首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Development最新文献

英文 中文
Development of L1-L2 naming skills in a monolingual context: Evidence from children and adolescents 单语环境下 L1-L2 命名能力的发展:来自儿童和青少年的证据
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101492

Adolescence is marked by significant developmental changes that can influence language processing and control. This study aimed to uncover developmental differences in language co-activation and control in unbalanced Spanish (L1)-English (L2) bilinguals. Children and adolescents attending bilingual schools within a L1 monolingual context completed a picture-naming task including cognates and non-cognates nouns, with collection of behavioral and ERP data. The study consistently found a cognate facilitation effect (CFE) in L2, evident in enhanced accuracy, faster reaction times, and reduced N400 negativity for cognates in comparison with no-cognate nouns. However, in L1, CFE was only observed in the N400 component, indicating weaker transfer from L2 to L1. Additionally, children exhibited greater N200 negativity when naming cognates in L1, while adolescents showed no N200 modulations, suggesting differences in frontal control region involvement and potential differences in control strategies. Language co-activation appears independent of maturation, while language control depends on development.

青春期的显著发育变化会影响语言处理和控制。本研究旨在揭示不平衡的西班牙语(第一语言)-英语(第二语言)双语者在语言共同激活和控制方面的发展差异。在母语为单语的环境下,在双语学校就读的儿童和青少年完成了一项图片命名任务,包括认知和非认知名词,并收集了行为和ERP数据。研究一致发现,在 L2 中存在认知促进效应(CFE),表现为认知名词与非认知名词相比,认知名词的准确性提高、反应时间加快、N400 负性降低。然而,在 L1 中,只在 N400 成分中观察到 CFE,这表明从 L2 到 L1 的迁移较弱。此外,儿童在用 L1 命名同义词时表现出更大的 N200 负性,而青少年则没有表现出 N200 调节,这表明额叶控制区参与的差异和控制策略的潜在差异。语言共同激活似乎与成熟无关,而语言控制则取决于发育情况。
{"title":"Development of L1-L2 naming skills in a monolingual context: Evidence from children and adolescents","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101492","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101492","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Adolescence is marked by significant developmental changes that can influence language processing and control. This study aimed to uncover developmental differences in language co-activation and control in unbalanced Spanish (L1)-English (L2) bilinguals. Children and adolescents attending bilingual schools within a L1 monolingual context completed a picture-naming task including cognates and non-cognates nouns, with collection of behavioral and ERP data. The study consistently found a cognate facilitation effect (CFE) in L2, evident in enhanced accuracy, faster reaction times, and reduced N400 negativity for cognates in comparison with no-cognate nouns. However, in L1, CFE was only observed in the N400 component, indicating weaker transfer from L2 to L1. Additionally, children exhibited greater N200 negativity when naming cognates in L1, while adolescents showed no N200 modulations, suggesting differences in frontal control region involvement and potential differences in control strategies. Language co-activation appears independent of maturation, while language control depends on development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000777/pdfft?md5=878b3e6bde9c9e49c6dc1ad47de64ec2&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000777-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The shape bias in Mandarin-exposed young autistic children: The role of abstract shape representation 接触普通话的自闭症幼儿的形状偏差:抽象形状表征的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101491

The shape bias is an important word learning strategy in children’s language development. Although some studies have observed an absent or atypical shape bias in autistic children, there is no converging evidence regarding its underlying bases. Moreover, previous research has been exclusively conducted in learners of Indo-European languages, yet it is unclear whether the shape bias is a universal word learning constraint across languages. This study aims to investigate the shape bias and its association with shape representation ability in 40 1–3-year-old non-autistic children, and 41 2–6-year-old autistic children, exposed to Mandarin Chinese. The results suggested that Mandarin-exposed non-autistic children exhibited a shape bias, while autistic children did not. Further, a positive correlation was found between the shape representation accuracy and shape bias performance in the autistic group. These findings provide cross-linguistic evidence for the shape bias as a word learning constraint in non-autistic toddlers but challenges in utilizing this constraint in word learning by young autistic children. Importantly, these results shed new light on the critical role of abstract representations of object shape in facilitating shape bias knowledge in autistic children.

形状偏误是儿童语言发展过程中一种重要的单词学习策略。虽然有些研究观察到自闭症儿童不存在形状偏误或形状偏误不典型,但关于其根本原因却没有一致的证据。此外,以往的研究都是针对印欧语言学习者进行的,但形状偏误是否是一种跨语言的普遍词汇学习限制尚不清楚。本研究旨在调查 40 名 1-3 岁非自闭症儿童和 41 名 2-6 岁自闭症儿童在接触普通话后出现的形状偏差及其与形状表征能力的关联。结果表明,接触普通话的非自闭症儿童表现出形状偏差,而自闭症儿童则没有。此外,还发现自闭症儿童组的形状表征准确性与形状偏差表现之间存在正相关。这些发现提供了跨语言证据,证明形状偏差是非自闭症幼儿学习单词的一种制约因素,但自闭症幼儿在利用这种制约因素学习单词时却面临挑战。重要的是,这些结果揭示了物体形状的抽象表征在促进自闭症儿童形状偏差知识方面的关键作用。
{"title":"The shape bias in Mandarin-exposed young autistic children: The role of abstract shape representation","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101491","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101491","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The shape bias is an important word learning strategy in children’s language development. Although some studies have observed an absent or atypical shape bias in autistic children, there is no converging evidence regarding its underlying bases. Moreover, previous research has been exclusively conducted in learners of Indo-European languages, yet it is unclear whether the shape bias is a universal word learning constraint across languages. This study aims to investigate the shape bias and its association with shape representation ability in 40 1–3-year-old non-autistic children, and 41 2–6-year-old autistic children, exposed to Mandarin Chinese. The results suggested that Mandarin-exposed non-autistic children exhibited a shape bias, while autistic children did not. Further, a positive correlation was found between the shape representation accuracy and shape bias performance in the autistic group. These findings provide cross-linguistic evidence for the shape bias as a word learning constraint in non-autistic toddlers but challenges in utilizing this constraint in word learning by young autistic children. Importantly, these results shed new light on the critical role of abstract representations of object shape in facilitating shape bias knowledge in autistic children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The “How many?” task inadequately assesses the understanding of the cardinality principle 有多少?"任务没有充分评估对万有引力原则的理解
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101500

Once children have acquired the cardinality principle, they understand that the last number-word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set. This principle is often assessed using the “How many?” task, which consists in asking “How many?” objects there are in a set after children have counted them. However, we show in this study that out of 188 kindergarteners (mean age: 4 ½ years), 42 (22,3 %) succeeded in repeating the last count word in the “How many?” task but failed to correctly apply the one-to-one correspondence principle during counting. Even when only the easiest countable sets were considered (i.e., linear and homogeneous collections or sets with a very limited number of objects), still more than 10 % of children from our sample repeated the last count word but failed to apply the one-to-one correspondence principle. Such developmental profile, in which children understand that the last word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set but fail to grasp that each individual object must be associated with a single number word to determine this total, is not psychologically plausible. We conclude that the “How many?” task leads to an inaccurate assessment of the cardinality principle, in both its basic and more meaningful conception, in a non-negligible number of young children.

一旦孩子们掌握了 "万有引力 "原则,他们就会明白,数数时使用的最后一个数 字代表了一组物体的总数。对这一原则的评估通常采用 "有多少?"任务,即在儿童数完一组物体后,询问 "有多少?"。然而,我们在本研究中发现,在 188 名幼儿园儿童(平均年龄:4 岁半)中,有 42 人(22.3%)在 "有多少?"任务中成功地重复了最后一个数词,但在数数时却未能正确应用一一对应原则。即使只考虑最简单的可数集(即线性和同质集合或物体数量非常有限的集合),我们的样本中仍有超过 10% 的儿童重复了最后一个数词,但未能应用一一对应原则。在这种发展过程中,幼儿懂得计数时使用的最后一个数词代表集合中物体的总数,但却不懂得每个物体都必须与一个数词相关联才能确定总数,这在心理学上是说不通的。我们的结论是,"有多少?"任务会导致相当数量的幼儿对万有引力原理的基本概念和更有意义的概念作出不准确的评估。
{"title":"The “How many?” task inadequately assesses the understanding of the cardinality principle","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101500","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101500","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Once children have acquired the cardinality principle, they understand that the last number-word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set. This principle is often assessed using the <em>“How many?”</em> task, which consists in asking “How many?” objects there are in a set after children have counted them. However, we show in this study that out of 188 kindergarteners (mean age: 4 ½ years), 42 (22,3 %) succeeded in repeating the last count word in the <em>“How many?”</em> task but failed to correctly apply the one-to-one correspondence principle during counting. Even when only the easiest countable sets were considered (i.e., linear and homogeneous collections or sets with a very limited number of objects), still more than 10 % of children from our sample repeated the last count word but failed to apply the one-to-one correspondence principle. Such developmental profile, in which children understand that the last word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set but fail to grasp that each individual object must be associated with a single number word to determine this total, is not psychologically plausible. We conclude that the <em>“How many?”</em> task leads to an inaccurate assessment of the cardinality principle, in both its basic and more meaningful conception, in a non-negligible number of young children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000856/pdfft?md5=07734e18e2d6021099b4a13ce4141d72&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000856-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children’s perceptions of intergroup similarity and dissimilarity and their association with attitudes towards a conflict out-group 儿童对群体间相似性和不相似性的看法及其与对冲突外群体态度的关联
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101499

Children’s perceptions of similarity and dissimilarity between in- and out-groups and their associations with intergroup attitudes were examined. Using mixed qualitative and quantitative methods, 5- and 8-year-old Jewish-Israeli children (N = 100, 48 % girls) were investigated. They were asked about Arabs (which are considered a conflict out-group). Specifically, they were asked whether a Jewish girl/boy and an Arab girl/boy are similar or dissimilar and in what way. They were also asked about their attitudes towards Arabs. In general, children reported more intergroup dissimilarities than similarities between Jews and Arabs. Children’s perceptions revealed that they considered three types of properties as fundamental for intergroup (dis)similarity: appearance, psychological characteristics, and social identity. The results indicate that children form intergroup dissimilarity perceptions at an early age, but only at about 8 years of age do these perceptions become associated with negative attitudes towards the out-group. These findings highlight the importance of promoting early awareness of similarities between groups to mitigate the development of negative attitudes as children grow older.

研究考察了儿童对本群体和外群体之间相似性和不相似性的看法及其与群体间态度的关联。研究采用定性和定量相结合的方法,对 5 岁和 8 岁的以色列犹太儿童(人数 = 100,48% 为女童)进行了调查。他们被问及有关阿拉伯人(被视为冲突外群体)的问题。具体地说,他们被问及犹太女孩/男孩和阿拉伯女孩/男孩是相似还是不相似,在哪些方面相似。他们还被问及对阿拉伯人的态度。总的来说,孩子们报告的群体间差异多于犹太人和阿拉伯人之间的相似性。儿童的看法表明,他们认为三类属性是群体间(不)相似性的基本要素:外表、心理特征和社会身份。研究结果表明,儿童在很小的时候就会形成群体间不相似的观念,但只有到了 8 岁左右,这些观念才会与对外群体的消极态度联系起来。这些研究结果凸显了促进儿童尽早认识群体间相似性的重要性,以减轻儿童在长大后形成的消极态度。
{"title":"Children’s perceptions of intergroup similarity and dissimilarity and their association with attitudes towards a conflict out-group","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101499","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101499","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children’s perceptions of similarity and dissimilarity between in- and out-groups and their associations with intergroup attitudes were examined. Using mixed qualitative and quantitative methods, 5- and 8-year-old Jewish-Israeli children (<em>N</em> = 100, 48 % girls) were investigated. They were asked about Arabs (which are considered a conflict out-group). Specifically, they were asked whether a Jewish girl/boy and an Arab girl/boy are similar or dissimilar and in what way. They were also asked about their attitudes towards Arabs. In general, children reported more intergroup dissimilarities than similarities between Jews and Arabs. Children’s perceptions revealed that they considered three types of properties as fundamental for intergroup (dis)similarity: appearance, psychological characteristics, and social identity. The results indicate that children form intergroup dissimilarity perceptions at an early age, but only at about 8 years of age do these perceptions become associated with negative attitudes towards the out-group. These findings highlight the importance of promoting early awareness of similarities between groups to mitigate the development of negative attitudes as children grow older.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inquiry and argumentation skill development work in conjunction 探究与论证技能培养相结合
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101464
Si Xiao, Deanna Kuhn

The argument and inquiry families of scientific thinking skills overlap. Yet they rarely are investigated together, with researchers instead focusing on one or the other. Here we hypothesize that inclusion of an additional intervention focused on inquiry will enhance the outcome of an established intervention known to be successful in developing argument skills, compared to the argument intervention alone. An Argument only (A) and an Argument and Inquiry (A&I) group of young adolescents participated in one or the other of these intensive intervention groups for four hours daily over a two-week period. Both groups made progress in argument skills as expected, but the A&I group showed greater gains in use of evidence to support claims, stronger forms of counterargument, and integrative however arguments that connect opposing claims to one another. The A&I group showed a particular advantage in reconciling contrasting claims, an epistemological understanding that lies at the core of scientific thinking. These findings suggest the value of conceptualizing argument skill development in a broader framework that includes an investigative component as well as the skill of coordinating claims and evidence that is fundamental to argument.

科学思维能力中的论证和探究系列是相互重叠的。然而,研究人员很少将它们放在一起研究,而是只关注其中之一。在此,我们假设,与单纯的论证干预相比,加入额外的以探究为重点的干预会增强已知能成功培养论证技能的既定干预的效果。一组青少年只参加论证(A),另一组青少年参加论证和探究(A&I),他们在两周时间里每天参加其中一个或另一个强化干预小组,每次四小时。两组在论证技能方面都取得了预期的进步,但 A&I 组在使用证据支持论点、更有力的反驳形式以及将对立论点相互联系起来的综合论证方面的进步更大。A&I组在调和对立主张方面表现出特别的优势,这是一种认识论理解,是科学思维的核心。这些研究结果表明,在一个更广泛的框架内对论证技能的发展进行概念化是有价值的,这个框架既包括调查成分,也包括协调主张和证据的技能,这是论证的基础。
{"title":"Inquiry and argumentation skill development work in conjunction","authors":"Si Xiao,&nbsp;Deanna Kuhn","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101464","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101464","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The argument and inquiry families of scientific thinking skills overlap. Yet they rarely are investigated together, with researchers instead focusing on one or the other. Here we hypothesize that inclusion of an additional intervention focused on inquiry will enhance the outcome of an established intervention known to be successful in developing argument skills, compared to the argument intervention alone. An Argument only (A) and an Argument and Inquiry (A&amp;I) group of young adolescents participated in one or the other of these intensive intervention groups for four hours daily over a two-week period. Both groups made progress in argument skills as expected, but the A&amp;I group showed greater gains in use of evidence to support claims, stronger forms of counterargument, and integrative <em>however</em> arguments that connect opposing claims to one another. The A&amp;I group showed a particular advantage in reconciling contrasting claims, an epistemological understanding that lies at the core of scientific thinking. These findings suggest the value of conceptualizing argument skill development in a broader framework that includes an investigative component as well as the skill of coordinating claims and evidence that is fundamental to argument.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141485097","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does reading fluency mediate the relationship between cognitive-linguistic skills and reading comprehension? A study in European Portuguese 阅读流畅性是否能调节认知语言技能与阅读理解之间的关系?欧洲葡萄牙语研究
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101490

Research has consistently revealed the existence of an interconnection between reading comprehension, word reading, reading fluency, vocabulary and rapid naming. The main goal of this study was to explore the possible mediating role of reading fluency in the relationship between reading comprehension and the remaining skills, and to test whether the magnitude of these relationships was similar across different years of schooling. For this purpose, a longitudinal study with two assessment time points was carried out in a sample of 2nd and 3rd graders who were learning to read in European Portuguese, an intermediate-depth orthography. The results evidenced that reading fluency not only directly influences reading comprehension but also plays a mediating role in the relationship between reading comprehension and skills such as word reading and rapid naming. On other hand, the results indicate a unique effect of vocabulary on reading comprehension. Taken together, these results have important implications for educational practice, suggesting that explicit intervention in reading should include both the teaching and training in reading fluency and the construction of a richer lexical repertoire.

研究表明,阅读理解、单词阅读、阅读流利性、词汇量和快速命名之间存在着相互联系。本研究的主要目的是探讨阅读流畅性在阅读理解与其余技能之间的关系中可能起到的中介作用,并检验这些关系的程度在不同的学年是否相似。为此,研究人员对二年级和三年级的学生进行了一项纵向研究,对他们进行了两个时间点的评估,这些学生正在学习用欧洲葡萄牙语进行阅读,这是一种中等深度的正字法。结果表明,阅读流畅性不仅直接影响阅读理解能力,而且在阅读理解能力与单词阅读和快速命名等技能之间的关系中起着中介作用。另一方面,研究结果表明词汇对阅读理解有独特的影响。综上所述,这些结果对教育实践具有重要的启示意义,表明对阅读的明确干预应包括阅读流利性的教学和训练,以及构建更丰富的词汇库。
{"title":"Does reading fluency mediate the relationship between cognitive-linguistic skills and reading comprehension? A study in European Portuguese","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101490","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101490","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research has consistently revealed the existence of an interconnection between reading comprehension, word reading, reading fluency, vocabulary and rapid naming. The main goal of this study was to explore the possible mediating role of reading fluency in the relationship between reading comprehension and the remaining skills, and to test whether the magnitude of these relationships was similar across different years of schooling. For this purpose, a longitudinal study with two assessment time points was carried out in a sample of 2nd and 3rd graders who were learning to read in European Portuguese, an intermediate-depth orthography. The results evidenced that reading fluency not only directly influences reading comprehension but also plays a mediating role in the relationship between reading comprehension and skills such as word reading and rapid naming. On other hand, the results indicate a unique effect of vocabulary on reading comprehension. Taken together, these results have important implications for educational practice, suggesting that explicit intervention in reading should include both the teaching and training in reading fluency and the construction of a richer lexical repertoire.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000753/pdfft?md5=41b713bb3cee8cafd1b5d57a04c56b5c&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000753-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142084357","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dating early memories: When did events really happen? 约会早期记忆:事件到底发生在什么时候?
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489

Can children accurately date their early memories? This question has important real-life consequences such as when jurors evaluate the credibility of child eyewitness testimony in court. Answering this question is difficult given that adults present at remembered events may be inaccurate themselves in retroactively dating the memories recalled by their children, and often cannot provide reliable validation. In this study, prior to child interviews the parents of 6- to 13-year-olds provided eight memories of events with known dates, two each from when children were age 2, 3, 4, and 5 years. A total of 104 6- to 13-year-olds participated (47 % female, 70 % White and 26 % Asian or multi-ethnic), recruited from Canada (36 %) and USA (64 %). Children typically made systematic dating errors. Memories of events that had occurred when children had been age 2 were misdated by 1½ years on average, and as children’s age at the time of remembered events increased, misdating errors decreased. Errors usually involved children thinking they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually were – a phenomenon termed ‘forward telescoping’ (versus ‘backward telescoping, when individuals think that they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually had been). For example, many of the events from when children were age 2 were recalled by the children, but they misdated them to older ages. Although ‘age of memory’ (age of the child at the time of the remembered event) was significantly related to errors in dating, with more errors for memories from younger periods of their lives, ‘age of child’ at the time they did the memory task did not differ depending upon how old the children were. Findings have theoretical and forensic implications.

儿童能否准确地确定其早期记忆的日期?这个问题在现实生活中具有重要影响,例如陪审员在法庭上评估儿童目击证人证词的可信度时。要回答这个问题是很困难的,因为当时在场的成年人在追溯孩子的记忆时可能并不准确,而且往往无法提供可靠的验证。在本研究中,6 到 13 岁儿童的父母在对儿童进行访谈之前,提供了 8 个已知日期的事件记忆,其中 2 个分别来自儿童 2 岁、3 岁、4 岁和 5 岁时。共有 104 名 6 至 13 岁的儿童(47% 为女性,70% 为白人,26% 为亚裔或多种族)参加了此次访谈,他们分别来自加拿大(36%)和美国(64%)。儿童通常会犯系统性的约会错误。儿童对 2 岁时发生的事件的记忆平均错记了 1 年半,随着儿童记忆事件发生时年龄的增加,错记日期的错误也随之减少。错误通常涉及儿童认为他们在记忆事件发生时的年龄比实际年龄大--这种现象被称为 "向前伸缩"(相对于 "向后伸缩",即个人认为他们在记忆事件发生时的年龄比实际年龄大)。例如,孩子们回忆起了许多他们 2 岁时发生的事情,但他们却错误地把这些事情的时间推算到了更大的年龄。虽然 "记忆年龄"(记忆事件发生时儿童的年龄)与年代错误有很大关系,儿童对较年轻时期的记忆错误较多,但儿童做记忆任务时的 "儿童年龄 "并不因儿童的年龄而异。研究结果具有理论和法医学意义。
{"title":"Dating early memories: When did events really happen?","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Can children accurately date their early memories? This question has important real-life consequences such as when jurors evaluate the credibility of child eyewitness testimony in court. Answering this question is difficult given that adults present at remembered events may be inaccurate themselves in retroactively dating the memories recalled by their children, and often cannot provide reliable validation. In this study, prior to child interviews the parents of 6- to 13-year-olds provided eight memories of events with known dates, two each from when children were age 2, 3, 4, and 5 years. A total of 104 6- to 13-year-olds participated (47 % female, 70 % White and 26 % Asian or multi-ethnic), recruited from Canada (36 %) and USA (64 %). Children typically made systematic dating errors. Memories of events that had occurred when children had been age 2 were misdated by 1½ years on average, and as children’s age at the time of remembered events increased, misdating errors decreased. Errors usually involved children thinking they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually were – a phenomenon termed ‘forward telescoping’ (versus ‘backward telescoping, when individuals think that they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually had been). For example, many of the events from when children were age 2 were recalled by the children, but they misdated them to older ages. Although ‘age of memory’ (age of the child at the time of the remembered event) was significantly related to errors in dating, with more errors for memories from younger periods of their lives, ‘age of child’ at the time they did the memory task did not differ depending upon how old the children were. Findings have theoretical and forensic implications.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000741/pdfft?md5=937432cd47a9c31986716e10d60efb5f&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000741-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142039608","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Failure supports 3- to 6-year-old children’s mechanistic exploration 失败支持 3-6 岁儿童进行机械探索
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101485

This study investigated whether contexts of failure improve preschool children’s mechanistic reasoning. We showed 3- to 6-year-old children (N = 55, M = 5;2) how to make an unfamiliar toy work to play a goal-directed game. Between conditions we manipulated children’s success in making the toy work by surreptitiously turning a hidden causal switch ON (Success) or OFF (Failure) before they interacted with the toy. We then measured children’s exploration of the toy, explanations for how the toy worked, and generalizations about how a new functioning toy would work. Children in the Failure condition were more likely to discover the hidden causal mechanism and talk about it in their explanations about the toy. Younger children spent more time exploring the toy than older children but were not more likely to discover the hidden causal mechanism. The findings are discussed as they relate to the emergence of spontaneous mechanistic exploration over development, how this then supports mechanistic reasoning, and the role of failure in children’s early causal reasoning.

本研究探讨了失败情境是否能提高学龄前儿童的机械推理能力。我们向 3 到 6 岁的儿童(人数=55,=5;2)演示了如何让一个陌生的玩具工作,以玩一个目标导向的游戏。在不同的条件下,我们通过在儿童与玩具互动之前偷偷打开一个隐藏的因果开关(成功)或关闭(失败),来操纵儿童使玩具工作的成功率。然后,我们测量了儿童对玩具的探索、对玩具工作原理的解释以及对新功能玩具工作原理的概括。在 "失败 "条件下,儿童更有可能发现隐藏的因果机制,并在解释玩具时谈论它。年龄较小的儿童比年龄较大的儿童花更多时间探索玩具,但发现隐藏因果机制的可能性并不大。本研究讨论了这些发现,因为它们与儿童在成长过程中出现的自发机械探索、这种探索如何支持机械推理以及失败在儿童早期因果推理中的作用有关。
{"title":"Failure supports 3- to 6-year-old children’s mechanistic exploration","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101485","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101485","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigated whether contexts of failure improve preschool children’s mechanistic reasoning. We showed 3- to 6-year-old children (N = 55, <em>M</em> = 5;2) how to make an unfamiliar toy work to play a goal-directed game. Between conditions we manipulated children’s success in making the toy work by surreptitiously turning a hidden causal switch ON (Success) or OFF (Failure) before they interacted with the toy. We then measured children’s exploration of the toy, explanations for how the toy worked, and generalizations about how a new functioning toy would work. Children in the Failure condition were more likely to discover the hidden causal mechanism and talk about it in their explanations about the toy. Younger children spent more time exploring the toy than older children but were not more likely to discover the hidden causal mechanism. The findings are discussed as they relate to the emergence of spontaneous mechanistic exploration over development, how this then supports mechanistic reasoning, and the role of failure in children’s early causal reasoning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141930444","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity are associated in fictive storytelling and autobiographical memories in typically developing children and adolescents 典型发育期儿童和青少年虚构故事和自传体记忆中的叙事连贯性和思维复杂性相关联
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101484

Different lines of evidence indicate that the ability to communicate narratives coherently is related to children's social-emotional development. However, it is unknown whether narrative coherence is genre-specific or generalizes across autobiographical memories and fictive stories, and if autobiographical and fictive narratives show different or similar associations with mentalizing language, cognitive functions, social and daily functioning. Addressing these questions may provide important clues about the development of narrative communication skills in children and adolescents. We assessed 86 typically developing children and adolescents aged 7–14, examining narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity in six autobiographical memories and five fictional stories, alongside intellectual functioning and parent and teacher reports on social and adaptive functioning. Results showed that the measures on narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity, respectively, were associated across autobiographical memories and fictional stories. Moreover, narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity was related to each other on both fictive and autobiographical stories. Higher narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity on autobiographical memories were specifically related to better social-emotional reciprocity reported by teachers, who likely have more opportunities than parents to observe the child’s daily interaction with peers. Our findings suggest that narrative coherence and mentalizing language in school-aged children generalize across genres. Being able to communicate personal narratives coherently with use of mentalizing language appears to be important for the social-emotional interplay of children and adolescents.

各种证据表明,连贯的叙事交流能力与儿童的社会情感发展有关。然而,叙事的连贯性是针对特定体裁的,还是普遍适用于自传体记忆和虚构故事的,自传体叙事和虚构叙事与心智化语言、认知功能、社交和日常功能是否表现出不同或相似的关联,这些问题都不得而知。解决这些问题可为儿童和青少年叙事交流技能的发展提供重要线索。我们对 86 名 7-14 岁发育正常的儿童和青少年进行了评估,研究了六段自传记忆和五个虚构故事中的叙事连贯性和心智化复杂性,同时还研究了智力功能以及家长和教师对社会和适应功能的报告。结果显示,叙事连贯性和心智化复杂性的测量结果分别与自传体记忆和虚构故事相关。此外,在虚构故事和自传故事中,叙事连贯性和心智化复杂性也相互关联。自传体记忆中较高的叙事连贯性和心智化复杂性与教师报告的较好的社会情感互惠特别相关,因为教师可能比家长有更多机会观察孩子与同伴的日常互动。我们的研究结果表明,学龄儿童的叙事连贯性和心智化语言具有跨体裁的普遍性。能够使用心理化语言连贯地交流个人叙事似乎对儿童和青少年的社会情感互动非常重要。
{"title":"Narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity are associated in fictive storytelling and autobiographical memories in typically developing children and adolescents","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101484","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101484","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Different lines of evidence indicate that the ability to communicate narratives coherently is related to children's social-emotional development. However, it is unknown whether narrative coherence is genre-specific or generalizes across autobiographical memories and fictive stories, and if autobiographical and fictive narratives show different or similar associations with mentalizing language, cognitive functions, social and daily functioning. Addressing these questions may provide important clues about the development of narrative communication skills in children and adolescents. We assessed 86 typically developing children and adolescents aged 7–14, examining narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity in six autobiographical memories and five fictional stories, alongside intellectual functioning and parent and teacher reports on social and adaptive functioning. Results showed that the measures on narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity, respectively, were associated across autobiographical memories and fictional stories. Moreover, narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity was related to each other on both fictive and autobiographical stories. Higher narrative coherence and mentalizing complexity on autobiographical memories were specifically related to better social-emotional reciprocity reported by teachers, who likely have more opportunities than parents to observe the child’s daily interaction with peers. Our findings suggest that narrative coherence and mentalizing language in school-aged children generalize across genres. Being able to communicate personal narratives coherently with use of mentalizing language appears to be important for the social-emotional interplay of children and adolescents.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000698/pdfft?md5=f9e701cb6f2db0b882ced77129146e05&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000698-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141770903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A longitudinal study of Turkish-Dutch children’s language mixing in single-language settings: Language status, language proficiency, cognitive control and developmental language disorder 土耳其-荷兰儿童在单一语言环境中的语言混合纵向研究:语言地位、语言能力、认知控制和发育性语言障碍
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101481
Elma Blom , Gülşah Yazıcı , Tessel Boerma , Merel van Witteloostuijn

The aim of this study was to investigate the role of language status, language proficiency, cognitive control and Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children’s language mixing in single-language settings. We investigated these factors over time following 31 children (20 with typical development, 11 with DLD), from the age of 5 or 6 years until they were 7 or 8 years old. Children more often mix the majority-societal language (Dutch) into the minority-heritage language (Turkish) than the other way around. Higher proficiency in Dutch, lower proficiency in Turkish, and having DLD are linked to more mixing in the Turkish setting. Effects of cognitive control on children’s language mixing are limited. Linguistic factors at a child-external and child-internal level impact on children’s mixing in single-language settings, and are more important than domain-general cognitive control. Increasing language proficiency in Turkish could explain why children mix less as they grow older.

本研究的目的是调查语言地位、语言能力、认知控制和发育性语言障碍(DLD)对土耳其-荷兰双语儿童在单一语言环境中的语言混合所起的作用。我们对 31 名儿童(20 名发育典型儿童和 11 名发育语言障碍儿童)从 5 或 6 岁到 7 或 8 岁期间的语言混合情况进行了调查。儿童更经常地将多数社会语言(荷兰语)混合到少数遗产语言(土耳其语)中,而不是相反。较高的荷兰语熟练程度、较低的土耳其语熟练程度以及患有 DLD 都与土耳其语环境中更多的混用有关。认知控制对儿童语言混合的影响有限。儿童外部和儿童内部的语言因素会影响儿童在单一语言环境中的语言混合,而且比一般领域认知控制更为重要。土耳其语语言能力的提高可以解释为什么儿童随着年龄的增长混合语言的情况会减少。
{"title":"A longitudinal study of Turkish-Dutch children’s language mixing in single-language settings: Language status, language proficiency, cognitive control and developmental language disorder","authors":"Elma Blom ,&nbsp;Gülşah Yazıcı ,&nbsp;Tessel Boerma ,&nbsp;Merel van Witteloostuijn","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101481","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101481","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The aim of this study was to investigate the role of language status, language proficiency, cognitive control and Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children’s language mixing in single-language settings. We investigated these factors over time following 31 children (20 with typical development, 11 with DLD), from the age of 5 or 6 years until they were 7 or 8 years old. Children more often mix the majority-societal language (Dutch) into the minority-heritage language (Turkish) than the other way around. Higher proficiency in Dutch, lower proficiency in Turkish, and having DLD are linked to more mixing in the Turkish setting. Effects of cognitive control on children’s language mixing are limited. Linguistic factors at a child-external and child-internal level impact on children’s mixing in single-language settings, and are more important than domain-general cognitive control. Increasing language proficiency in Turkish could explain why children mix less as they grow older.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000662/pdfft?md5=c528390c37142e71cdad767e3d94d6d7&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000662-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141540744","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1