选择盆栽实验还是野外实验时需要权衡的问题

IF 8.3 1区 生物学 Q1 PLANT SCIENCES New Phytologist Pub Date : 2024-11-20 DOI:10.1111/nph.20292
Daniel Montesinos
{"title":"选择盆栽实验还是野外实验时需要权衡的问题","authors":"Daniel Montesinos","doi":"10.1111/nph.20292","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div>The ability to control and account for environmental conditions is essential when conducting ecological experiments. Two frequent approaches are pot and field experiments. Pot experiments under common garden conditions are excellent for detecting distinct plant responses to specific experimental treatments. Even maternal effects can be accounted for if plants are grown under the same conditions over generations. Pot experiments are exceptional at detecting even slight differences in response to highly controlled factors, but they are not always a meaningful representation of plant responses under more complex and variable natural habitats. <blockquote><p>‘… the most insightful finding by Zhu <i>et al</i>. was that only field mixtures allowed for the identification of traits that were predictive of future species abundance and persistence …’.</p>\n<div></div>\n</blockquote>\n</div>\n<p>Alternatively, field experiments allow for the manipulative control of a limited number of factors under realistic natural conditions. However, most environmental conditions are unaccounted for, making replication difficult, which often results in contradictory evidence. Hence, results may be unquestionably realistic but also unlikely to be consistently replicable, with results being contingent both on location and on time due to factors such as unaccounted variation in soil biota or differences in precipitation among years or between sites.</p>\n<p>Plant physiology and ecology often delve into the study of trade-offs. For a scientist designing an experiment, the choice of field vs common garden poses a fundamental choice with its own intrinsic trade-offs. Many individual studies have illustrated the limitations of pot vs field experiments, but a recent study by Zhu <i>et al</i>. (<span>2024</span>; https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.20160), published in <i>New Phytologist</i>, showcases these trade-offs at a particularly large scale (64 different species) and interesting context (exotic vs native species). The authors grew plants of each of the 64 species individually in pots and compared the traits of the different native and exotic species with mixes of the same species planted in the field.</p>\n<p>Zhu <i>et al</i>. found interesting results that generally confirmed expectations, but in the field study only, not the one in pots. The study was not without limitations: the comparison of plants grown in pots individually with plants grown in field mixes presented some caveats. It remains unclear whether any difference between native and exotic species - or the absence of a difference - could be due to the absence of competition in the pot experiment or to differences in other factors.</p>\n<p>We know that exotic species tend to outperform natives in most scenarios (van Kleunen <i>et al</i>., <span>2010</span>), but those advantages are sometimes evident only under certain conditions, typically under disturbance (Jauni <i>et al</i>., <span>2015</span>; Xiao <i>et al</i>., <span>2016</span>; Montesinos, <span>2022</span>), when resources are abundant or increasing (Davis <i>et al</i>., <span>2000</span>; Zhang <i>et al</i>., <span>2022</span>; Arias <i>et al</i>., <span>2023</span>) or, alternatively, only when resources are low, or plants experience stress (Funk &amp; Vitousek, <span>2007</span>; Santamarina <i>et al</i>., <span>2022</span>). It is possible that the differences observed by Zhu <i>et al</i>. between both experiments were due to the more competitive conditions experienced by plants under field conditions. We cannot discard that a pot experiment involving a mix of species similar to that in the field experiment would not have resulted in similar results. However, at least some previous studies with similar experimental designs found that competition was a factor in reducing - not increasing - trait differences between natives and exotics (Blumenthaal &amp; Hufbauer, <span>2007</span>). This suggests that the problem might not be the absence of competition in Zhu <i>et al</i>.'s pot experiment, but rather the intrinsic limitations associated with pot experiments.</p>\n<p>Perhaps the most insightful finding by Zhu <i>et al</i>. was that only field mixtures allowed for the identification of traits that were predictive of future species abundance and persistence (specific leaf area (SLA), plant height, aboveground biomass). Field mixtures also showed that exotics were overall more persistent than natives, and perennials more than annuals. These results agree with common expectations for native–exotic species interactions, and some previous studies have shown how field studies can detect native and exotic differences that pot experiments are not able to discern (Leffler <i>et al</i>., <span>2014</span>). Still, previous studies are limited to one or a few species for such comparisons, and meta-analysis such as those by Leffler <i>et al</i>. (<span>2014</span>) provide an integrative view that cannot, however, provide direct evidence in the way that a single study comparing 64 species simultaneously can.</p>\n<p>It is therefore comforting to see large scale experiments verifying our theoretical assumptions and yet it is concerning to recognize that pot experiments might be missing so much. Two rapid conclusions can be drawn: first, pot experiments are safe in that they are unlikely to unrealistically magnify or overrepresent existing differences, that is, they are unlikely to result in a type I statistical error (false positive); second, pot experiments might mislead us into ignoring factors and traits that would be highly informative if studied in the field. That is, they are highly likely to result in type II statistical errors (false negative).</p>\n<p>This is a sobering insight, and the confirmation of it both by meta-analysis based on numerous, but small, studies (Leffler <i>et al</i>., <span>2014</span>), and also by large scale experiments such as the one by Zhu <i>et al</i>., should make us reconsider the trade-offs involved. There will always be a need to run experiments in highly controlled pot experiments, but those experiments should focus on specific traits or treatments that have been broadly identified in the field beforehand. Exploring a broad range of traits in pot experiments to determine which ones will be meaningful predictors might appear as an efficient and attractive option, but it could end up being efficient in appearance only. It is likely that such an approach would erroneously lead us to ignore important predictive traits or factors that should have been identified previously via field studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":214,"journal":{"name":"New Phytologist","volume":"17 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":8.3000,"publicationDate":"2024-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Trade-offs involved in the choice of pot vs field experiments\",\"authors\":\"Daniel Montesinos\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/nph.20292\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div>The ability to control and account for environmental conditions is essential when conducting ecological experiments. Two frequent approaches are pot and field experiments. Pot experiments under common garden conditions are excellent for detecting distinct plant responses to specific experimental treatments. Even maternal effects can be accounted for if plants are grown under the same conditions over generations. Pot experiments are exceptional at detecting even slight differences in response to highly controlled factors, but they are not always a meaningful representation of plant responses under more complex and variable natural habitats. <blockquote><p>‘… the most insightful finding by Zhu <i>et al</i>. was that only field mixtures allowed for the identification of traits that were predictive of future species abundance and persistence …’.</p>\\n<div></div>\\n</blockquote>\\n</div>\\n<p>Alternatively, field experiments allow for the manipulative control of a limited number of factors under realistic natural conditions. However, most environmental conditions are unaccounted for, making replication difficult, which often results in contradictory evidence. Hence, results may be unquestionably realistic but also unlikely to be consistently replicable, with results being contingent both on location and on time due to factors such as unaccounted variation in soil biota or differences in precipitation among years or between sites.</p>\\n<p>Plant physiology and ecology often delve into the study of trade-offs. For a scientist designing an experiment, the choice of field vs common garden poses a fundamental choice with its own intrinsic trade-offs. Many individual studies have illustrated the limitations of pot vs field experiments, but a recent study by Zhu <i>et al</i>. (<span>2024</span>; https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.20160), published in <i>New Phytologist</i>, showcases these trade-offs at a particularly large scale (64 different species) and interesting context (exotic vs native species). The authors grew plants of each of the 64 species individually in pots and compared the traits of the different native and exotic species with mixes of the same species planted in the field.</p>\\n<p>Zhu <i>et al</i>. found interesting results that generally confirmed expectations, but in the field study only, not the one in pots. The study was not without limitations: the comparison of plants grown in pots individually with plants grown in field mixes presented some caveats. It remains unclear whether any difference between native and exotic species - or the absence of a difference - could be due to the absence of competition in the pot experiment or to differences in other factors.</p>\\n<p>We know that exotic species tend to outperform natives in most scenarios (van Kleunen <i>et al</i>., <span>2010</span>), but those advantages are sometimes evident only under certain conditions, typically under disturbance (Jauni <i>et al</i>., <span>2015</span>; Xiao <i>et al</i>., <span>2016</span>; Montesinos, <span>2022</span>), when resources are abundant or increasing (Davis <i>et al</i>., <span>2000</span>; Zhang <i>et al</i>., <span>2022</span>; Arias <i>et al</i>., <span>2023</span>) or, alternatively, only when resources are low, or plants experience stress (Funk &amp; Vitousek, <span>2007</span>; Santamarina <i>et al</i>., <span>2022</span>). It is possible that the differences observed by Zhu <i>et al</i>. between both experiments were due to the more competitive conditions experienced by plants under field conditions. We cannot discard that a pot experiment involving a mix of species similar to that in the field experiment would not have resulted in similar results. However, at least some previous studies with similar experimental designs found that competition was a factor in reducing - not increasing - trait differences between natives and exotics (Blumenthaal &amp; Hufbauer, <span>2007</span>). This suggests that the problem might not be the absence of competition in Zhu <i>et al</i>.'s pot experiment, but rather the intrinsic limitations associated with pot experiments.</p>\\n<p>Perhaps the most insightful finding by Zhu <i>et al</i>. was that only field mixtures allowed for the identification of traits that were predictive of future species abundance and persistence (specific leaf area (SLA), plant height, aboveground biomass). Field mixtures also showed that exotics were overall more persistent than natives, and perennials more than annuals. These results agree with common expectations for native–exotic species interactions, and some previous studies have shown how field studies can detect native and exotic differences that pot experiments are not able to discern (Leffler <i>et al</i>., <span>2014</span>). Still, previous studies are limited to one or a few species for such comparisons, and meta-analysis such as those by Leffler <i>et al</i>. (<span>2014</span>) provide an integrative view that cannot, however, provide direct evidence in the way that a single study comparing 64 species simultaneously can.</p>\\n<p>It is therefore comforting to see large scale experiments verifying our theoretical assumptions and yet it is concerning to recognize that pot experiments might be missing so much. Two rapid conclusions can be drawn: first, pot experiments are safe in that they are unlikely to unrealistically magnify or overrepresent existing differences, that is, they are unlikely to result in a type I statistical error (false positive); second, pot experiments might mislead us into ignoring factors and traits that would be highly informative if studied in the field. That is, they are highly likely to result in type II statistical errors (false negative).</p>\\n<p>This is a sobering insight, and the confirmation of it both by meta-analysis based on numerous, but small, studies (Leffler <i>et al</i>., <span>2014</span>), and also by large scale experiments such as the one by Zhu <i>et al</i>., should make us reconsider the trade-offs involved. There will always be a need to run experiments in highly controlled pot experiments, but those experiments should focus on specific traits or treatments that have been broadly identified in the field beforehand. Exploring a broad range of traits in pot experiments to determine which ones will be meaningful predictors might appear as an efficient and attractive option, but it could end up being efficient in appearance only. It is likely that such an approach would erroneously lead us to ignore important predictive traits or factors that should have been identified previously via field studies.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":214,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"New Phytologist\",\"volume\":\"17 1\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":8.3000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-11-20\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"New Phytologist\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"99\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.20292\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"生物学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"PLANT SCIENCES\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"New Phytologist","FirstCategoryId":"99","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.20292","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PLANT SCIENCES","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

在进行生态实验时,控制和考虑环境条件的能力至关重要。常用的两种方法是盆栽实验和田间试验。在普通花园条件下进行的盆栽实验可以很好地检测植物对特定实验处理的不同反应。如果几代植物都在相同的条件下生长,甚至母本效应也能被考虑在内。盆栽实验在检测植物对高度受控因素的反应的微小差异方面非常出色,但并不总能代表植物在更复杂多变的自然生境下的反应。Zhu等人最有洞察力的发现是,只有田间混合物才能确定预测未来物种丰度和持久性的性状...... "另外,田间试验可以在真实的自然条件下对有限的因素进行人为控制。然而,大多数环境条件都是无法估量的,因此很难进行复制,这往往会导致证据相互矛盾。因此,实验结果可能无疑是真实的,但也不可能始终如一地具有可重复性,由于土壤生物区系的变化或不同年份或不同地点之间降水量的差异等因素的影响,实验结果既取决于地点,也取决于时间。对于设计实验的科学家来说,选择田间还是普通花园是一个基本的选择,这本身就存在权衡问题。许多个别研究已经说明了盆栽实验与野外实验的局限性,但最近发表在《新植物学家》(New Phytologist)上的 Zhu 等人(2024; https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.20160)的一项研究以特别大的规模(64 个不同物种)和有趣的背景(外来物种与本地物种)展示了这些权衡。朱等人发现了一些有趣的结果,总体上证实了人们的预期,但这只是在实地研究中,而不是在盆栽研究中。这项研究并非没有局限性:将盆栽植物与田间混合种植的植物进行比较存在一些问题。我们知道,在大多数情况下,外来物种往往优于本地物种(van Kleunen et al、我们知道,在大多数情况下,外来物种的表现往往优于本地物种(van Kleunen 等人,2010 年),但这些优势有时仅在特定条件下才会显现,通常是在干扰条件下(Jauni 等人,2015 年;Xiao 等人,2016 年;Montesinos,2022 年)、资源丰富或增加时(Davis 等人,2000 年;Zhang 等人,2022 年;Arias 等人,2023 年),或者仅在资源较少或植物遭受压力时(Funk &amp; Vitousek, 2007; Santamarina 等人,2022 年)。Zhu 等人在两个实验中观察到的差异可能是由于植物在田间条件下经历了更激烈的竞争。我们不能排除盆栽实验中的物种组合与田间实验中的物种组合相似也不会产生类似的结果。不过,至少之前一些采用类似实验设计的研究发现,竞争是减少而非增加本地与外来物种之间性状差异的一个因素(Blumenthaal &amp; Hufbauer, 2007)。这表明,问题可能不在于 Zhu 等人的盆栽实验中没有竞争,而是与盆栽实验相关的内在局限性。Zhu 等人最有洞察力的发现或许是,只有田间混合物才能确定可预测未来物种丰度和持久性的性状(比叶面积、株高、地上生物量)。野外混合物还表明,外来物种的持久性总体上高于本地物种,多年生植物的持久性高于一年生植物。这些结果符合人们对本地与外来物种相互作用的普遍预期,而且之前的一些研究也表明,田间研究可以发现盆栽实验无法辨别的本地与外来物种差异(Leffler 等人,2014 年)。不过,以前的研究仅限于对一个或几个物种进行比较,而像 Leffler 等人(2014 年)所做的荟萃分析提供了一个综合视角,但无法像同时比较 64 个物种的单一研究那样提供直接证据。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Trade-offs involved in the choice of pot vs field experiments
The ability to control and account for environmental conditions is essential when conducting ecological experiments. Two frequent approaches are pot and field experiments. Pot experiments under common garden conditions are excellent for detecting distinct plant responses to specific experimental treatments. Even maternal effects can be accounted for if plants are grown under the same conditions over generations. Pot experiments are exceptional at detecting even slight differences in response to highly controlled factors, but they are not always a meaningful representation of plant responses under more complex and variable natural habitats.

‘… the most insightful finding by Zhu et al. was that only field mixtures allowed for the identification of traits that were predictive of future species abundance and persistence …’.

Alternatively, field experiments allow for the manipulative control of a limited number of factors under realistic natural conditions. However, most environmental conditions are unaccounted for, making replication difficult, which often results in contradictory evidence. Hence, results may be unquestionably realistic but also unlikely to be consistently replicable, with results being contingent both on location and on time due to factors such as unaccounted variation in soil biota or differences in precipitation among years or between sites.

Plant physiology and ecology often delve into the study of trade-offs. For a scientist designing an experiment, the choice of field vs common garden poses a fundamental choice with its own intrinsic trade-offs. Many individual studies have illustrated the limitations of pot vs field experiments, but a recent study by Zhu et al. (2024; https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.20160), published in New Phytologist, showcases these trade-offs at a particularly large scale (64 different species) and interesting context (exotic vs native species). The authors grew plants of each of the 64 species individually in pots and compared the traits of the different native and exotic species with mixes of the same species planted in the field.

Zhu et al. found interesting results that generally confirmed expectations, but in the field study only, not the one in pots. The study was not without limitations: the comparison of plants grown in pots individually with plants grown in field mixes presented some caveats. It remains unclear whether any difference between native and exotic species - or the absence of a difference - could be due to the absence of competition in the pot experiment or to differences in other factors.

We know that exotic species tend to outperform natives in most scenarios (van Kleunen et al., 2010), but those advantages are sometimes evident only under certain conditions, typically under disturbance (Jauni et al., 2015; Xiao et al., 2016; Montesinos, 2022), when resources are abundant or increasing (Davis et al., 2000; Zhang et al., 2022; Arias et al., 2023) or, alternatively, only when resources are low, or plants experience stress (Funk & Vitousek, 2007; Santamarina et al., 2022). It is possible that the differences observed by Zhu et al. between both experiments were due to the more competitive conditions experienced by plants under field conditions. We cannot discard that a pot experiment involving a mix of species similar to that in the field experiment would not have resulted in similar results. However, at least some previous studies with similar experimental designs found that competition was a factor in reducing - not increasing - trait differences between natives and exotics (Blumenthaal & Hufbauer, 2007). This suggests that the problem might not be the absence of competition in Zhu et al.'s pot experiment, but rather the intrinsic limitations associated with pot experiments.

Perhaps the most insightful finding by Zhu et al. was that only field mixtures allowed for the identification of traits that were predictive of future species abundance and persistence (specific leaf area (SLA), plant height, aboveground biomass). Field mixtures also showed that exotics were overall more persistent than natives, and perennials more than annuals. These results agree with common expectations for native–exotic species interactions, and some previous studies have shown how field studies can detect native and exotic differences that pot experiments are not able to discern (Leffler et al., 2014). Still, previous studies are limited to one or a few species for such comparisons, and meta-analysis such as those by Leffler et al. (2014) provide an integrative view that cannot, however, provide direct evidence in the way that a single study comparing 64 species simultaneously can.

It is therefore comforting to see large scale experiments verifying our theoretical assumptions and yet it is concerning to recognize that pot experiments might be missing so much. Two rapid conclusions can be drawn: first, pot experiments are safe in that they are unlikely to unrealistically magnify or overrepresent existing differences, that is, they are unlikely to result in a type I statistical error (false positive); second, pot experiments might mislead us into ignoring factors and traits that would be highly informative if studied in the field. That is, they are highly likely to result in type II statistical errors (false negative).

This is a sobering insight, and the confirmation of it both by meta-analysis based on numerous, but small, studies (Leffler et al., 2014), and also by large scale experiments such as the one by Zhu et al., should make us reconsider the trade-offs involved. There will always be a need to run experiments in highly controlled pot experiments, but those experiments should focus on specific traits or treatments that have been broadly identified in the field beforehand. Exploring a broad range of traits in pot experiments to determine which ones will be meaningful predictors might appear as an efficient and attractive option, but it could end up being efficient in appearance only. It is likely that such an approach would erroneously lead us to ignore important predictive traits or factors that should have been identified previously via field studies.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
New Phytologist
New Phytologist 生物-植物科学
自引率
5.30%
发文量
728
期刊介绍: New Phytologist is an international electronic journal published 24 times a year. It is owned by the New Phytologist Foundation, a non-profit-making charitable organization dedicated to promoting plant science. The journal publishes excellent, novel, rigorous, and timely research and scholarship in plant science and its applications. The articles cover topics in five sections: Physiology & Development, Environment, Interaction, Evolution, and Transformative Plant Biotechnology. These sections encompass intracellular processes, global environmental change, and encourage cross-disciplinary approaches. The journal recognizes the use of techniques from molecular and cell biology, functional genomics, modeling, and system-based approaches in plant science. Abstracting and Indexing Information for New Phytologist includes Academic Search, AgBiotech News & Information, Agroforestry Abstracts, Biochemistry & Biophysics Citation Index, Botanical Pesticides, CAB Abstracts®, Environment Index, Global Health, and Plant Breeding Abstracts, and others.
期刊最新文献
Pinpointing the timing of meiosis: a critical factor in evaluating the impact of abiotic stresses on the fertility of cereal crops Meta-analysis reveals globally sourced commercial mycorrhizal inoculants fall short Trade-offs involved in the choice of pot vs field experiments Herbicidal interference: glyphosate drives both the ecology and evolution of plant–herbivore interactions Matthew Naish
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1