Hearsay Exceptions: Adjusting the Ratio of Intuition to Psychological Science

Q2 Social Sciences Law and Contemporary Problems Pub Date : 2002-01-01 DOI:10.2307/1192365
J. Myers, I. Cordón, S. Ghetti, G. Goodman
{"title":"Hearsay Exceptions: Adjusting the Ratio of Intuition to Psychological Science","authors":"J. Myers, I. Cordón, S. Ghetti, G. Goodman","doi":"10.2307/1192365","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"John E.B. Myers (*) Ingrid Cordon (**) Simona Ghetti (***) Gail S. Goodman (****) I INTRODUCTION Exceptions to the hearsay rule grew out of intuitive beliefs about human nature. (1) Thus, dying declarations were thought to be reliable because dying declarants hesitate to meet their Maker with a lie upon their lips. (2) Excited utterances are reliable because traumatic stress is believed to still the capacity to lie. Statements to doctors are trustworthy because patients are believed to have an incentive to tell the truth to the doctor. Psychological science based on empirical study of human behavior played no role in the origin of most hearsay exceptions. The exceptions, after all, came into being long before William James and Wilhem Wundt established their laboratories in 1875 and 1879. (3) Today, psychological science has made tremendous progress, and it is appropriate to ask whether modern psychology supports or undermines the intuitive foundations of hearsay exceptions. This paper explores this issue by examining three exceptions: excited utterances, statements for purposes of medical diagnosis or treatment, and the residual hearsay exception. These selections are appropriate because the focus is child declarants, and these exceptions play key roles in child abuse litigation. II THE EXCITED UTTERANCE EXCEPTION The origins of the excited utterance exception can be traced to the eighteenth century. (4) The exception is invoked frequently in child abuse litigation. (5) The exception is codified in Rule 803(2) of the Federal Rules of Evidence, which provides that the hearsay rule does not exclude statements \"relating to a startling event or condition made while the declarant was under the stress of excitement caused by the event or condition.\" (6) The primary rationale for the exception is that statements are trustworthy when made shortly following a startling event and while the declarant remains affected by the stress caused by the event. Traumatic stress is thought to lower the probability that statements are a product of conscious fabrication. (7) The excited utterance exception has three requirements. First, there must be an exciting event. Second, the out-of-court statement must relate to the event. Third, the statement must be made while the declarant is under the stress of excitement induced by the event. Courts consider several factors to determine whether hearsay statements were made under the stress of excitement caused by a startling event. We discuss these factors below, employing a two-step process. We begin by outlining legal principles. The legal analysis will then be followed by relevant psychological principles. We conclude the discussion of excited utterances by grappling with the fundamental question: Does traumatic stress still the capacity to lie? Some of the legal factors involved in this determination include: Spontaneity. For many courts, spontaneity is the most important factor in determining whether a statement was a product of reflective thought or a spontaneous reaction to a startling event. The more spontaneous the statement, the more likely the court is to apply the excited utterance exception. If a child had an opportunity to reflect, the balance tips away from admissibility. Questioning. A child may make a statement in response to questions about a startling event. Courts agree that questioning does not necessarily destroy the excitement required for this exception. Thus, simple questions like \"What happened?\" do not destroy excitement or spontaneity. As questioning increases, however, and especially as questions become suggestive, the odds decrease that the child's statement is a spontaneous reaction to a startling event. Lapse of time. The longer the delay between a startling event and an out-of-court statement, the less likely the declarant was excited when the statement was made. …","PeriodicalId":39484,"journal":{"name":"Law and Contemporary Problems","volume":"34 1","pages":"3-46"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2002-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"7","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Law and Contemporary Problems","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.2307/1192365","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"Social Sciences","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7

Abstract

John E.B. Myers (*) Ingrid Cordon (**) Simona Ghetti (***) Gail S. Goodman (****) I INTRODUCTION Exceptions to the hearsay rule grew out of intuitive beliefs about human nature. (1) Thus, dying declarations were thought to be reliable because dying declarants hesitate to meet their Maker with a lie upon their lips. (2) Excited utterances are reliable because traumatic stress is believed to still the capacity to lie. Statements to doctors are trustworthy because patients are believed to have an incentive to tell the truth to the doctor. Psychological science based on empirical study of human behavior played no role in the origin of most hearsay exceptions. The exceptions, after all, came into being long before William James and Wilhem Wundt established their laboratories in 1875 and 1879. (3) Today, psychological science has made tremendous progress, and it is appropriate to ask whether modern psychology supports or undermines the intuitive foundations of hearsay exceptions. This paper explores this issue by examining three exceptions: excited utterances, statements for purposes of medical diagnosis or treatment, and the residual hearsay exception. These selections are appropriate because the focus is child declarants, and these exceptions play key roles in child abuse litigation. II THE EXCITED UTTERANCE EXCEPTION The origins of the excited utterance exception can be traced to the eighteenth century. (4) The exception is invoked frequently in child abuse litigation. (5) The exception is codified in Rule 803(2) of the Federal Rules of Evidence, which provides that the hearsay rule does not exclude statements "relating to a startling event or condition made while the declarant was under the stress of excitement caused by the event or condition." (6) The primary rationale for the exception is that statements are trustworthy when made shortly following a startling event and while the declarant remains affected by the stress caused by the event. Traumatic stress is thought to lower the probability that statements are a product of conscious fabrication. (7) The excited utterance exception has three requirements. First, there must be an exciting event. Second, the out-of-court statement must relate to the event. Third, the statement must be made while the declarant is under the stress of excitement induced by the event. Courts consider several factors to determine whether hearsay statements were made under the stress of excitement caused by a startling event. We discuss these factors below, employing a two-step process. We begin by outlining legal principles. The legal analysis will then be followed by relevant psychological principles. We conclude the discussion of excited utterances by grappling with the fundamental question: Does traumatic stress still the capacity to lie? Some of the legal factors involved in this determination include: Spontaneity. For many courts, spontaneity is the most important factor in determining whether a statement was a product of reflective thought or a spontaneous reaction to a startling event. The more spontaneous the statement, the more likely the court is to apply the excited utterance exception. If a child had an opportunity to reflect, the balance tips away from admissibility. Questioning. A child may make a statement in response to questions about a startling event. Courts agree that questioning does not necessarily destroy the excitement required for this exception. Thus, simple questions like "What happened?" do not destroy excitement or spontaneity. As questioning increases, however, and especially as questions become suggestive, the odds decrease that the child's statement is a spontaneous reaction to a startling event. Lapse of time. The longer the delay between a startling event and an out-of-court statement, the less likely the declarant was excited when the statement was made. …
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
道听途说例外:调整直觉与心理科学的比例
约翰·e·b·迈尔斯(*)英格丽·戈登(**)西蒙娜·盖蒂(***)盖尔·s·古德曼(****)道听途说规则的例外情况源于对人性的直觉信念。因此,临终的宣言被认为是可靠的,因为临终的宣言者在面对他们的造物主时犹豫不决,嘴上说着谎言。(2)兴奋的话语是可靠的,因为创伤性应激被认为仍然具有说谎的能力。对医生的陈述是值得信赖的,因为人们相信病人有向医生说实话的动机。基于对人类行为的实证研究的心理科学在大多数传闻例外的起源中没有发挥作用。毕竟,早在1875年和1879年威廉·詹姆斯和威廉姆·冯特建立实验室之前,这些例外就已经存在了。(3)今天,心理科学已经取得了巨大的进步,我们有必要提出这样的问题:现代心理学是支持还是破坏道听途说例外的直觉基础?本文通过检查三种例外情况来探讨这一问题:激动的话语、医学诊断或治疗目的的陈述和残余传闻例外。这些选择是适当的,因为重点是儿童声明人,这些例外在虐待儿童诉讼中起着关键作用。兴奋话语例外的起源可以追溯到18世纪。(4)在虐待儿童诉讼中经常援引例外情况。(5)例外情况在《联邦证据规则》第803(2)条中被编纂,该规则规定道听途说规则不排除“在声明人因该事件或情况而处于紧张状态时所作的与令人吃惊的事件或情况有关的陈述”。(6)例外情况的主要理由是,在令人吃惊的事件发生后不久,当声明人仍然受到事件造成的压力的影响时,陈述是可信的。创伤压力被认为降低了陈述是有意识捏造的产物的可能性。(7)兴奋性话语例外有三个条件。首先,必须有一个激动人心的事件。其次,庭外陈述必须与事件相关。第三,声明必须在声明人因事件而处于紧张兴奋状态时作出。法院考虑几个因素来确定传闻陈述是否是在令人吃惊的事件引起的兴奋的压力下作出的。我们将采用两步流程在下面讨论这些因素。我们首先概述法律原则。在进行法律分析之后,将遵循有关的心理学原则。我们通过解决一个基本问题来结束对激动话语的讨论:创伤性压力是否仍然具有说谎的能力?在这一决定中涉及的一些法律因素包括:对许多法院来说,在决定陈述是反思思考的产物还是对惊人事件的自发反应时,自发性是最重要的因素。陈述越自发,法院就越有可能适用激动性陈述例外。如果一个孩子有机会反思,那么这种平衡就会偏离可接受性。讯问。孩子可能会在回答关于一个令人吃惊的事件的问题时做出陈述。法院同意质疑并不一定会破坏这种例外情况所需要的兴奋感。因此,像“发生了什么?”这样简单的问题不会破坏兴奋或自发性。然而,随着问题的增加,特别是当问题变得具有暗示性时,孩子的陈述是对惊人事件的自发反应的可能性就会降低。时间的流逝。令人震惊的事件和庭外陈述之间的间隔越长,陈述人在陈述时兴奋的可能性就越小。…
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
Law and Contemporary Problems
Law and Contemporary Problems Social Sciences-Law
CiteScore
2.00
自引率
0.00%
发文量
1
期刊介绍: Law and Contemporary Problems was founded in 1933 and is the oldest journal published at Duke Law School. It is a quarterly, interdisciplinary, faculty-edited publication of Duke Law School. L&CP recognizes that many fields in the sciences, social sciences, and humanities can enhance the development and understanding of law. It is our purpose to seek out these areas of overlap and to publish balanced symposia that enlighten not just legal readers, but readers from these other disciplines as well. L&CP uses a symposium format, generally publishing one symposium per issue on a topic of contemporary concern. Authors and articles are selected to ensure that each issue collectively creates a unified presentation of the contemporary problem under consideration. L&CP hosts an annual conference at Duke Law School featuring the authors of one of the year’s four symposia.
期刊最新文献
The Influence of Re-Selection on Independent Decision Making in State Supreme Courts Voting Rights and the “Statutory Constitution” Challenging Gender in Single-Sex Spaces: Lessons from a Feminist Softball League Treaties and Human Rights: The Role of Long-Term Trends Correcting Federalism Mistakes in Statutory Interpretation: The Supreme Court and the Federal Arbitration Act
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1