反叛管理理论

IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY American Journal of Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI:10.5406/19398298.136.3.08
Joachim I. Krueger
{"title":"反叛管理理论","authors":"Joachim I. Krueger","doi":"10.5406/19398298.136.3.08","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The sciences, and psychology is no exception, pose questions to nature and seek to move toward increasingly accurate and efficient models of reality (Popper, 1972). Science is fruitful when it offers effective applications. We can use what we have learned to craft interventions to get what we want. Psychological science has made many contributions to human welfare, education, therapy, and the management of people, among other things (Forgas, Crano, & Fiedler, 2020). These improvements do not require the assumption that human nature is fundamentally flawed. Human nature just is, and yet we might tweak things to our advantage. We are perfect the way we are, and we could use a little improvement. This is the Zen of psychology.The Art of Insubordination: How to Dissent & Defy Effectively is presented as a cookbook and handbook intended to promote an attitude of resistance to society's “unhelpful norms” (p. IX) and to provide insight into the skills that enable the “principled insubordinate” to prevail. Such an attitude, at least if shared by many, we are told, can deliver technological progress, individual happiness, and social harmony. Why is a revolution of insubordination necessary? In psychology, a narrative of original sin and redemption is not uncommon, and it is most evident in the rhetoric of bias and error (Krueger & Funder, 2004; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Wikipedia, the author reports, lists over 100 psychological biases. This deluge of irrationality is critical to the narrative because now we can ask, What if all these biases were eradicated? Many debiasing researchers have cut their teeth on this challenge—and broken a few—only to learn that many biases are features of a well-honed system (Krueger & Massey, 2009). Is an end run around the business of debiasing possible, such that “principled insubordination neutralizes our cognitive biases” (p. 44)?How might this be accomplished? Principled insubordination, we are told, promotes creativity, curiosity, and well-being. However, we are left wondering whether there are things that principled insubordination cannot accomplish. If it is the source of all that is good, beautiful, and true, we must rush to master and apply it. Why haven't we? Perhaps we haven't because many social norms have their uses (Sunstein, 2019). Majority opinions are often correct (Hastie & Kameda, 2005), and traditions can be empowering (Bicchieri, 2005; reviewed by Krueger, 2006). False majority opinions and oppressive traditions are—by definition—a problem, but the question is which social norms are false or oppressive and how we know the difference. Why might we want to assume that with all the cognitive biases infecting the ordinary mind, people are adept at telling insubordination-worthy social norms from beneficent ones? Assuming that people can, at least some of the time, tell the difference, are they still flawed by being overall too timid or too complacent with what is familiar?Some people do not lack courage. “A tribe of heroic bias bashers live among us” (p. 46), the author declares and reviews the works of some exemplary rebels, innovators, and catalysts of social change. An ideal type of rebel is Charles Darwin, who outmaneuvered the Church of England and his competitor Alfred Russell Wallace. Darwin, it seems, had mastered The Art of Principled Insubordination without having read it. Other historical thinkers were less fortunate. We read that al-Jāhiz was executed for his heretical ideas, although according to another tradition he died a scholar's death when a pile of books fell on his head (Ashtiani, Johnstone, Latham, Serjeant, & Smith, 2008). At any rate, the author announces that “The Art of Insubordination is what Darwin's thirty unlucky predecessors wish they had read before embarking on their lonely quests” (p. 9).The methods of insubordination work well, we are told, and “published studies provide the scientific evidence explaining why” (p. 7). Again, however, there is little guidance as to when to rebel and when to hold back. It is doubtful that rebels-in-waiting simply “know the difference between reckless and principled insubordination” (p. 15). Likewise, it is tautologically true that “dissenters boost the odds of convincing others if they take a careful measure of society's prejudices and calibrate their speech and actions accordingly” (p. 6); the question is how would-be rebels can gauge the risks they run in advance.To conceptualize this decision problem, let us consider a sketch of a Rebellion Management Theory conceived along the lines of standard frameworks (Heck & Krueger, 2015; Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, 2000). This theory would have us look at the intersections of reality and action. Reality may offer a friendly or a hostile environment (Soyer & Hogarth, 2020; reviewed by Grüning & Krueger, 2021). A friendly environment provides a stage suitable for change; a hostile environment does not. Action consists of either rebellion or conformity. If the would-be rebels have their wits about them, they will rebel in a friendly environment and conform in a hostile one. It is a psychological and an empirical question of whether they can tell the difference. If they cannot, then those who rebel in a hostile environment commit a type I error, a fool's errand, and those who fail to rebel in a friendly environment commit a type II error, a missed opportunity.This could be the end of it if a modest version of decision theory carried the day. But the author wants more. In his “cookbook” (p. 9), he aims to enable the would-be rebel to turn a hostile environment into a friendly one. That is, a pre-rebellion is necessary before the rebellion proper can hope to succeed. The author describes ingredients of such groundwork, reviewing research on how minorities can organize themselves, how allies can be won and kept, and how, to paraphrase his words, “a culture of dissent can be cultivated.” This is a strong claim, and a cautious reader would note that even if research produces statistically significant results, the prospects of turning a hostile environment into a friendly one remain highly uncertain. As a self-help book, “Insubordination” leans heavily into a can-do attitude. Caveat emptor, one wants to warn.If victory is achieved, there is still a dark side. George Orwell (1945) worried about successful rebels coming to resemble the oppressors they overthrew. Leon Trotsky (1906/2010) called for a continual state of revolution. The author shares the worry about what might happen if principled insubordination succeeded too well. Recalling the breaking-bad of Robespierre and former Bolivian president Evo Morales as poignant examples, he homes in on the contemporary scene, which is rife with attempts at cultural revolution performed in the name of social justice (for critical discussions see Pinker, 2021, reviewed by Krueger, 2022a; or Stanovich, 2021, reviewed by Krueger, 2022b). Examples of power grabs and vengeful overreach are readily observable in today's world.How can insurgence be domesticated into reconciliation? The author counsels the well-established strategies of intergroup contact, inclusive categorizations, and humor. Jokes, by their very nature, will make someone uncomfortable, especially those earnest creatures who have forgotten the lightness of being (Warren & McGraw, 2016). The author rightfully wonders: Are we prepared to abandon all humor? There is more interesting science in this book, including research on self-regulation techniques such as purposefulness (Kang, Strecher, Kim, & Falk, 2019), moral courage (Goodwin, Graham, & Diekmann, 2020), or self-distancing (Gainsburg & Kross, 2020). Alas, the style in which Insubordination is written detracts from the message. There is inappropriate hyperbole and cringeworthy attempts at humor, as when we read that some folks “lacked cojones” (p. 19), that others were “pissed” (p. 38), or that “bad shit goes down” (p. 92). There is an excess of profanity, as when “other important dimensions of daily life either totally suck, suck despite some recent improvement, or only moderately suck” (p. 40), when the Urban Dictionary is consulted to reveal that a particularly colorful expression refers to a “fucked up situation” (p. 56), or when the author asserts he is “not making this shit up” (p. 136). He hectors the reader, as when he exclaims, “I want you to behave more rebelliously. . . . As I like to say after a whiskey or two, insubordination is a portal to the adjacent possible” (p. 52), or “Don't be a naysaying asshole” (p. 67). “Disregard these principles,” he warns, “and you all but ensure your failure” (p. 60).If this were not enough, the author opens a window into his own world, taking a full paragraph (p. 102) to list ways in which he has failed to exercise prudent coping mechanisms. These admissions are unsettling, especially their aggressive tone and their being written in the present tense (“I provoke verbal altercations with strangers and loved ones. I attack people relentlessly online.”). The reader will be forgiven for being astonished. This intemperate style is a pity because some of the presented science is interesting and potentially important. To reiterate, the critical flaw in its presentation is the lack of attention to its limitations and boundary conditions. The reader interested in learning about the science and practice of dissent and rebellion is better served by, say, Sunstein (2003). The self-conscious and inappropriate style in which Insubordination is written might be a feature of a rebellion the goals of which are difficult to discern. However, it is fair to say that such unregulated prose does a disservice to psychological science and to the communication of its findings to the public.","PeriodicalId":48063,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9000,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Rebellion Management Theory\",\"authors\":\"Joachim I. Krueger\",\"doi\":\"10.5406/19398298.136.3.08\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"The sciences, and psychology is no exception, pose questions to nature and seek to move toward increasingly accurate and efficient models of reality (Popper, 1972). Science is fruitful when it offers effective applications. We can use what we have learned to craft interventions to get what we want. Psychological science has made many contributions to human welfare, education, therapy, and the management of people, among other things (Forgas, Crano, & Fiedler, 2020). These improvements do not require the assumption that human nature is fundamentally flawed. Human nature just is, and yet we might tweak things to our advantage. We are perfect the way we are, and we could use a little improvement. This is the Zen of psychology.The Art of Insubordination: How to Dissent & Defy Effectively is presented as a cookbook and handbook intended to promote an attitude of resistance to society's “unhelpful norms” (p. IX) and to provide insight into the skills that enable the “principled insubordinate” to prevail. Such an attitude, at least if shared by many, we are told, can deliver technological progress, individual happiness, and social harmony. Why is a revolution of insubordination necessary? In psychology, a narrative of original sin and redemption is not uncommon, and it is most evident in the rhetoric of bias and error (Krueger & Funder, 2004; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Wikipedia, the author reports, lists over 100 psychological biases. This deluge of irrationality is critical to the narrative because now we can ask, What if all these biases were eradicated? Many debiasing researchers have cut their teeth on this challenge—and broken a few—only to learn that many biases are features of a well-honed system (Krueger & Massey, 2009). Is an end run around the business of debiasing possible, such that “principled insubordination neutralizes our cognitive biases” (p. 44)?How might this be accomplished? Principled insubordination, we are told, promotes creativity, curiosity, and well-being. However, we are left wondering whether there are things that principled insubordination cannot accomplish. If it is the source of all that is good, beautiful, and true, we must rush to master and apply it. Why haven't we? Perhaps we haven't because many social norms have their uses (Sunstein, 2019). Majority opinions are often correct (Hastie & Kameda, 2005), and traditions can be empowering (Bicchieri, 2005; reviewed by Krueger, 2006). False majority opinions and oppressive traditions are—by definition—a problem, but the question is which social norms are false or oppressive and how we know the difference. Why might we want to assume that with all the cognitive biases infecting the ordinary mind, people are adept at telling insubordination-worthy social norms from beneficent ones? Assuming that people can, at least some of the time, tell the difference, are they still flawed by being overall too timid or too complacent with what is familiar?Some people do not lack courage. “A tribe of heroic bias bashers live among us” (p. 46), the author declares and reviews the works of some exemplary rebels, innovators, and catalysts of social change. An ideal type of rebel is Charles Darwin, who outmaneuvered the Church of England and his competitor Alfred Russell Wallace. Darwin, it seems, had mastered The Art of Principled Insubordination without having read it. Other historical thinkers were less fortunate. We read that al-Jāhiz was executed for his heretical ideas, although according to another tradition he died a scholar's death when a pile of books fell on his head (Ashtiani, Johnstone, Latham, Serjeant, & Smith, 2008). At any rate, the author announces that “The Art of Insubordination is what Darwin's thirty unlucky predecessors wish they had read before embarking on their lonely quests” (p. 9).The methods of insubordination work well, we are told, and “published studies provide the scientific evidence explaining why” (p. 7). Again, however, there is little guidance as to when to rebel and when to hold back. It is doubtful that rebels-in-waiting simply “know the difference between reckless and principled insubordination” (p. 15). Likewise, it is tautologically true that “dissenters boost the odds of convincing others if they take a careful measure of society's prejudices and calibrate their speech and actions accordingly” (p. 6); the question is how would-be rebels can gauge the risks they run in advance.To conceptualize this decision problem, let us consider a sketch of a Rebellion Management Theory conceived along the lines of standard frameworks (Heck & Krueger, 2015; Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, 2000). This theory would have us look at the intersections of reality and action. Reality may offer a friendly or a hostile environment (Soyer & Hogarth, 2020; reviewed by Grüning & Krueger, 2021). A friendly environment provides a stage suitable for change; a hostile environment does not. Action consists of either rebellion or conformity. If the would-be rebels have their wits about them, they will rebel in a friendly environment and conform in a hostile one. It is a psychological and an empirical question of whether they can tell the difference. If they cannot, then those who rebel in a hostile environment commit a type I error, a fool's errand, and those who fail to rebel in a friendly environment commit a type II error, a missed opportunity.This could be the end of it if a modest version of decision theory carried the day. But the author wants more. In his “cookbook” (p. 9), he aims to enable the would-be rebel to turn a hostile environment into a friendly one. That is, a pre-rebellion is necessary before the rebellion proper can hope to succeed. The author describes ingredients of such groundwork, reviewing research on how minorities can organize themselves, how allies can be won and kept, and how, to paraphrase his words, “a culture of dissent can be cultivated.” This is a strong claim, and a cautious reader would note that even if research produces statistically significant results, the prospects of turning a hostile environment into a friendly one remain highly uncertain. As a self-help book, “Insubordination” leans heavily into a can-do attitude. Caveat emptor, one wants to warn.If victory is achieved, there is still a dark side. George Orwell (1945) worried about successful rebels coming to resemble the oppressors they overthrew. Leon Trotsky (1906/2010) called for a continual state of revolution. The author shares the worry about what might happen if principled insubordination succeeded too well. Recalling the breaking-bad of Robespierre and former Bolivian president Evo Morales as poignant examples, he homes in on the contemporary scene, which is rife with attempts at cultural revolution performed in the name of social justice (for critical discussions see Pinker, 2021, reviewed by Krueger, 2022a; or Stanovich, 2021, reviewed by Krueger, 2022b). Examples of power grabs and vengeful overreach are readily observable in today's world.How can insurgence be domesticated into reconciliation? The author counsels the well-established strategies of intergroup contact, inclusive categorizations, and humor. Jokes, by their very nature, will make someone uncomfortable, especially those earnest creatures who have forgotten the lightness of being (Warren & McGraw, 2016). The author rightfully wonders: Are we prepared to abandon all humor? There is more interesting science in this book, including research on self-regulation techniques such as purposefulness (Kang, Strecher, Kim, & Falk, 2019), moral courage (Goodwin, Graham, & Diekmann, 2020), or self-distancing (Gainsburg & Kross, 2020). Alas, the style in which Insubordination is written detracts from the message. There is inappropriate hyperbole and cringeworthy attempts at humor, as when we read that some folks “lacked cojones” (p. 19), that others were “pissed” (p. 38), or that “bad shit goes down” (p. 92). There is an excess of profanity, as when “other important dimensions of daily life either totally suck, suck despite some recent improvement, or only moderately suck” (p. 40), when the Urban Dictionary is consulted to reveal that a particularly colorful expression refers to a “fucked up situation” (p. 56), or when the author asserts he is “not making this shit up” (p. 136). He hectors the reader, as when he exclaims, “I want you to behave more rebelliously. . . . As I like to say after a whiskey or two, insubordination is a portal to the adjacent possible” (p. 52), or “Don't be a naysaying asshole” (p. 67). “Disregard these principles,” he warns, “and you all but ensure your failure” (p. 60).If this were not enough, the author opens a window into his own world, taking a full paragraph (p. 102) to list ways in which he has failed to exercise prudent coping mechanisms. These admissions are unsettling, especially their aggressive tone and their being written in the present tense (“I provoke verbal altercations with strangers and loved ones. I attack people relentlessly online.”). The reader will be forgiven for being astonished. This intemperate style is a pity because some of the presented science is interesting and potentially important. To reiterate, the critical flaw in its presentation is the lack of attention to its limitations and boundary conditions. The reader interested in learning about the science and practice of dissent and rebellion is better served by, say, Sunstein (2003). The self-conscious and inappropriate style in which Insubordination is written might be a feature of a rebellion the goals of which are difficult to discern. However, it is fair to say that such unregulated prose does a disservice to psychological science and to the communication of its findings to the public.\",\"PeriodicalId\":48063,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"American Journal of Psychology\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.9000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-10-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"American Journal of Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.5406/19398298.136.3.08\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"American Journal of Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.5406/19398298.136.3.08","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

科学,心理学也不例外,向自然提出问题,并寻求走向越来越准确和有效的现实模型(Popper, 1972)。当科学提供有效的应用时,它是富有成果的。我们可以利用我们所学到的来精心设计干预措施来得到我们想要的。心理科学为人类福利、教育、治疗和人员管理做出了许多贡献(福加斯、克拉诺和费德勒,2020)。这些改进并不需要假设人性从根本上是有缺陷的。人的本性就是这样,但我们可能会把事情做得对我们有利。我们现在的样子已经很完美了,我们还需要一点改进。这就是心理学的禅宗。《不服从的艺术:如何有效地提出异议和反抗》是一本食谱和手册,旨在促进一种抵制社会“无益规范”的态度(第9页),并提供对使“有原则的不服从”占上风的技能的见解。我们被告知,这种态度,至少如果被许多人分享,可以带来技术进步、个人幸福和社会和谐。为什么不服从的革命是必要的?在心理学中,关于原罪和救赎的叙述并不罕见,在偏见和错误的修辞中最为明显(Krueger & Funder, 2004;Nisbett & Ross, 1980)。作者报告说,维基百科列出了100多种心理偏见。这种非理性的泛滥对叙事至关重要,因为现在我们可以问,如果所有这些偏见都被根除了呢?许多消除偏见的研究人员已经在这个挑战上有所突破——并且打破了一些——只是为了了解许多偏见是一个经过良好磨练的系统的特征(Krueger & Massey, 2009)。是否有可能绕过消除偏见的业务,从而“有原则的不服从可以中和我们的认知偏见”(第44页)?如何做到这一点呢?我们被告知,有原则的反抗能促进创造力、好奇心和幸福感。然而,我们不知道是否有原则性的不服从不能完成的事情。如果它是一切善、美、真之源,我们就必须赶紧掌握并运用它。我们为什么没有呢?也许我们没有,因为许多社会规范都有其用途(Sunstein, 2019)。多数人的意见往往是正确的(Hastie & Kameda, 2005),传统可以赋予权力(Bicchieri, 2005;Krueger, 2006)。错误的多数意见和压迫性的传统是一个问题,但问题是哪些社会规范是错误的或压迫性的,以及我们如何区分它们。为什么我们要假设,在所有的认知偏见影响着普通人的头脑的情况下,人们能够熟练地区分不服从的社会规范和仁慈的社会规范?假设人们至少在某些时候能够分辨出两者之间的区别,那么他们是否仍然因为对熟悉的事物过于胆怯或过于自满而存在缺陷呢?有些人并不缺乏勇气。“我们中间生活着一群英勇的偏见抨击者”(第46页),作者宣称并回顾了一些典型的反叛者、创新者和社会变革催化剂的作品。查尔斯·达尔文就是一个理想的叛逆者,他以智取的手段战胜了英国国教和他的竞争对手阿尔弗雷德·拉塞尔·华莱士。达尔文似乎没有读过《有原则的不服从的艺术》就掌握了这本书。其他历史思想家就没那么幸运了。我们读到al-Jāhiz因为他的异端思想而被处决,尽管根据另一种传统,当一堆书落在他的头上时,他以学者的身份死亡(Ashtiani, Johnstone, Latham, Serjeant, & Smith, 2008)。无论如何,作者宣称“《不服从的艺术》是达尔文的三十位不幸的前辈们在开始他们孤独的探索之前希望能读到的书”(第9页)。我们被告知,不服从的方法效果很好,而且“已发表的研究提供了解释原因的科学证据”(第7页)。然而,关于何时反叛、何时退让,再次没有什么指导。值得怀疑的是,等待中的叛逆者只是“知道鲁莽和有原则的不服从之间的区别”(第15页)。同样,“如果持不同政见者仔细衡量社会偏见,并相应地调整自己的言论和行动,那么他们说服他人的几率就会增加”(第6页);问题是,潜在的叛乱分子如何提前评估他们所面临的风险。为了概念化这个决策问题,让我们考虑一下按照标准框架(Heck & Krueger, 2015;Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, 2000)。这个理论会让我们看到现实和行动的交集。现实可能提供一个友好或敌对的环境(Soyer & Hogarth, 2020;gr<s:1>宁和克鲁格,2021年审查)。一个友好的环境提供了一个适合改变的舞台;充满敌意的环境则不然。行动要么是反叛,要么是顺从。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Rebellion Management Theory
The sciences, and psychology is no exception, pose questions to nature and seek to move toward increasingly accurate and efficient models of reality (Popper, 1972). Science is fruitful when it offers effective applications. We can use what we have learned to craft interventions to get what we want. Psychological science has made many contributions to human welfare, education, therapy, and the management of people, among other things (Forgas, Crano, & Fiedler, 2020). These improvements do not require the assumption that human nature is fundamentally flawed. Human nature just is, and yet we might tweak things to our advantage. We are perfect the way we are, and we could use a little improvement. This is the Zen of psychology.The Art of Insubordination: How to Dissent & Defy Effectively is presented as a cookbook and handbook intended to promote an attitude of resistance to society's “unhelpful norms” (p. IX) and to provide insight into the skills that enable the “principled insubordinate” to prevail. Such an attitude, at least if shared by many, we are told, can deliver technological progress, individual happiness, and social harmony. Why is a revolution of insubordination necessary? In psychology, a narrative of original sin and redemption is not uncommon, and it is most evident in the rhetoric of bias and error (Krueger & Funder, 2004; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Wikipedia, the author reports, lists over 100 psychological biases. This deluge of irrationality is critical to the narrative because now we can ask, What if all these biases were eradicated? Many debiasing researchers have cut their teeth on this challenge—and broken a few—only to learn that many biases are features of a well-honed system (Krueger & Massey, 2009). Is an end run around the business of debiasing possible, such that “principled insubordination neutralizes our cognitive biases” (p. 44)?How might this be accomplished? Principled insubordination, we are told, promotes creativity, curiosity, and well-being. However, we are left wondering whether there are things that principled insubordination cannot accomplish. If it is the source of all that is good, beautiful, and true, we must rush to master and apply it. Why haven't we? Perhaps we haven't because many social norms have their uses (Sunstein, 2019). Majority opinions are often correct (Hastie & Kameda, 2005), and traditions can be empowering (Bicchieri, 2005; reviewed by Krueger, 2006). False majority opinions and oppressive traditions are—by definition—a problem, but the question is which social norms are false or oppressive and how we know the difference. Why might we want to assume that with all the cognitive biases infecting the ordinary mind, people are adept at telling insubordination-worthy social norms from beneficent ones? Assuming that people can, at least some of the time, tell the difference, are they still flawed by being overall too timid or too complacent with what is familiar?Some people do not lack courage. “A tribe of heroic bias bashers live among us” (p. 46), the author declares and reviews the works of some exemplary rebels, innovators, and catalysts of social change. An ideal type of rebel is Charles Darwin, who outmaneuvered the Church of England and his competitor Alfred Russell Wallace. Darwin, it seems, had mastered The Art of Principled Insubordination without having read it. Other historical thinkers were less fortunate. We read that al-Jāhiz was executed for his heretical ideas, although according to another tradition he died a scholar's death when a pile of books fell on his head (Ashtiani, Johnstone, Latham, Serjeant, & Smith, 2008). At any rate, the author announces that “The Art of Insubordination is what Darwin's thirty unlucky predecessors wish they had read before embarking on their lonely quests” (p. 9).The methods of insubordination work well, we are told, and “published studies provide the scientific evidence explaining why” (p. 7). Again, however, there is little guidance as to when to rebel and when to hold back. It is doubtful that rebels-in-waiting simply “know the difference between reckless and principled insubordination” (p. 15). Likewise, it is tautologically true that “dissenters boost the odds of convincing others if they take a careful measure of society's prejudices and calibrate their speech and actions accordingly” (p. 6); the question is how would-be rebels can gauge the risks they run in advance.To conceptualize this decision problem, let us consider a sketch of a Rebellion Management Theory conceived along the lines of standard frameworks (Heck & Krueger, 2015; Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, 2000). This theory would have us look at the intersections of reality and action. Reality may offer a friendly or a hostile environment (Soyer & Hogarth, 2020; reviewed by Grüning & Krueger, 2021). A friendly environment provides a stage suitable for change; a hostile environment does not. Action consists of either rebellion or conformity. If the would-be rebels have their wits about them, they will rebel in a friendly environment and conform in a hostile one. It is a psychological and an empirical question of whether they can tell the difference. If they cannot, then those who rebel in a hostile environment commit a type I error, a fool's errand, and those who fail to rebel in a friendly environment commit a type II error, a missed opportunity.This could be the end of it if a modest version of decision theory carried the day. But the author wants more. In his “cookbook” (p. 9), he aims to enable the would-be rebel to turn a hostile environment into a friendly one. That is, a pre-rebellion is necessary before the rebellion proper can hope to succeed. The author describes ingredients of such groundwork, reviewing research on how minorities can organize themselves, how allies can be won and kept, and how, to paraphrase his words, “a culture of dissent can be cultivated.” This is a strong claim, and a cautious reader would note that even if research produces statistically significant results, the prospects of turning a hostile environment into a friendly one remain highly uncertain. As a self-help book, “Insubordination” leans heavily into a can-do attitude. Caveat emptor, one wants to warn.If victory is achieved, there is still a dark side. George Orwell (1945) worried about successful rebels coming to resemble the oppressors they overthrew. Leon Trotsky (1906/2010) called for a continual state of revolution. The author shares the worry about what might happen if principled insubordination succeeded too well. Recalling the breaking-bad of Robespierre and former Bolivian president Evo Morales as poignant examples, he homes in on the contemporary scene, which is rife with attempts at cultural revolution performed in the name of social justice (for critical discussions see Pinker, 2021, reviewed by Krueger, 2022a; or Stanovich, 2021, reviewed by Krueger, 2022b). Examples of power grabs and vengeful overreach are readily observable in today's world.How can insurgence be domesticated into reconciliation? The author counsels the well-established strategies of intergroup contact, inclusive categorizations, and humor. Jokes, by their very nature, will make someone uncomfortable, especially those earnest creatures who have forgotten the lightness of being (Warren & McGraw, 2016). The author rightfully wonders: Are we prepared to abandon all humor? There is more interesting science in this book, including research on self-regulation techniques such as purposefulness (Kang, Strecher, Kim, & Falk, 2019), moral courage (Goodwin, Graham, & Diekmann, 2020), or self-distancing (Gainsburg & Kross, 2020). Alas, the style in which Insubordination is written detracts from the message. There is inappropriate hyperbole and cringeworthy attempts at humor, as when we read that some folks “lacked cojones” (p. 19), that others were “pissed” (p. 38), or that “bad shit goes down” (p. 92). There is an excess of profanity, as when “other important dimensions of daily life either totally suck, suck despite some recent improvement, or only moderately suck” (p. 40), when the Urban Dictionary is consulted to reveal that a particularly colorful expression refers to a “fucked up situation” (p. 56), or when the author asserts he is “not making this shit up” (p. 136). He hectors the reader, as when he exclaims, “I want you to behave more rebelliously. . . . As I like to say after a whiskey or two, insubordination is a portal to the adjacent possible” (p. 52), or “Don't be a naysaying asshole” (p. 67). “Disregard these principles,” he warns, “and you all but ensure your failure” (p. 60).If this were not enough, the author opens a window into his own world, taking a full paragraph (p. 102) to list ways in which he has failed to exercise prudent coping mechanisms. These admissions are unsettling, especially their aggressive tone and their being written in the present tense (“I provoke verbal altercations with strangers and loved ones. I attack people relentlessly online.”). The reader will be forgiven for being astonished. This intemperate style is a pity because some of the presented science is interesting and potentially important. To reiterate, the critical flaw in its presentation is the lack of attention to its limitations and boundary conditions. The reader interested in learning about the science and practice of dissent and rebellion is better served by, say, Sunstein (2003). The self-conscious and inappropriate style in which Insubordination is written might be a feature of a rebellion the goals of which are difficult to discern. However, it is fair to say that such unregulated prose does a disservice to psychological science and to the communication of its findings to the public.
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
American Journal of Psychology
American Journal of Psychology PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY-
CiteScore
1.30
自引率
7.70%
发文量
35
期刊介绍: The American Journal of Psychology (AJP) was founded in 1887 by G. Stanley Hall and was edited in its early years by Titchener, Boring, and Dallenbach. The Journal has published some of the most innovative and formative papers in psychology throughout its history. AJP explores the science of the mind and behavior, publishing reports of original research in experimental psychology, theoretical presentations, combined theoretical and experimental analyses, historical commentaries, and in-depth reviews of significant books.
期刊最新文献
Africentric Perception of Intelligent Behaviours and the Use of Folklore to Facilitate Moral and Interpersonal Skills in Children Trauma and Percieved Internal Support of Police Officers in Kenya Entrepreneurial habits and Adolescent Socialization into Economic Independence among the Bamilékés of West Cameroon Rage at Strangers: Anger Elicitation and Regulation as a Function of Relationship Type Rebellion Management Theory
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1