对心脏有害的东西对大脑也有害?

IF 1 Q4 CARDIAC & CARDIOVASCULAR SYSTEMS Heart and Mind Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI:10.4103/hm.hm-d-23-00014
Eduard Guasch, Gemma Sangüesa, Montserrat Batlle
{"title":"对心脏有害的东西对大脑也有害?","authors":"Eduard Guasch, Gemma Sangüesa, Montserrat Batlle","doi":"10.4103/hm.hm-d-23-00014","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Dear Editor, We have read with great interest the most recent issue of Heart and Mind addressing the relationship between exercise, heart, and cognition.[1,2] Dr. Jenna Taylor excellently summarized the intertwine between cardiovascular and brain health and how regular moderate and vigorous exercises positively impinge the cardiovascular-cognition connection.[1] The growing core of evidence supporting exercise as a promising tool to blunt cardiovascular risk factors, but also cognitive impairment, is exhaustively reviewed. However, while the benefits of regular moderate exercise are undoubted and convincingly supported by preclinical and clinical data, the consequences of long-term strenuous exercise are still uncertain. In fact, emerging evidence shows that the relationship between training load and cardiovascular and brain health is not linear (the more exercise, the more benefit), but rather U-shaped. In recent years, long-term strenuous exercise has shown to increase the risk of cardiovascular disease, particularly cardiac arrhythmias , in certain populations. Atrial fibrillation risk is heightened in male endurance athletes, exercise is central to the pathophysiology of the arrhythmogenic cardiomyopathy in some patients, and the (formerly considered) physiological athlete’s bradycardia could evolve into clinically relevant sinus node dysfunction.[3] Moreover, recent data also suggest aortic tunica media damage in heavily trained animals[4] and more intense coronary atherosclerosis in male master marathon runners.[5] Could these negative effects extend to the brain? The robustness of clinical trials assessing long-term exposures is jeopardized by confounding factors and the lack of control groups. Although caution is needed when translating the conclusions of animal work to humans, preclinical studies restrict confounding factors and enable a more precise comparison between exercise loads. In an animal model, we have recently shown that high-intensity exercise could not replicate many of the moderate exercise-induced benefits on cerebrovascular and connectivity efficiency enhancement, both underlying improved learning capacity.[6] These results suggest that the maximal benefit of exercise in brain health is obtained at moderate doses while very high loads yield limited effects, thereby supporting the existence of a “sweet spot” for exercise intensity and duration on cognition.[7] Clinical studies confirm this notion, including a large epidemiological trial in which former professional athletes (i.e., soccer players) had a higher risk of neurodegenerative disease than the general population.[8] The pathophysiology behind many of the deleterious effects of strenuous exercise is only partially understood. The reduced cerebral blood flow occurring in an animal model after long-term strenuous training[6] would link the unexpected cognitive effects to recently demonstrated exercise-induced vascular damage.[4,5] Each strenuous bout of exercise superimposes a hemodynamic and biochemical stress on the cardiovascular system, which may result in transient right ventricular dysfunction.[9] Similarly, transient oxidative stress and altered mitochondrial energetics in the brain could mediate the observed reversal of benefits at high-intensity exercise.[6] Experiments conducted in human just after strenuous exercise support transient cognitive decline and impaired brain flow regulation. Finally, factors other than exercise itself may explain these deleterious effects. Repetitive concussion could not only contribute to deteriorate brain health in heavy contact sports, but also in sports such as football, as recently suggested.[10] Altogether, animal models and human trials suggest that the cognitive benefits promoted by regular exercise may be highly dependent on exercise intensity and duration; strenuous and long-term forms of exercise may paradoxically show deleterious. These data need to be confirmed, though, in large and well-designed studies. O’Keefe et al. appropriately state in their editorial[2] that “what is good for the heart is good for the brain.” Should we also claim that “what is bad for the heart is bad for the brain?” Financial support and sponsorship This work was partially supported by grants from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (PI19/00443 and PI22/00953), CERCA program/Generalitat de Catalunya, CIBERCV (16/11/00354), Consejo Superior de Deportes (EXP_75119), and the European Regional Development Fund (FEDER). Conflicts of interest Dr. Eduard Guasch is an Editorial Board member of Heart and Mind. The article was subject to the journal’s standard procedures, with peer review handled independently of Dr. Eduard Guasch and the research group. There are no conflicts of interest.","PeriodicalId":34653,"journal":{"name":"Heart and Mind","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0000,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"What is bad for the heart is bad for the brain?\",\"authors\":\"Eduard Guasch, Gemma Sangüesa, Montserrat Batlle\",\"doi\":\"10.4103/hm.hm-d-23-00014\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Dear Editor, We have read with great interest the most recent issue of Heart and Mind addressing the relationship between exercise, heart, and cognition.[1,2] Dr. Jenna Taylor excellently summarized the intertwine between cardiovascular and brain health and how regular moderate and vigorous exercises positively impinge the cardiovascular-cognition connection.[1] The growing core of evidence supporting exercise as a promising tool to blunt cardiovascular risk factors, but also cognitive impairment, is exhaustively reviewed. However, while the benefits of regular moderate exercise are undoubted and convincingly supported by preclinical and clinical data, the consequences of long-term strenuous exercise are still uncertain. In fact, emerging evidence shows that the relationship between training load and cardiovascular and brain health is not linear (the more exercise, the more benefit), but rather U-shaped. In recent years, long-term strenuous exercise has shown to increase the risk of cardiovascular disease, particularly cardiac arrhythmias , in certain populations. Atrial fibrillation risk is heightened in male endurance athletes, exercise is central to the pathophysiology of the arrhythmogenic cardiomyopathy in some patients, and the (formerly considered) physiological athlete’s bradycardia could evolve into clinically relevant sinus node dysfunction.[3] Moreover, recent data also suggest aortic tunica media damage in heavily trained animals[4] and more intense coronary atherosclerosis in male master marathon runners.[5] Could these negative effects extend to the brain? The robustness of clinical trials assessing long-term exposures is jeopardized by confounding factors and the lack of control groups. Although caution is needed when translating the conclusions of animal work to humans, preclinical studies restrict confounding factors and enable a more precise comparison between exercise loads. In an animal model, we have recently shown that high-intensity exercise could not replicate many of the moderate exercise-induced benefits on cerebrovascular and connectivity efficiency enhancement, both underlying improved learning capacity.[6] These results suggest that the maximal benefit of exercise in brain health is obtained at moderate doses while very high loads yield limited effects, thereby supporting the existence of a “sweet spot” for exercise intensity and duration on cognition.[7] Clinical studies confirm this notion, including a large epidemiological trial in which former professional athletes (i.e., soccer players) had a higher risk of neurodegenerative disease than the general population.[8] The pathophysiology behind many of the deleterious effects of strenuous exercise is only partially understood. The reduced cerebral blood flow occurring in an animal model after long-term strenuous training[6] would link the unexpected cognitive effects to recently demonstrated exercise-induced vascular damage.[4,5] Each strenuous bout of exercise superimposes a hemodynamic and biochemical stress on the cardiovascular system, which may result in transient right ventricular dysfunction.[9] Similarly, transient oxidative stress and altered mitochondrial energetics in the brain could mediate the observed reversal of benefits at high-intensity exercise.[6] Experiments conducted in human just after strenuous exercise support transient cognitive decline and impaired brain flow regulation. Finally, factors other than exercise itself may explain these deleterious effects. Repetitive concussion could not only contribute to deteriorate brain health in heavy contact sports, but also in sports such as football, as recently suggested.[10] Altogether, animal models and human trials suggest that the cognitive benefits promoted by regular exercise may be highly dependent on exercise intensity and duration; strenuous and long-term forms of exercise may paradoxically show deleterious. These data need to be confirmed, though, in large and well-designed studies. O’Keefe et al. appropriately state in their editorial[2] that “what is good for the heart is good for the brain.” Should we also claim that “what is bad for the heart is bad for the brain?” Financial support and sponsorship This work was partially supported by grants from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (PI19/00443 and PI22/00953), CERCA program/Generalitat de Catalunya, CIBERCV (16/11/00354), Consejo Superior de Deportes (EXP_75119), and the European Regional Development Fund (FEDER). Conflicts of interest Dr. Eduard Guasch is an Editorial Board member of Heart and Mind. The article was subject to the journal’s standard procedures, with peer review handled independently of Dr. Eduard Guasch and the research group. There are no conflicts of interest.\",\"PeriodicalId\":34653,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Heart and Mind\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-01-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Heart and Mind\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.4103/hm.hm-d-23-00014\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q4\",\"JCRName\":\"CARDIAC & CARDIOVASCULAR SYSTEMS\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Heart and Mind","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.4103/hm.hm-d-23-00014","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"CARDIAC & CARDIOVASCULAR SYSTEMS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

亲爱的编辑:我们怀着极大的兴趣阅读了最近一期的《心与心》杂志,讨论了运动、心脏和认知之间的关系。[1,2]詹娜·泰勒博士出色地总结了心血管和大脑健康之间的相互关系,以及有规律的适度和剧烈运动如何积极地影响心血管和认知之间的联系。[1]越来越多的核心证据支持运动是一种有希望的工具,可以降低心血管风险因素,也可以降低认知障碍。然而,尽管定期适度运动的好处是毋庸置疑的,并且有临床前和临床数据令人信服地支持,但长期剧烈运动的后果仍不确定。事实上,新出现的证据表明,训练负荷与心血管和大脑健康之间的关系不是线性的(运动越多,益处越多),而是u形的。近年来,长期剧烈运动已显示出在某些人群中增加心血管疾病,特别是心律失常的风险。男性耐力运动员房颤风险增高,运动是部分患者致心律失常性心肌病病理生理学的核心,(以前认为的)生理性运动员心动过缓可能演变为临床相关的窦房结功能障碍。[3]此外,最近的数据还表明,在训练有素的动物中存在主动脉中膜损伤[4],而男性马拉松大师运动员的冠状动脉粥样硬化更为严重[5]。这些负面影响会延伸到大脑吗?评估长期暴露的临床试验的稳健性因混杂因素和缺乏对照组而受到损害。虽然在将动物实验的结论转化为人类实验时需要谨慎,但临床前研究限制了混杂因素,并使运动负荷之间的比较更加精确。在动物模型中,我们最近表明,高强度运动不能复制许多中等强度运动引起的脑血管和连接效率增强的好处,这两者都是学习能力提高的基础。[6]这些结果表明,运动对大脑健康的最大益处是在中等剂量下获得的,而非常高的负荷产生的效果有限,从而支持运动强度和持续时间对认知的“最佳点”的存在。[7]临床研究证实了这一观点,包括一项大型流行病学试验,其中前职业运动员(即足球运动员)患神经退行性疾病的风险高于一般人群。[8]剧烈运动的许多有害影响背后的病理生理学只是部分被理解。在长期剧烈训练后的动物模型中出现的脑血流量减少[6]将意想不到的认知影响与最近证实的运动引起的血管损伤联系起来。[4,5]每次剧烈运动都会对心血管系统施加血液动力学和生化压力,这可能导致一过性右心室功能障碍[9]。同样,大脑中短暂的氧化应激和线粒体能量的改变可以介导观察到的高强度运动的益处逆转。[6]在剧烈运动后进行的人体实验支持短暂的认知能力下降和脑流量调节受损。最后,运动本身以外的因素也可以解释这些有害的影响。正如最近提出的那样,反复的脑震荡不仅会在激烈的身体接触运动中导致大脑健康恶化,而且在足球等运动中也会导致大脑健康恶化。[10]总之,动物模型和人体试验表明,定期运动所促进的认知益处可能高度依赖于运动强度和持续时间;矛盾的是,剧烈和长期的运动形式可能显示出有害。然而,这些数据需要在大型和精心设计的研究中得到证实。O 'Keefe等人在他们的社论[2]中恰当地指出,“对心脏有益的东西对大脑也有益。”我们是否也应该宣称“对心脏有害的东西也对大脑有害?”本研究得到了西班牙卡洛斯三世研究所(PI19/00443和PI22/00953)、CERCA项目/Generalitat de Catalunya、CIBERCV(16/11/00354)、Consejo Superior de Deportes (EXP_75119)和欧洲区域发展基金(FEDER)的部分资助。edward Guasch博士是Heart and Mind杂志的编辑委员会成员。这篇文章遵循了该杂志的标准程序,同行评议独立于爱德华·瓜施博士和研究小组进行。没有利益冲突。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
What is bad for the heart is bad for the brain?
Dear Editor, We have read with great interest the most recent issue of Heart and Mind addressing the relationship between exercise, heart, and cognition.[1,2] Dr. Jenna Taylor excellently summarized the intertwine between cardiovascular and brain health and how regular moderate and vigorous exercises positively impinge the cardiovascular-cognition connection.[1] The growing core of evidence supporting exercise as a promising tool to blunt cardiovascular risk factors, but also cognitive impairment, is exhaustively reviewed. However, while the benefits of regular moderate exercise are undoubted and convincingly supported by preclinical and clinical data, the consequences of long-term strenuous exercise are still uncertain. In fact, emerging evidence shows that the relationship between training load and cardiovascular and brain health is not linear (the more exercise, the more benefit), but rather U-shaped. In recent years, long-term strenuous exercise has shown to increase the risk of cardiovascular disease, particularly cardiac arrhythmias , in certain populations. Atrial fibrillation risk is heightened in male endurance athletes, exercise is central to the pathophysiology of the arrhythmogenic cardiomyopathy in some patients, and the (formerly considered) physiological athlete’s bradycardia could evolve into clinically relevant sinus node dysfunction.[3] Moreover, recent data also suggest aortic tunica media damage in heavily trained animals[4] and more intense coronary atherosclerosis in male master marathon runners.[5] Could these negative effects extend to the brain? The robustness of clinical trials assessing long-term exposures is jeopardized by confounding factors and the lack of control groups. Although caution is needed when translating the conclusions of animal work to humans, preclinical studies restrict confounding factors and enable a more precise comparison between exercise loads. In an animal model, we have recently shown that high-intensity exercise could not replicate many of the moderate exercise-induced benefits on cerebrovascular and connectivity efficiency enhancement, both underlying improved learning capacity.[6] These results suggest that the maximal benefit of exercise in brain health is obtained at moderate doses while very high loads yield limited effects, thereby supporting the existence of a “sweet spot” for exercise intensity and duration on cognition.[7] Clinical studies confirm this notion, including a large epidemiological trial in which former professional athletes (i.e., soccer players) had a higher risk of neurodegenerative disease than the general population.[8] The pathophysiology behind many of the deleterious effects of strenuous exercise is only partially understood. The reduced cerebral blood flow occurring in an animal model after long-term strenuous training[6] would link the unexpected cognitive effects to recently demonstrated exercise-induced vascular damage.[4,5] Each strenuous bout of exercise superimposes a hemodynamic and biochemical stress on the cardiovascular system, which may result in transient right ventricular dysfunction.[9] Similarly, transient oxidative stress and altered mitochondrial energetics in the brain could mediate the observed reversal of benefits at high-intensity exercise.[6] Experiments conducted in human just after strenuous exercise support transient cognitive decline and impaired brain flow regulation. Finally, factors other than exercise itself may explain these deleterious effects. Repetitive concussion could not only contribute to deteriorate brain health in heavy contact sports, but also in sports such as football, as recently suggested.[10] Altogether, animal models and human trials suggest that the cognitive benefits promoted by regular exercise may be highly dependent on exercise intensity and duration; strenuous and long-term forms of exercise may paradoxically show deleterious. These data need to be confirmed, though, in large and well-designed studies. O’Keefe et al. appropriately state in their editorial[2] that “what is good for the heart is good for the brain.” Should we also claim that “what is bad for the heart is bad for the brain?” Financial support and sponsorship This work was partially supported by grants from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (PI19/00443 and PI22/00953), CERCA program/Generalitat de Catalunya, CIBERCV (16/11/00354), Consejo Superior de Deportes (EXP_75119), and the European Regional Development Fund (FEDER). Conflicts of interest Dr. Eduard Guasch is an Editorial Board member of Heart and Mind. The article was subject to the journal’s standard procedures, with peer review handled independently of Dr. Eduard Guasch and the research group. There are no conflicts of interest.
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
1.10
自引率
0.00%
发文量
10
审稿时长
19 weeks
期刊最新文献
Depression and Adverse Cardiovascular Disease Outcomes: Cause or Consequence? New Insights and Clinical Practice on Heart, Brain, and Mind Enhancing Palliative Care for Deep Vein Thrombosis: A Scoping Review with Clinical Insights on the Integration of Physical Activity Adherent Right Atrial Cardiac Lipoma with Obstructive Symptoms: An Unusual Case Report Is Self-Reported Obstructive Sleep Apnea Associated with Cardiac Distress? A Network Analysis
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1