{"title":"互惠声誉管理:学龄前儿童用共同指责来回应共同荣誉","authors":"Trisha Katz, Michael Tomasello","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101519","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In a single experiment, we asked whether children would be more likely to accept blame for another’s transgression when the individual had previously told a prosocial lie that improved the child’s reputation. 3- and 5-year-old children (<em>N</em>=120) were introduced to two puppets, one of whom needed help sorting toys and the other of whom helped. In the reciprocity condition, in response to the other's questioning, the helper puppet gave undue credit to the child for helping sort the toys; in the control condition the helper (accurately) took all the credit himself. Subsequently, the helper puppet transgressed by making a loud noise while the first puppet slept. In response to being roused, the first puppet blamed both the true transgressor and the innocent child. Upon being inappropriately blamed, 5-year-olds (but not 3-year-olds) behaved more prosocially by more often accepting inappropriate blame (implicitly). Five-year-olds (but-not 3-year-olds) also actively lied more often about their blameworthiness in the reciprocity condition. The fact that children reciprocated undue credit with the act of sharing blame suggests that, by age 5, children feel compelled to reciprocate intangible, reputational favors.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101519"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8000,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Reciprocal reputation management: Preschoolers respond to shared credit with shared blame\",\"authors\":\"Trisha Katz, Michael Tomasello\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101519\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><div>In a single experiment, we asked whether children would be more likely to accept blame for another’s transgression when the individual had previously told a prosocial lie that improved the child’s reputation. 3- and 5-year-old children (<em>N</em>=120) were introduced to two puppets, one of whom needed help sorting toys and the other of whom helped. In the reciprocity condition, in response to the other's questioning, the helper puppet gave undue credit to the child for helping sort the toys; in the control condition the helper (accurately) took all the credit himself. Subsequently, the helper puppet transgressed by making a loud noise while the first puppet slept. In response to being roused, the first puppet blamed both the true transgressor and the innocent child. Upon being inappropriately blamed, 5-year-olds (but not 3-year-olds) behaved more prosocially by more often accepting inappropriate blame (implicitly). Five-year-olds (but-not 3-year-olds) also actively lied more often about their blameworthiness in the reciprocity condition. The fact that children reciprocated undue credit with the act of sharing blame suggests that, by age 5, children feel compelled to reciprocate intangible, reputational favors.</div></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":51422,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Cognitive Development\",\"volume\":\"72 \",\"pages\":\"Article 101519\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.8000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-10-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Cognitive Development\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424001047\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Cognitive Development","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424001047","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
Reciprocal reputation management: Preschoolers respond to shared credit with shared blame
In a single experiment, we asked whether children would be more likely to accept blame for another’s transgression when the individual had previously told a prosocial lie that improved the child’s reputation. 3- and 5-year-old children (N=120) were introduced to two puppets, one of whom needed help sorting toys and the other of whom helped. In the reciprocity condition, in response to the other's questioning, the helper puppet gave undue credit to the child for helping sort the toys; in the control condition the helper (accurately) took all the credit himself. Subsequently, the helper puppet transgressed by making a loud noise while the first puppet slept. In response to being roused, the first puppet blamed both the true transgressor and the innocent child. Upon being inappropriately blamed, 5-year-olds (but not 3-year-olds) behaved more prosocially by more often accepting inappropriate blame (implicitly). Five-year-olds (but-not 3-year-olds) also actively lied more often about their blameworthiness in the reciprocity condition. The fact that children reciprocated undue credit with the act of sharing blame suggests that, by age 5, children feel compelled to reciprocate intangible, reputational favors.
期刊介绍:
Cognitive Development contains the very best empirical and theoretical work on the development of perception, memory, language, concepts, thinking, problem solving, metacognition, and social cognition. Criteria for acceptance of articles will be: significance of the work to issues of current interest, substance of the argument, and clarity of expression. For purposes of publication in Cognitive Development, moral and social development will be considered part of cognitive development when they are related to the development of knowledge or thought processes.