Guest editorial: new horizons in organizational cognition

Claudia Toma, I. Menezes, D. Secchi
{"title":"Guest editorial: new horizons in organizational cognition","authors":"Claudia Toma, I. Menezes, D. Secchi","doi":"10.1108/ijotb-09-2021-169","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The expression “organizational cognition” (OC) has been around for quite some time now (Ilgen et al., 1994; Hodgkinson and Healey, 2008; Walsh, 1995). Applications, more than theoretical developments, gave OC a place within the management literature so that a cognitive component is found more and more often in published research. Yet, what the word “cognition”means within the context of organizations remains vague and highly dependent on theoretical assumptions (Secchi and Adamsen, 2017). On the one hand, cognition is addressed starting from more traditional organizational characteristics, with attention to, among the many, leadership (Lord and Emrich, 2001), teams and groups (Ilgen et al., 2005; Toma and Butera, 2015) and individual social responsibility (Crilly et al., 2008; Secchi, 2009). On the other hand, organization theory and behavior are addressed from the perspective of cognition, with particular emphasis to, for example, cultural ecologies (Hutchins, 2014), human interactivity (Steffensen and Pedersen, 2014) and decision-making (Bardone, 2011; Toma and Butera, 2009). The former, and more traditional, assumptions can be called representationalistwhile the latter belong to the embodied, distributed and extended cognition (EDEC) perspective. When taking a representationalist stance, researchers operate on the assumption that cognitive processes involve the mirroring of the observed in a cognitive “device.” Traditionally, this has been considered to be the human brain, where observed phenomena are represented. The EDEC perspectives take a different set of assumptions and make the issue of representation secondary, when not irrelevant. This is not the place to get involved in the debate between these two streams of research because (1) it is a relatively old one (e.g. Varela et al., 1991), and (2) it is not the central message of this editorial. All thatmatters here is that an EDEC stance consists in defining cognition as a systemwhere a number of internal resources (e.g. brain, body, feelings) aremixed and interplaywith external resources (e.g. artifacts, other people). Even though there has been a long tradition of EDEC perspectives in the cognitive sciences (Varela et al., 1991; Hutchins, 1995), they have not been recognized in OC until very recently. For example, Healey et al. (2018) discuss distributed cognition within a representationalist framework, and Hodgkinson (2015) has acknowledged their existence in his influential review of managerial and organizational cognition (MOC). The two lines of reasoning about OC are still separate but, perhaps, less irreconcilable. Of course, some would keep a radical take while others would want to establish a dialogue and explore the territory around their field. This Special Issue New Horizons in Organizational Cognition has been created with the intention of drawing attention to the different aspects of cognition research applied to and concerning organizations in an attempt to understand the extent to which the two stances above communicate, diverge, merge or dialogue. The idea was that of encouraging a reflection starting from diverse theoretical perspectives, e.g. computationalism and its opposite, social identity theory, mental representationalism and its critics, sensemaking, as well as from different disciplinary traditions, even if outside organizational theory and behavior, e.g. cognitive psychology, social cognition, cognitive science, artificial intelligence. This would have encouraged a wide approach to OC and, at the same time, helped frame where next can the field move toward. The Special Issue was specifically designed to attract those perspectives that serve as a bridge between existing and forthcoming OC studies. This offers a wider opening to more Guest editorial","PeriodicalId":35239,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Organization Theory and Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2021-11-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"International Journal of Organization Theory and Behavior","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1108/ijotb-09-2021-169","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"Social Sciences","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

The expression “organizational cognition” (OC) has been around for quite some time now (Ilgen et al., 1994; Hodgkinson and Healey, 2008; Walsh, 1995). Applications, more than theoretical developments, gave OC a place within the management literature so that a cognitive component is found more and more often in published research. Yet, what the word “cognition”means within the context of organizations remains vague and highly dependent on theoretical assumptions (Secchi and Adamsen, 2017). On the one hand, cognition is addressed starting from more traditional organizational characteristics, with attention to, among the many, leadership (Lord and Emrich, 2001), teams and groups (Ilgen et al., 2005; Toma and Butera, 2015) and individual social responsibility (Crilly et al., 2008; Secchi, 2009). On the other hand, organization theory and behavior are addressed from the perspective of cognition, with particular emphasis to, for example, cultural ecologies (Hutchins, 2014), human interactivity (Steffensen and Pedersen, 2014) and decision-making (Bardone, 2011; Toma and Butera, 2009). The former, and more traditional, assumptions can be called representationalistwhile the latter belong to the embodied, distributed and extended cognition (EDEC) perspective. When taking a representationalist stance, researchers operate on the assumption that cognitive processes involve the mirroring of the observed in a cognitive “device.” Traditionally, this has been considered to be the human brain, where observed phenomena are represented. The EDEC perspectives take a different set of assumptions and make the issue of representation secondary, when not irrelevant. This is not the place to get involved in the debate between these two streams of research because (1) it is a relatively old one (e.g. Varela et al., 1991), and (2) it is not the central message of this editorial. All thatmatters here is that an EDEC stance consists in defining cognition as a systemwhere a number of internal resources (e.g. brain, body, feelings) aremixed and interplaywith external resources (e.g. artifacts, other people). Even though there has been a long tradition of EDEC perspectives in the cognitive sciences (Varela et al., 1991; Hutchins, 1995), they have not been recognized in OC until very recently. For example, Healey et al. (2018) discuss distributed cognition within a representationalist framework, and Hodgkinson (2015) has acknowledged their existence in his influential review of managerial and organizational cognition (MOC). The two lines of reasoning about OC are still separate but, perhaps, less irreconcilable. Of course, some would keep a radical take while others would want to establish a dialogue and explore the territory around their field. This Special Issue New Horizons in Organizational Cognition has been created with the intention of drawing attention to the different aspects of cognition research applied to and concerning organizations in an attempt to understand the extent to which the two stances above communicate, diverge, merge or dialogue. The idea was that of encouraging a reflection starting from diverse theoretical perspectives, e.g. computationalism and its opposite, social identity theory, mental representationalism and its critics, sensemaking, as well as from different disciplinary traditions, even if outside organizational theory and behavior, e.g. cognitive psychology, social cognition, cognitive science, artificial intelligence. This would have encouraged a wide approach to OC and, at the same time, helped frame where next can the field move toward. The Special Issue was specifically designed to attract those perspectives that serve as a bridge between existing and forthcoming OC studies. This offers a wider opening to more Guest editorial
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
嘉宾评论:组织认知的新视野
“组织认知”(OC)一词的出现已经有一段时间了(Ilgen et al., 1994;Hodgkinson and Healey, 2008;沃尔什,1995)。应用,而不是理论的发展,在管理文献中给了OC一个位置,因此在发表的研究中发现越来越多的认知成分。然而,在组织的背景下,“认知”这个词的含义仍然模糊,并且高度依赖于理论假设(Secchi和Adamsen, 2017)。一方面,认知是从更传统的组织特征开始的,其中关注领导力(Lord and Emrich, 2001)、团队和群体(Ilgen et al., 2005;Toma and Butera, 2015)和个人社会责任(Crilly et al., 2008;西奇,2009)。另一方面,组织理论和行为是从认知的角度来解决的,特别强调文化生态(Hutchins, 2014),人类互动(Steffensen和Pedersen, 2014)和决策(Bardone, 2011;Toma和Butera, 2009)。前者和更传统的假设可以被称为表征主义,而后者属于具身、分布和扩展认知(EDEC)的观点。当采取表征主义立场时,研究人员假设认知过程涉及在认知“装置”中观察到的镜像。传统上,这被认为是人类的大脑,在那里观察到的现象被表示。EDEC的观点采用了一组不同的假设,并将表示问题放在次要位置,即使不是无关紧要的。这不是参与这两种研究流之间争论的地方,因为(1)这是一个相对较旧的研究(例如Varela et al., 1991),(2)它不是这篇社论的中心信息。这里重要的是,EDEC的立场包括将认知定义为一个系统,在这个系统中,许多内部资源(如大脑、身体、感觉)混合在一起,并与外部资源(如人工制品、其他人)相互作用。尽管认知科学中有很长的EDEC观点传统(Varela et al., 1991;Hutchins, 1995),它们直到最近才在OC中得到认可。例如,Healey等人(2018)在表征主义框架内讨论了分布式认知,Hodgkinson(2015)在他对管理和组织认知(MOC)的有影响力的评论中承认了它们的存在。关于OC的两种推理方式仍然是分开的,但也许不那么不可调和了。当然,有些人会保持激进的态度,而另一些人则希望建立对话并探索他们所在领域的领域。本期《组织认知新视野》特刊的目的是引起人们对应用于组织和与组织有关的认知研究的不同方面的关注,试图理解上述两种立场在多大程度上沟通、分歧、融合或对话。这个想法是鼓励从不同的理论角度出发的反思,例如计算主义及其对立面,社会认同理论,心理表征主义及其批评者,语义,以及来自不同学科传统的反思,即使是在组织理论和行为之外,例如认知心理学,社会认知,认知科学,人工智能。这将鼓励更广泛的OC方法,同时有助于确定该领域的下一步发展方向。这期特刊是专门设计来吸引那些作为现有和即将进行的OC研究之间桥梁的观点。这为更多的客座编辑提供了更广阔的空间
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
International Journal of Organization Theory and Behavior
International Journal of Organization Theory and Behavior Social Sciences-Public Administration
CiteScore
3.20
自引率
0.00%
发文量
11
期刊介绍: The International Journal of Organization Theory and Behavior brings together researchers and practitioners, both within and outside the United States, who are in the areas of organization theory, management, development, and behavior. This journal covers all private, public and not-for-profit organizations’ theories and behavior.
期刊最新文献
Workplace mitigators of the negative relationship between stress and health How do workplace stressors during COVID-19 affect health frontline employees in Iran: Investigating the role of employee resilience and constituent attachment Exploring context-related challenges and adaptive responses while working from home during COVID-19 Helicopter helping in the organization: its conceptualization, key characteristics and possible antecedents and consequences “Can politics really be beneficial?” Toward a model for positive politics through consensus
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1