睡眠与神经学、失眠、睡眠呼吸紊乱、噩梦等!

IF 3.4 3区 医学 Q2 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Journal of Sleep Research Pub Date : 2024-08-27 DOI:10.1111/jsr.14323
Dieter Riemann
{"title":"睡眠与神经学、失眠、睡眠呼吸紊乱、噩梦等!","authors":"Dieter Riemann","doi":"10.1111/jsr.14323","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dear members of the ESRS,</p><p>Dear readers of JSR,</p><p>welcome to the fifth edition of the Journal of Sleep Research in 2024, which will be published in October 2024.</p><p>This issue encompasses several reviews and meta-analyses on very different aspects of sleep research and sleep medicine, for example sensory gating and gaining in sleep or gastro-oesophageal reflux during sleep.</p><p>Furthermore, there is a broad range of articles covering sleep and neurology, insomnia, sleep-disordered breathing, nightmares, pain, memory consolidation, caffeine etc.</p><p>As always let me highlight a few of the articles for you:</p><p>Xie and Feeney (<span>2024</span>) touch on a largely neglected subject in sleep research: what is the impact of non-sexual touch on sleep quality? In their narrative review the authors want to shed light on associations between all kinds of non-sexual touch (affectionate touch, touch therapies, touch with animals and inanimate objects) and several aspects of sleep quality. It seems that so-called touch therapies (e.g. massage etc.) exert a positive influence on sleep quality. The same applies for affectionate touch (e.g. hugging). Touch deprivation and touch aversion, on the other hand, negatively impact sleep quality. Interestingly, positive associations between skin-to-skin contact and sleep quality has been shown clearly for newborns and infants.</p><p>The authors summarise that the existing database supports positive associations between touch and sleep quality, though further controlled studies are needed.</p><p>Sanli et al. (<span>2024</span>) investigated the effect of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) for the management of Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS). 46 randomly selected patients with RLS were randomized to a) treatment with pramipexole plus 10 sessions of TENS or b) only pramipexole for four weeks. The IRLRS and the PSQI were used as outcome measures. In the combined treatment, stronger effects on both scales were observed for the combination treatment at posttreatment and at follow-up. Thus this study gives preliminary evidence that TENS as add on therapy might be beneficial for patients with RLS.</p><p>Moulder et al. (<span>2024</span>) studied whether the therapeutic effects of cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) depend on pre-existing sleep–wake discrepancy. This discrepancy was defined as an inconsistency between a persons's subjective and objective sleep (a common phenomenon in patients with insomnia). To this end, the authors studied a real-world sample of 386 patients who were treated in their outpatient insomnia clinic. All patients received a polysomnographic sleep study and a sleep diary which was used to calculate the sleep–wake discrepancy. CBT-I according to clinical standards was delivered in 6 weekly face to face sessions. Major outcome measures were the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) and other questionnaires, which were administered pre- and post-treatment and at 3-months follow-up. According to the calculated sleep–wake discrepancy, the sample was split up in quintiles. With respect to specific sleep variables like sleep onset latency (SOL) or wake time after sleep onset (WASO) no difference could be found at post-treatment or follow-up between the five quintiles. In conclusion, the authors summarise that the extent of pre-existing sleep–wake discrepancy does not influence the outcome of CBT-I. These are important data, as they were gained from a real world clinical practice and as they stress that clinical CBT-I effects in patients with insomnia do not depend on sleep–wake state discrepancy.</p><p>Reichardt et al. (<span>2024</span>) were interested in the question whether naps containing REM sleep might facilitate the generalization of emotional memories. Mnemonic discrimination was measured with the emotional variant of the mnemonic separation task in a sample of 113 volunteers who spent a daytime nap between learning and testing, compared with another group who spent an equivalent time awake between both test sessions. Summarising, it was found that the discrimination of similar but previously unseen items to previously seen items, is enhanced in case of negative compared with neutral/ positive stimuli. Sleep and wake groups did not differ in memnoric performance. However, participants entering REM sleep when napping displayed increased generalisation of emotional memories, thus indicating that REM naps shape emotional memories in a way that enhances recognition at the expense of detailed memory representations.</p><p>Looking at a related issue, Lam et al. (<span>2024</span>) wanted to clarify whether a sleepless night might disrupt the resolution of emotional conflicts. 25 so-called healthy controls (young adults) underwent a protocol with a night of at home sleep control compared to a night of full sleep deprivation (SD) in the laboratory. Following both conditions, all participants completed an emotional conflict task with simultaneous EEG recordings to determine event related potentials (ERP). Emotional faces (happy or fearful) overlaid with words (fear or happy) were used as stimuli creating either congruent or incongruent trials – participants had to indicate whether the facial expression was happy or fearful. As outcomes accuracy and reaction times on the emotional conflict task and the P300 component of the ERPs were used. SD reduced alertness and led to longer reaction times and higher error rates. In the SD group, more errors occurred following congruent compared to incongruent trials. The authors conclude that these data support the assumption that SD may impair the regulation of emotional processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":17057,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Sleep Research","volume":"33 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.4000,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jsr.14323","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Sleep and neurology, insomnia, sleep disordered breathing, nightmares and much more!\",\"authors\":\"Dieter Riemann\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/jsr.14323\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Dear members of the ESRS,</p><p>Dear readers of JSR,</p><p>welcome to the fifth edition of the Journal of Sleep Research in 2024, which will be published in October 2024.</p><p>This issue encompasses several reviews and meta-analyses on very different aspects of sleep research and sleep medicine, for example sensory gating and gaining in sleep or gastro-oesophageal reflux during sleep.</p><p>Furthermore, there is a broad range of articles covering sleep and neurology, insomnia, sleep-disordered breathing, nightmares, pain, memory consolidation, caffeine etc.</p><p>As always let me highlight a few of the articles for you:</p><p>Xie and Feeney (<span>2024</span>) touch on a largely neglected subject in sleep research: what is the impact of non-sexual touch on sleep quality? In their narrative review the authors want to shed light on associations between all kinds of non-sexual touch (affectionate touch, touch therapies, touch with animals and inanimate objects) and several aspects of sleep quality. It seems that so-called touch therapies (e.g. massage etc.) exert a positive influence on sleep quality. The same applies for affectionate touch (e.g. hugging). Touch deprivation and touch aversion, on the other hand, negatively impact sleep quality. Interestingly, positive associations between skin-to-skin contact and sleep quality has been shown clearly for newborns and infants.</p><p>The authors summarise that the existing database supports positive associations between touch and sleep quality, though further controlled studies are needed.</p><p>Sanli et al. (<span>2024</span>) investigated the effect of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) for the management of Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS). 46 randomly selected patients with RLS were randomized to a) treatment with pramipexole plus 10 sessions of TENS or b) only pramipexole for four weeks. The IRLRS and the PSQI were used as outcome measures. In the combined treatment, stronger effects on both scales were observed for the combination treatment at posttreatment and at follow-up. Thus this study gives preliminary evidence that TENS as add on therapy might be beneficial for patients with RLS.</p><p>Moulder et al. (<span>2024</span>) studied whether the therapeutic effects of cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) depend on pre-existing sleep–wake discrepancy. This discrepancy was defined as an inconsistency between a persons's subjective and objective sleep (a common phenomenon in patients with insomnia). To this end, the authors studied a real-world sample of 386 patients who were treated in their outpatient insomnia clinic. All patients received a polysomnographic sleep study and a sleep diary which was used to calculate the sleep–wake discrepancy. CBT-I according to clinical standards was delivered in 6 weekly face to face sessions. Major outcome measures were the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) and other questionnaires, which were administered pre- and post-treatment and at 3-months follow-up. According to the calculated sleep–wake discrepancy, the sample was split up in quintiles. With respect to specific sleep variables like sleep onset latency (SOL) or wake time after sleep onset (WASO) no difference could be found at post-treatment or follow-up between the five quintiles. In conclusion, the authors summarise that the extent of pre-existing sleep–wake discrepancy does not influence the outcome of CBT-I. These are important data, as they were gained from a real world clinical practice and as they stress that clinical CBT-I effects in patients with insomnia do not depend on sleep–wake state discrepancy.</p><p>Reichardt et al. (<span>2024</span>) were interested in the question whether naps containing REM sleep might facilitate the generalization of emotional memories. Mnemonic discrimination was measured with the emotional variant of the mnemonic separation task in a sample of 113 volunteers who spent a daytime nap between learning and testing, compared with another group who spent an equivalent time awake between both test sessions. Summarising, it was found that the discrimination of similar but previously unseen items to previously seen items, is enhanced in case of negative compared with neutral/ positive stimuli. Sleep and wake groups did not differ in memnoric performance. However, participants entering REM sleep when napping displayed increased generalisation of emotional memories, thus indicating that REM naps shape emotional memories in a way that enhances recognition at the expense of detailed memory representations.</p><p>Looking at a related issue, Lam et al. (<span>2024</span>) wanted to clarify whether a sleepless night might disrupt the resolution of emotional conflicts. 25 so-called healthy controls (young adults) underwent a protocol with a night of at home sleep control compared to a night of full sleep deprivation (SD) in the laboratory. Following both conditions, all participants completed an emotional conflict task with simultaneous EEG recordings to determine event related potentials (ERP). Emotional faces (happy or fearful) overlaid with words (fear or happy) were used as stimuli creating either congruent or incongruent trials – participants had to indicate whether the facial expression was happy or fearful. As outcomes accuracy and reaction times on the emotional conflict task and the P300 component of the ERPs were used. SD reduced alertness and led to longer reaction times and higher error rates. In the SD group, more errors occurred following congruent compared to incongruent trials. The authors conclude that these data support the assumption that SD may impair the regulation of emotional processing.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":17057,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Journal of Sleep Research\",\"volume\":\"33 5\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":3.4000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-08-27\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jsr.14323\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Journal of Sleep Research\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jsr.14323\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"医学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"CLINICAL NEUROLOGY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Sleep Research","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jsr.14323","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"CLINICAL NEUROLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

情绪面孔(快乐或恐惧)与词语(恐惧或快乐)叠加在一起作为刺激物,产生一致或不一致的试验--参与者必须指出面部表情是快乐还是恐惧。情绪冲突任务的准确性和反应时间以及 ERP 的 P300 分量被用作结果。自毁降低了警觉性,导致反应时间延长和错误率升高。在自毁行为组中,与不一致试验相比,一致试验后出现的错误更多。作者总结说,这些数据支持自毁可能会损害情绪处理调节的假设。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Sleep and neurology, insomnia, sleep disordered breathing, nightmares and much more!

Dear members of the ESRS,

Dear readers of JSR,

welcome to the fifth edition of the Journal of Sleep Research in 2024, which will be published in October 2024.

This issue encompasses several reviews and meta-analyses on very different aspects of sleep research and sleep medicine, for example sensory gating and gaining in sleep or gastro-oesophageal reflux during sleep.

Furthermore, there is a broad range of articles covering sleep and neurology, insomnia, sleep-disordered breathing, nightmares, pain, memory consolidation, caffeine etc.

As always let me highlight a few of the articles for you:

Xie and Feeney (2024) touch on a largely neglected subject in sleep research: what is the impact of non-sexual touch on sleep quality? In their narrative review the authors want to shed light on associations between all kinds of non-sexual touch (affectionate touch, touch therapies, touch with animals and inanimate objects) and several aspects of sleep quality. It seems that so-called touch therapies (e.g. massage etc.) exert a positive influence on sleep quality. The same applies for affectionate touch (e.g. hugging). Touch deprivation and touch aversion, on the other hand, negatively impact sleep quality. Interestingly, positive associations between skin-to-skin contact and sleep quality has been shown clearly for newborns and infants.

The authors summarise that the existing database supports positive associations between touch and sleep quality, though further controlled studies are needed.

Sanli et al. (2024) investigated the effect of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) for the management of Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS). 46 randomly selected patients with RLS were randomized to a) treatment with pramipexole plus 10 sessions of TENS or b) only pramipexole for four weeks. The IRLRS and the PSQI were used as outcome measures. In the combined treatment, stronger effects on both scales were observed for the combination treatment at posttreatment and at follow-up. Thus this study gives preliminary evidence that TENS as add on therapy might be beneficial for patients with RLS.

Moulder et al. (2024) studied whether the therapeutic effects of cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) depend on pre-existing sleep–wake discrepancy. This discrepancy was defined as an inconsistency between a persons's subjective and objective sleep (a common phenomenon in patients with insomnia). To this end, the authors studied a real-world sample of 386 patients who were treated in their outpatient insomnia clinic. All patients received a polysomnographic sleep study and a sleep diary which was used to calculate the sleep–wake discrepancy. CBT-I according to clinical standards was delivered in 6 weekly face to face sessions. Major outcome measures were the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) and other questionnaires, which were administered pre- and post-treatment and at 3-months follow-up. According to the calculated sleep–wake discrepancy, the sample was split up in quintiles. With respect to specific sleep variables like sleep onset latency (SOL) or wake time after sleep onset (WASO) no difference could be found at post-treatment or follow-up between the five quintiles. In conclusion, the authors summarise that the extent of pre-existing sleep–wake discrepancy does not influence the outcome of CBT-I. These are important data, as they were gained from a real world clinical practice and as they stress that clinical CBT-I effects in patients with insomnia do not depend on sleep–wake state discrepancy.

Reichardt et al. (2024) were interested in the question whether naps containing REM sleep might facilitate the generalization of emotional memories. Mnemonic discrimination was measured with the emotional variant of the mnemonic separation task in a sample of 113 volunteers who spent a daytime nap between learning and testing, compared with another group who spent an equivalent time awake between both test sessions. Summarising, it was found that the discrimination of similar but previously unseen items to previously seen items, is enhanced in case of negative compared with neutral/ positive stimuli. Sleep and wake groups did not differ in memnoric performance. However, participants entering REM sleep when napping displayed increased generalisation of emotional memories, thus indicating that REM naps shape emotional memories in a way that enhances recognition at the expense of detailed memory representations.

Looking at a related issue, Lam et al. (2024) wanted to clarify whether a sleepless night might disrupt the resolution of emotional conflicts. 25 so-called healthy controls (young adults) underwent a protocol with a night of at home sleep control compared to a night of full sleep deprivation (SD) in the laboratory. Following both conditions, all participants completed an emotional conflict task with simultaneous EEG recordings to determine event related potentials (ERP). Emotional faces (happy or fearful) overlaid with words (fear or happy) were used as stimuli creating either congruent or incongruent trials – participants had to indicate whether the facial expression was happy or fearful. As outcomes accuracy and reaction times on the emotional conflict task and the P300 component of the ERPs were used. SD reduced alertness and led to longer reaction times and higher error rates. In the SD group, more errors occurred following congruent compared to incongruent trials. The authors conclude that these data support the assumption that SD may impair the regulation of emotional processing.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Journal of Sleep Research
Journal of Sleep Research 医学-临床神经学
CiteScore
9.00
自引率
6.80%
发文量
234
审稿时长
6-12 weeks
期刊介绍: The Journal of Sleep Research is dedicated to basic and clinical sleep research. The Journal publishes original research papers and invited reviews in all areas of sleep research (including biological rhythms). The Journal aims to promote the exchange of ideas between basic and clinical sleep researchers coming from a wide range of backgrounds and disciplines. The Journal will achieve this by publishing papers which use multidisciplinary and novel approaches to answer important questions about sleep, as well as its disorders and the treatment thereof.
期刊最新文献
Poor sleep quality is a risk factor for adverse clinical outcomes in patients with acute aortic dissection: A prospective cohort study. Pilot analysis of magnetic resonance imaging-based contributors to patient-centred optimization of mandibular advancement devices in obstructive sleep apnea. Differential effects of sleep position and sleep stage on the severity of obstructive sleep apnea. Sex-specific changes in sleep quality with aging: Insights from wearable device analysis. The efficacy and safety of dual orexin receptor antagonists in obstructive sleep apnea: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1