Understanding Other Minds

IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Language Learning and Development Pub Date : 2020-12-10 DOI:10.1080/15475441.2020.1831502
A. Papafragou
{"title":"Understanding Other Minds","authors":"A. Papafragou","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2020.1831502","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Human beings spend vast amounts of time thinking about what other people believe, want, hope, and fear. Understanding other minds is part of our social nature and forms the basis of the social fabric of our communities and our world. For the past 35 years, a large research field has explored the inner workings of mental state reasoning, broadly known as Theory of Mind (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). At the core of this field lies the question of how Theory of Mind develops – namely, how children come to understand that other people have beliefs, desires, intentions, and other mental states that can be used to explain or predict behavior. Many studies have focused specifically on how children develop the understanding that access to information and the resulting beliefs in other people’s minds may be different from their own, and that sometimes other people can hold beliefs that are false (e.g., Scott & Baillargeon, 2017; Wellman, 2014). From the perspective of language acquisition, the ability to entertain thoughts about other minds can help explain how children acquire language so rapidly and without overt instruction. Most obviously, perhaps, Theory of Mind mechanisms can contribute to the discovery of semantic meaning; for instance, children can infer the meaning of a novel word by consulting the speaker’s eye gaze or mental state (Baldwin, 1991; Bloom, 2000). Furthermore, understanding other minds allows learners to bridge the gap between what words and sentences mean and what a speaker intends to communicate by uttering them in a specific context – for instance, whether the speaker is being literal or ironic (Grice, 1975; Sperber & Wilson, 1986). Thus, from the perspective of the young learner, the ability to think about other minds can be used both to constrain hypotheses about what a novel word means and to contextually enrich the meaning of known words and structures. However, the nature of mindreading abilities, their availability and growth in both humans and other animals, and their specific contribution to language learning are all still a matter of debate. With these issues in mind, the present author and the leadership of the Society for Language Development organized a symposium on the topic of “Understanding other minds” on November 12, 2018, at Boston University. The invited speakers were Gyorgi Gergely, Alexandra Rosati, and Jill deVilliers. The goal of the symposium was to highlight classic and more recent findings and theorizing in this rapidly changing field, and to promote discussion of where the field should go next. The three speakers were later invited to prepare articles for a special section of Language Learning and Development dedicated to the same topic. One of them, Gyorgi Gergely, was not able to contribute a paper within the timeframe of the volume. The present special section represents two important perspectives on how we understand others, how this understanding relates to language, and how thoughts about others might originate in our primate relatives. In her article, Jill deVilliers addresses the question of how mental state language and the understanding of propositional attitudes relate to each other. The paper raises the possibility that properties of natural language might promote – and not simply reflect – mindreading abilities. Specifically, deVilliers speculates that some propositional attitude meanings come about through the syntax of natural language and considers several possible mechanisms that might produce this result. One possibility is that language may provide representational and/or computational resources that facilitate","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"55 1","pages":"69 - 70"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5000,"publicationDate":"2020-12-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Language Learning and Development","FirstCategoryId":"98","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2020.1831502","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"0","JCRName":"LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Human beings spend vast amounts of time thinking about what other people believe, want, hope, and fear. Understanding other minds is part of our social nature and forms the basis of the social fabric of our communities and our world. For the past 35 years, a large research field has explored the inner workings of mental state reasoning, broadly known as Theory of Mind (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). At the core of this field lies the question of how Theory of Mind develops – namely, how children come to understand that other people have beliefs, desires, intentions, and other mental states that can be used to explain or predict behavior. Many studies have focused specifically on how children develop the understanding that access to information and the resulting beliefs in other people’s minds may be different from their own, and that sometimes other people can hold beliefs that are false (e.g., Scott & Baillargeon, 2017; Wellman, 2014). From the perspective of language acquisition, the ability to entertain thoughts about other minds can help explain how children acquire language so rapidly and without overt instruction. Most obviously, perhaps, Theory of Mind mechanisms can contribute to the discovery of semantic meaning; for instance, children can infer the meaning of a novel word by consulting the speaker’s eye gaze or mental state (Baldwin, 1991; Bloom, 2000). Furthermore, understanding other minds allows learners to bridge the gap between what words and sentences mean and what a speaker intends to communicate by uttering them in a specific context – for instance, whether the speaker is being literal or ironic (Grice, 1975; Sperber & Wilson, 1986). Thus, from the perspective of the young learner, the ability to think about other minds can be used both to constrain hypotheses about what a novel word means and to contextually enrich the meaning of known words and structures. However, the nature of mindreading abilities, their availability and growth in both humans and other animals, and their specific contribution to language learning are all still a matter of debate. With these issues in mind, the present author and the leadership of the Society for Language Development organized a symposium on the topic of “Understanding other minds” on November 12, 2018, at Boston University. The invited speakers were Gyorgi Gergely, Alexandra Rosati, and Jill deVilliers. The goal of the symposium was to highlight classic and more recent findings and theorizing in this rapidly changing field, and to promote discussion of where the field should go next. The three speakers were later invited to prepare articles for a special section of Language Learning and Development dedicated to the same topic. One of them, Gyorgi Gergely, was not able to contribute a paper within the timeframe of the volume. The present special section represents two important perspectives on how we understand others, how this understanding relates to language, and how thoughts about others might originate in our primate relatives. In her article, Jill deVilliers addresses the question of how mental state language and the understanding of propositional attitudes relate to each other. The paper raises the possibility that properties of natural language might promote – and not simply reflect – mindreading abilities. Specifically, deVilliers speculates that some propositional attitude meanings come about through the syntax of natural language and considers several possible mechanisms that might produce this result. One possibility is that language may provide representational and/or computational resources that facilitate
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
理解他人的思想
人类花了大量的时间去思考别人相信什么、想要什么、希望什么和害怕什么。理解他人的思想是我们社会天性的一部分,也是我们社区和世界社会结构的基础。在过去的35年里,一个大型的研究领域探索了精神状态推理的内部运作,这被广泛地称为心理理论(Baron-Cohen et al., 1985)。这个领域的核心问题是心理理论是如何发展的——也就是说,孩子们是如何理解别人有信仰、欲望、意图和其他可以用来解释或预测行为的心理状态的。许多研究专门关注儿童如何发展理解,即获取信息的途径和他人心中由此产生的信念可能与自己的不同,有时其他人可能持有错误的信念(例如,Scott & Baillargeon, 2017;Wellman, 2014)。从语言习得的角度来看,思考他人思想的能力可以帮助解释为什么孩子在没有明显指导的情况下如此迅速地习得语言。最明显的可能是,心智理论机制有助于发现语义;例如,儿童可以通过参考说话人的目光或心理状态来推断一个新单词的意思(Baldwin, 1991;布鲁姆,2000)。此外,理解他人的想法可以让学习者在单词和句子的意思和说话者在特定的语境中想要表达的意思之间架起桥梁——例如,说话者是在字面上还是在讽刺(Grice, 1975;Sperber & Wilson, 1986)。因此,从年轻学习者的角度来看,思考他人想法的能力既可以用来限制对新单词含义的假设,也可以在上下文中丰富已知单词和结构的含义。然而,读心术的本质,它在人类和其他动物中的可用性和发展,以及它对语言学习的具体贡献,都仍然是一个有争议的问题。考虑到这些问题,本文作者和语言发展学会(Society for Language Development)的领导于2018年11月12日在波士顿大学组织了一场题为“理解其他思想”的研讨会。受邀的演讲者是Gyorgi Gergely, Alexandra Rosati和Jill deVilliers。研讨会的目的是强调在这个快速变化的领域中经典的和最新的发现和理论,并促进对该领域下一步发展方向的讨论。这三位发言者后来应邀为《语言学习与发展》专门讨论同一主题的一个栏目编写文章。其中一位,Gyorgi Gergely,未能在该卷的时间框架内提交一篇论文。本节特别介绍了两个重要的观点,即我们如何理解他人,这种理解如何与语言联系起来,以及关于他人的想法如何起源于我们的灵长类亲戚。Jill deVilliers在她的文章中阐述了心理状态语言和对命题态度的理解是如何相互关联的问题。这篇论文提出了一种可能性,即自然语言的特性可能会促进——而不仅仅是反映——读心术的能力。具体来说,deVilliers推测一些命题态度意义是通过自然语言的语法产生的,并考虑了几种可能产生这一结果的机制。一种可能性是,语言可以提供表征和/或计算资源,以方便
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
3.10
自引率
0.00%
发文量
26
期刊最新文献
Acquisition of a rare variant: ne-realization in the negative utterances of French children and their caregivers Thematic Content, Not Number Matching, Drives Syntactic Bootstrapping An Online Survey of Picture Book Reading Practices with Children Between the Ages of 0 and 30 Months Semantic Cues Facilitate Structural Generalizations in Artificial Language Learning Associations Between Joint Attention, Supported Joint Engagement and Language in TD Children and Children with ASD: Potential Sources of Individual and Group Differences in Language Outcomes
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1