会影响情节记忆的回忆。受试者手持软垫或硬垫,完成回忆片段记忆的任务。每次试验都会出现一个令人愉快或不愉快的特征词。在实验 1 中,受试者判断该词是否会引起自传体记忆。在实验 2 中,受试者判断该词是否引起了涉及其照顾者的情节记忆。在实验 1 中,当参与者手持软垫时,对不愉快词语的自我暗示判断次数少于手持硬垫时。在实验 2 中,手持软垫或硬垫对照顾者参照判断的数量没有显著影响。这些结果表明,柔软的触觉对回忆正面和负面自传体记忆的影响是不对称的。
{"title":"Haptic Sensations Modulate Judgment of Words that Elicit Self‐referent Memory","authors":"Toshiki Ikeda, Yuji Takeda","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12532","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12532","url":null,"abstract":"Previous studies have reported that soft haptic sensations influence higher‐order cognition, but few studies have investigated effects of soft haptic sensations on the recall of episodic memories. The present study examined whether or not holding a soft object influences the recall of episodes. Participants held either a soft or hard cushion and performed tasks in which they recalled episodic memories. On each trial, a pleasant or unpleasant trait word was presented. In Experiment 1, the participants judged whether or not the word elicited an autobiographical memory. In Experiment 2, the participants judged whether or not the word elicited a memory of an episode involving their caregiver. In Experiment 1, the number of self‐referent judgments for unpleasant words was smaller when participants held a soft cushion than when they held a hard cushion. In Experiment 2, holding soft or hard cushions had no significant effect on the number of caregiver‐referent judgments. These results suggest that effects of soft haptic sensations on the recall of positive and negative autobiographical memories are asymmetrical.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141363367","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Mobile App‐Based Positive Psychology Intervention for Well‐being 基于移动应用程序的积极心理学干预对幸福的影响
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12535
Hiroaki Uechi, T. Shimazaki, Koji Takenaka
This study examined the feasibility of a mobile app‐based positive psychology intervention (PPI) for enhancing well‐being. A total of 24 Japanese students (11 men and 13 women) were assigned to the intervention group and 24 students with the same attributes (sex and grade) as the intervention group were assigned to the control group. The mobile app‐based PPI consisted of three PPI activities for 2 weeks. Participants recorded how many character strengths they used (character strengths), how many letters they sent (gratitude letters), and the events that went well on that day (three good things). Statistical analysis was performed using analysis of variance, and the results revealed significant intervention effects on engagement and achievement among well‐being. Moreover, favorable results were observed regarding the feasibility of the intervention, where women had a more favorable attitude toward acceptability, implementation, adaptation, integration, and expansion of the app than men.
本研究探讨了基于手机应用的积极心理学干预(PPI)在提高幸福感方面的可行性。共有 24 名日本学生(11 男 13 女)被分配到干预组,24 名与干预组属性(性别和年级)相同的学生被分配到对照组。基于手机应用的品格优势活动包括三项品格优势活动,为期两周。参与者记录了他们使用了多少性格优势(性格优势)、寄出了多少封信(感谢信)以及当天顺利进行的事件(三件好事)。统计分析采用方差分析法进行,结果显示,干预措施对参与度和幸福感方面的成就有显著效果。此外,在干预的可行性方面也观察到了有利的结果,女性比男性对应用程序的可接受性、实施、适应、整合和扩展持更有利的态度。
{"title":"Effects of Mobile App‐Based Positive Psychology Intervention for Well‐being","authors":"Hiroaki Uechi, T. Shimazaki, Koji Takenaka","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12535","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12535","url":null,"abstract":"This study examined the feasibility of a mobile app‐based positive psychology intervention (PPI) for enhancing well‐being. A total of 24 Japanese students (11 men and 13 women) were assigned to the intervention group and 24 students with the same attributes (sex and grade) as the intervention group were assigned to the control group. The mobile app‐based PPI consisted of three PPI activities for 2 weeks. Participants recorded how many character strengths they used (character strengths), how many letters they sent (gratitude letters), and the events that went well on that day (three good things). Statistical analysis was performed using analysis of variance, and the results revealed significant intervention effects on engagement and achievement among well‐being. Moreover, favorable results were observed regarding the feasibility of the intervention, where women had a more favorable attitude toward acceptability, implementation, adaptation, integration, and expansion of the app than men.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141362584","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Japanese Psychological Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1